Revision as of 11:51, 3 December 2023 editCertes (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users243,571 editsm Disambiguated: Meta← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 17:36, 15 December 2024 edit undoFeérica (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users744 edits Added main articleTags: Visual edit Mobile edit Mobile web edit Advanced mobile edit | ||
(48 intermediate revisions by 34 users not shown) | |||
Line 4: | Line 4: | ||
{{distinguish|Stereotypy}} | {{distinguish|Stereotypy}} | ||
}} | }} | ||
{{Use dmy dates|date= |
{{Use dmy dates|date=January 2024}} | ||
] | ] | ||
] of a |
], with ] and black ]), particularly popular in early-20th-century ]s and ]s and popularized by ]]] | ||
⚫ | ]s and coffee, an example of perceived stereotypical behavior<ref>{{cite web |last=Critchfield |first=Austi |title=This is Why Doughnuts Are Associated With Cops |url= https://spoonuniversity.com/lifestyle/this-is-why-doughnuts-are-associated-with-cops |publisher=Spoon University |date=15 November 2017 |access-date=2 November 2020 |quote=The funny thing is, the whole cop and doughnuts thing is completely out of date – today, an officer could just as easily swing through a McDonald's drive through as he could a Krispy Kreme. Yet, the stereotype endures, even though police aren't seen at doughnut shops in nearly the numbers they used to have been. In a way, it's become a stereotype of itself, which is pretty ]. |url-status=live |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20171119061259/https://spoonuniversity.com/lifestyle/this-is-why-doughnuts-are-associated-with-cops |archive-date=19 November 2017}}</ref> in North America]] | ||
{{Discrimination sidebar|Related}} | {{Discrimination sidebar|Related}} | ||
⚫ | ]. |url-status=live |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20171119061259/https://spoonuniversity.com/lifestyle/this-is-why-doughnuts-are-associated-with-cops |archive-date=19 November 2017}}</ref> in |
||
In ], a '''stereotype''' is a generalized belief about a particular category of people.<ref>{{Cite book |title=Dictionary of Psychology |last=Cardwell |first=Mike |location=Chicago |publisher=Fitzroy Dearborn |date=1999 |isbn=9781579580643 |url= https://archive.org/details/dictionaryofpsyc0000card}}</ref> It is an expectation that people might have about every person of a particular group. The type of expectation can vary; it can be, for example, an expectation about the group's personality, preferences, appearance or ability. Stereotypes are often overgeneralized, inaccurate, and resistant to new information.<ref>{{Cite book | |
In ], a '''stereotype''' is a generalized belief about a particular category of people.<ref>{{Cite book |title=Dictionary of Psychology |last=Cardwell |first=Mike |location=Chicago |publisher=Fitzroy Dearborn |date=1999 |isbn=9781579580643 |url= https://archive.org/details/dictionaryofpsyc0000card}}</ref> It is an expectation that people might have about every person of a particular group. The type of expectation can vary; it can be, for example, an expectation about the group's personality, preferences, appearance or ability. Stereotypes are often ], inaccurate, and ].<ref>{{Cite book |last=Meyer |first=David G. |title=Intuition: Its Powers and Perils |publisher=Yale University Press |year=2008 |isbn=9780300130270 |pages=114–115}}</ref> A stereotype does not necessarily need to be a negative assumption. They may be positive, neutral, or negative. | ||
==Explicit stereotypes== | ==Explicit stereotypes== | ||
An explicit stereotype refers to stereotypes that one is aware that one holds, and is aware that one is using to judge people. If person ''A ''is making judgments about a {{em|particular}} person ''B'' from |
An explicit stereotype refers to stereotypes that one is aware that one holds, and is aware that one is using to judge people. If person ''A ''is making judgments about a {{em|particular}} person ''B'' from group ''G'', and person ''A'' has an explicit stereotype for group ''G'', their decision bias can be partially mitigated using conscious control; however, attempts to offset bias due to conscious awareness of a stereotype often fail at being truly impartial, due to either underestimating or overestimating the amount of bias being created by the stereotype. | ||
==Implicit stereotypes== | ==Implicit stereotypes== | ||
{{Main article|Implicit stereotype}} | |||
Implicit stereotypes are those that lay on individuals' subconsciousness, that they have no control or awareness of.<ref>{{Cite web |url= https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/faqs.html#faq1 |title=Frequently Asked Questions |website=Implicit.Harvard.edu |access-date=14 April 2018}}</ref> "Implicit stereotypes are built based on two concepts, associative networks in semantic (knowledge) memory and automatic activation".<ref name="Hinton">{{cite journal |last=Hinton |first=Perry |title=Implicit stereotypes and the predictive brain: Cognition and culture in "biased" person perception |journal=Palgrave Communications |date=1 September 2017 |volume=3 |issue=1 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.1057/palcomms.2017.86 |s2cid=54036730 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Implicit stereotypes are automatic and involuntary associations that people make between a social group and a domain or attribute. For example, one can have beliefs that women and men are equally capable of becoming successful electricians but at the same time many can associate electricians more with men than women.<ref name="Hinton" /> | |||
In ], a stereotype is any thought widely adopted about specific types of individuals or certain ways of behaving intended to represent the entire group of those individuals or behaviors as a whole.<ref name="McGarty Yzerbyt et al. (2002)">{{cite book |title=Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups |last1=McGarty |first1=Craig |last2=Yzerbyt |first2=Vincent Y. |last3=Spears |first3=Russel |publisher=] |date=2002 |isbn=9780521800471 |pages=1–15 |chapter=Social, cultural and cognitive factors in stereotype formation |chapter-url= http://catdir.loc.gov/catdir/samples/cam033/2002073438.pdf}}</ref> These thoughts or beliefs may or may not accurately reflect |
In ], a stereotype is any thought widely adopted about specific types of individuals or certain ways of behaving intended to represent the entire group of those individuals or behaviors as a whole.<ref name="McGarty Yzerbyt et al. (2002)">{{cite book |title=Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups |last1=McGarty |first1=Craig |last2=Yzerbyt |first2=Vincent Y. |last3=Spears |first3=Russel |publisher=] |date=2002 |isbn=9780521800471 |pages=1–15 |chapter=Social, cultural and cognitive factors in stereotype formation |chapter-url= http://catdir.loc.gov/catdir/samples/cam033/2002073438.pdf}}</ref> These thoughts or beliefs may or may not accurately reflect reality.<ref name="Judd et al. (1993)">{{cite journal |last1=Judd |first1=Charles M. |last2=Park |first2=Bernadette |date=1993 |title=Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes |journal=] |volume=100 |issue=1 |pages=109–128 |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.100.1.109 |pmid=8426877}}</ref><ref name="Cox et al. (2012)">{{cite journal |last1=Cox |first1=William T. L. |last2=Abramson |first2=Lyn Y. |last3=Devine |first3=Patricia G. |last4=Hollon |first4=Steven D. |title=Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Depression: The Integrated Perspective |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |date=September 2012 |volume=7 |issue=5 |pages=427–449 |doi=10.1177/1745691612455204 |pmid=26168502 |s2cid=1512121}}</ref> Within psychology and across other disciplines, different conceptualizations and theories of stereotyping exist, at times sharing commonalities, as well as containing contradictory elements. Even in the social sciences and some sub-disciplines of psychology, stereotypes are occasionally reproduced and can be identified in certain theories, for example, in assumptions about other cultures.<ref name="Chakkarath, P. (2010)">{{cite journal |last1=Chakkarath |first1=Pradeep |title=Stereotypes in social psychology: The 'West-East' differentiation as a reflection of Western traditions of thought |journal=Psychological Studies |date=March 2010 |volume=55 |issue=1 |pages=18–25 |doi=10.1007/s12646-010-0002-9 |s2cid=144061506 |url= https://www.ssoar.info/ssoar/handle/document/58094}}</ref> | ||
==Etymology== | ==Etymology== | ||
The term ''stereotype'' comes from the |
The term ''stereotype'' comes from the French adjective {{lang|fr|stéréotype}} and derives from the ] words {{lang|grc|στερεός}} ({{lang|grc|]}}), 'firm, solid'<ref>{{cite web |url= https://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0057%3Aentry%3Dstereo%2Fs |title=στερεός |first1=Henry George |last1=Liddell |first2=Robert |last2=Scott |work=A Greek-English Lexicon |via=Perseus Digital Library}}</ref> and {{lang|grc|τύπος}} ({{lang|grc|]}}), 'impression',<ref>{{cite web |url= https://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0057%3Aentry%3Dtu%2Fpos |title=τύπος |first1=Henry George |last1=Liddell |first2=Robert |last2=Scott |work=A Greek-English Lexicon |via=Perseus Digital Library}}</ref> hence 'solid impression on one or more ]/]'. | ||
|first1=Henry George |last1=Liddell |first2=Robert |last2=Scott |work=A Greek-English Lexicon |via=Perseus Digital Library}}</ref> and {{lang|grc|τύπος}} ({{lang|grc-Latn|]}}), 'impression',<ref>{{cite web |url= https://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.04.0057%3Aentry%3Dtu%2Fpos |title=τύπος |first1=Henry George |last1=Liddell |first2=Robert |last2=Scott |work=A Greek-English Lexicon |via=Perseus Digital Library}}</ref> hence 'solid impression on one or more ]/]'. | |||
The term was first used in the ] in 1798 by ], to describe a printing plate that duplicated any ]. The duplicate printing plate, or the ], is used for printing instead of the original. | The term was first used in the ] in 1798 by ], to describe a printing plate that duplicated any ]. The duplicate printing plate, or the ], is used for printing instead of the original. | ||
Outside of printing, the first reference to ''stereotype'' in English was in 1850, as a noun that meant 'image perpetuated without change'.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.etymonline.com/index.php?term=stereotype |title=stereotype – Origin and meaning |work=Online Etymology Dictionary |access-date=22 October 2017 |first=Douglas |last=Harper}}</ref> However, it was not until 1922 that ''stereotype'' was first used in the modern psychological sense by American journalist ] in his work '']''.<ref>{{cite book |last=Kleg |
Outside of printing, the first reference to ''stereotype'' in English was in 1850, as a noun that meant 'image perpetuated without change'.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.etymonline.com/index.php?term=stereotype |title=stereotype – Origin and meaning |work=Online Etymology Dictionary |access-date=22 October 2017 |first=Douglas |last=Harper}}</ref> However, it was not until 1922 that ''stereotype'' was first used in the modern psychological sense by American journalist ] in his work '']''.<ref>{{cite book |last=Kleg |first=Milton |title=Hate, Prejudice and Racism |url= https://archive.org/details/hateprejudicerac0000kleg |url-access=registration |date=1993 |publisher=State University of New York Press |location=Albany |pages=–137 |isbn=9780585054919}}</ref> | ||
==Relationship with other types of intergroup attitudes== | ==Relationship with other types of intergroup attitudes== | ||
Stereotypes, ], |
Stereotypes, ], racism, and discrimination<ref>{{Cite news |url= https://money.cnn.com/2015/11/25/news/economy/racial-discrimination-work/index.html |title=Working while brown: What discrimination looks like now |last=Vega |first=Tanzina |author-link=Tanzina Vega |work=CNNMoney |access-date=26 March 2018}}</ref> are understood as related but different concepts.<ref name="Fiske1998">{{cite book |last1=Fiske |first1=Susan T. |editor1-first=Daniel T. |editor1-last=Gilbert |editor2-first=Susan T. |editor2-last=Fiske |editor3-first=Gardner |editor3-last=Lindzey |title=The Handbook of Social Psychology |edition=4th |volume=2 |date=1998 |publisher=] |location=Boston, Massachusetts |isbn=9780195213768 |page=357 |chapter=Stereotyping, ⁰Prejudice, and Discrimination |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=w27pSuHLnLYC&pg=PA357}}</ref><ref name="Denmark">{{cite book |last1=Denmark |first1=Florence L. |editor1-first=Irving B. |editor1-last=Weiner |editor2-first=W. Edward |editor2-last=Craigheaid |title=The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology |edition=4th |volume=3 |date=2010 |publisher=] |location=Hoboken, New Jersey |isbn=9780470479216 |page=1277 |chapter=Prejudice and Discrimination |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=hhGdag3Wf-YC&pg=PA1276}}</ref><ref name="Katz & Braly (1935)" /><ref name="Oakes et al. (1994)" /> Stereotypes are regarded as the most ] component and often occurs without conscious awareness, whereas prejudice is the ] component of stereotyping and discrimination is one of the behavioral components of prejudicial reactions.<ref name="Fiske1998" /><ref name="Denmark" /><ref name="faculty.washington.edu">{{cite journal |last=Devine |first=Patricia G. |date=1989 |title=Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their Automatic and Controlled Components |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=56 |issue=1 |pages=5–18 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5 |s2cid=33975027 |url= https://www.researchgate.net/publication/229067921}}</ref> In this tripartite view of intergroup attitudes, stereotypes reflect expectations and beliefs about the members of groups perceived as different from one's own, prejudice represents the emotional response, and discrimination refers to actions.<ref name="Fiske1998" /><ref name="Denmark" /> | ||
Although related, the three concepts can exist independently of each other.<ref name="Denmark" /><ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> According to ] and Kenneth Braly, stereotyping leads to racial prejudice when people emotionally react to the name of a group, ascribe characteristics to members of that group, and then evaluate those characteristics.<ref name="Katz & Braly (1935)" /> | Although related, the three concepts can exist independently of each other.<ref name="Denmark" /><ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> According to ] and Kenneth Braly, stereotyping leads to racial prejudice when people emotionally react to the name of a group, ascribe characteristics to members of that group, and then evaluate those characteristics.<ref name="Katz & Braly (1935)" /> | ||
Line 36: | Line 36: | ||
* Justification of ill-founded prejudices or ignorance | * Justification of ill-founded prejudices or ignorance | ||
* Unwillingness to rethink one's attitudes and behavior | * Unwillingness to rethink one's attitudes and behavior | ||
* Preventing some people of stereotyped groups from entering or succeeding in activities or fields<ref name=Tilcsik>{{cite journal |last=Tilcsik |first=András |date=2011 |title=Pride and Prejudice: Employment Discrimination against Openly Gay Men in the United States |journal=] |volume=117 |issue=2 |pages=586–626 |doi=10.1086/661653 |pmid=22268247 |hdl=1807/34998 |s2cid=23542996 |hdl-access=free}}</ref> | * Preventing some people of stereotyped groups from entering or succeeding in activities or fields<ref name=Tilcsik> | ||
{{cite journal |last=Tilcsik |first=András |date=2011 |title=Pride and Prejudice: Employment Discrimination against Openly Gay Men in the United States |journal=] |volume=117 |issue=2 |pages=586–626 |doi=10.1086/661653 |pmid=22268247 |hdl=1807/34998 |s2cid=23542996 |hdl-access=free}}</ref> | |||
==Content== | ==Content== | ||
Line 43: | Line 44: | ||
Stereotype content refers to the attributes that people think characterize a group. Studies of stereotype content examine what people think of others, rather than the reasons and mechanisms involved in stereotyping.<ref name="operario&fiske">{{cite book |last1=Operario |first1=Don |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |editor1-first=Rupert |editor1-last=Brown |editor2-first=Samuel L. |editor2-last=Gaertner |title=Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes |date=2003 |publisher=] |location=Malden, Massachusetts |isbn=9781405106542 |pages=22–44 |chapter=Stereotypes: Content, Structures, Processes, and Context |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=Wfx55Z-Dw10C&pg=PA22}}</ref> | Stereotype content refers to the attributes that people think characterize a group. Studies of stereotype content examine what people think of others, rather than the reasons and mechanisms involved in stereotyping.<ref name="operario&fiske">{{cite book |last1=Operario |first1=Don |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |editor1-first=Rupert |editor1-last=Brown |editor2-first=Samuel L. |editor2-last=Gaertner |title=Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes |date=2003 |publisher=] |location=Malden, Massachusetts |isbn=9781405106542 |pages=22–44 |chapter=Stereotypes: Content, Structures, Processes, and Context |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=Wfx55Z-Dw10C&pg=PA22}}</ref> | ||
Early theories of stereotype content proposed by social psychologists such as ] assumed that stereotypes of ] reflected uniform ].<ref name="fiske et al. 2002">{{cite journal |last1=Fiske |first1=Susan T. |last2=Cuddy |first2=Amy J. C. |last3=Glick |first3=Peter |last4=Xu |first4=Jun |date=2002 |title=A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow From Perceived Status and Competition |journal=] |volume=82 |issue=6 |pages=878–902 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878 |url= http://www.cos.gatech.edu/facultyres/Diversity_Studies/Fiske_StereotypeContent.pdf |pmid=12051578|citeseerx=10.1.1.320.4001 |s2cid=17057403}}</ref><ref name=":1">{{cite book |last1=Cuddy |first1=Amy J. C. |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |editor1-first=Todd D. |editor1-last=Nelson |title=Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice against Older Persons |date=2002 |publisher=] |location=Cambridge, Massachusetts |isbn=9780262140775 |pages=7–8 |chapter=Doddering But Dear: Process, Content, and Function in Stereotyping of Older Persons |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=UvxEoFQ0LYwC&pg=PA7}}</ref> For instance, Katz and Braly argued in their classic 1933 study that ethnic stereotypes were uniformly negative.<ref name="operario&fiske" /> | Early theories of stereotype content proposed by social psychologists such as ] assumed that stereotypes of ] reflected uniform ].<ref name="fiske et al. 2002">{{cite journal |last1=Fiske |first1=Susan T. |last2=Cuddy |first2=Amy J. C. |last3=Glick |first3=Peter |last4=Xu |first4=Jun |date=2002 |title=A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow From Perceived Status and Competition |journal=] |volume=82 |issue=6 |pages=878–902 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878 |url= http://www.cos.gatech.edu/facultyres/Diversity_Studies/Fiske_StereotypeContent.pdf |pmid=12051578 |citeseerx=10.1.1.320.4001 |s2cid=17057403}}</ref><ref name=":1">{{cite book |last1=Cuddy |first1=Amy J. C. |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |editor1-first=Todd D. |editor1-last=Nelson |title=Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice against Older Persons |date=2002 |publisher=] |location=Cambridge, Massachusetts |isbn=9780262140775 |pages=7–8 |chapter=Doddering But Dear: Process, Content, and Function in Stereotyping of Older Persons |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=UvxEoFQ0LYwC&pg=PA7}}</ref> For instance, Katz and Braly argued in their classic 1933 study that ethnic stereotypes were uniformly negative.<ref name="operario&fiske" /> | ||
By contrast, a newer ] theorizes that stereotypes are frequently ambivalent and vary along two dimensions: warmth and competence. Warmth and competence are respectively predicted by lack of |
By contrast, a newer ] theorizes that stereotypes are frequently ambivalent and vary along two dimensions: warmth and competence. Warmth and competence are respectively predicted by lack of competition and ]. Groups that do not compete with the in-group for the same resources (e.g., college space) are perceived as warm, whereas high-status (e.g., economically or educationally successful) groups are considered competent. The groups within each of the four combinations of high and low levels of warmth and competence elicit distinct emotions.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Dovidio |author1-link=John Dovidio |first1=John F. |last2=Gaertner |first2=Samuel L. |editor1-first=Fiske |editor1-last=Susan T. |editor2-first=Daniel T. |editor2-last=Gilbert |editor3-first=Gardner |editor3-last=Lindzey |title=Handbook of Social Psychology |edition=5th |volume=2 |date=2010 |publisher=John Wiley |location=Hooboken, New Jersey |isbn=9780470137475 |page= |chapter=Intergroup Bias |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=Pye5IkCFgRYC&pg=PA1085 |url= https://archive.org/details/handbookofsocial5ed2unse/page/1085}}</ref> The model explains the phenomenon that some out-groups are admired but disliked, whereas others are liked but disrespected. This model was empirically tested on a variety of national and international ] and was found to reliably predict stereotype content.<ref name="fiske et al. 2002" /><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Cuddy |first1=Amy J. C. |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |last3=Kwan |first3=Virginia S. Y. |last4=Glick |first4=Peter |last5=Demoulin |first5=Stéphanie |last6=Leyens |first6=Jacques-Philippe |last7=Bond |first7=Michael Harris |last8=Croizet |first8=Jean-Claude |last9=Ellemers |first9=Naomi |last10=Sleebos |first10=Ed |last11=Htun |first11=Tin Tin |last12=Kim |first12=Hyun-Jeong |last13=Maio |first13=Greg |last14=Perry |first14=Judi |last15=Petkova |first15=Kristina |last16=Todorov |first16=Valery |last17=Rodríguez-Bailón |first17=Rosa |last18=Morales |first18=Elena |last19=Moya |first19=Miguel |last20=Palacios |first20=Marisol |last21=Smith |first21=Vanessa |last22=Perez |first22=Rolando |last23=Vala |first23=Jorge |last24=Ziegler |first24=Rene |title=Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences |journal=British Journal of Social Psychology |date=March 2009 |volume=48 |issue=1 |pages=1–33 |doi=10.1348/014466608X314935 |pmid=19178758 |pmc=3912751}}</ref> | ||
An even more recent model of stereotype content called the agency–beliefs–communion (ABC) model suggested that methods to study warmth and competence in the stereotype content model (SCM) were missing a crucial element, that being, stereotypes of social groups are often spontaneously generated.<ref name="Koch, Imhoff, et al. 2016">Koch, A.; Imhoff, R.; Dotsch, R.; Unkelbach, C.; & Alves, H. (2016). "The ABC of Stereotypes About Groups: Agency/Socioeconomic Success, Conservative-Progressive Beliefs, and Communion". ''Journal of Personality and Social Psychology'', 110, |
An even more recent model of stereotype content called the agency–beliefs–communion (ABC) model suggested that methods to study warmth and competence in the stereotype content model (SCM) were missing a crucial element, that being, stereotypes of social groups are often spontaneously generated.<ref name="Koch, Imhoff, et al. 2016">Koch, A.; Imhoff, R.; Dotsch, R.; Unkelbach, C.; & Alves, H. (2016). "The ABC of Stereotypes About Groups: Agency/Socioeconomic Success, Conservative-Progressive Beliefs, and Communion". ''Journal of Personality and Social Psychology'', 110, 675–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000046</ref> Experiments on the SCM usually ask participants to rate traits according to warmth and competence but this does not allow participants to use any other stereotype dimensions.<ref>"Groups' warmth is a personal matter: Understanding consensus on stereotype dimensions reconciles adversarial models of social evaluation". ''Journal of Experimental Social Psychology'', 89, 103995. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103995</ref> The ABC model, proposed by Koch and colleagues in 2016 is an estimate of how people spontaneously stereotype U.S social groups of people using traits. Koch et al. conducted several studies asking participants to list groups and sort them according to their similarity.<ref name="Koch, Imhoff, et al. 2016" /> Using statistical techniques, they revealed three dimensions that explained the similarity ratings. These three dimensions were agency (A), beliefs (B), and communion (C). Agency is associated with reaching goals, standing out and socio-economic status and is related to competence in the SCM, with some examples of traits including poor and wealthy, powerful and powerless, low status and high status. Beliefs is associated with views on the world, morals and conservative-progressive beliefs with some examples of traits including traditional and modern, religious and science-oriented or conventional and alternative. Finally, communion is associated with connecting with others and fitting in and is similar to warmth from the SCM, with some examples of traits including trustworthy and untrustworthy, cold and warm and repellent and likeable.<ref>Koch, A.; Yzerbyt, V.; Abele, A.; Ellemers, N.; & Fiske, S. T. (2021). "Chapter One – Social evaluation: Comparing models across interpersonal, intragroup, intergroup, several-group, and many-group contexts". In: Gawronski, B. (ed.), ''Advances in Experimental Social Psychology'' (Vol. 63, pp. 1–68). Academic Press. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2020.11.001</ref> According to research using this model, there is a curvilinear relationship between agency and communion.<ref>Dragon, P.; Holland, R.; Szumowska, E.; Suska, A.; Kossowska, M.; & Wigboldus, D. (2022). "Explaining the relationship between agency and communion by means of lay personality theories and connectionist modelling". https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/g847b"</ref> For example, if a group is high or low in the agency dimension then they may be seen as un-communal, whereas groups that are average in agency are seen as more communal.<ref>Cao, Y. T.; Sotnikova, A.; Daum'e, H.; Rudinger, R.; & Zou, L. X. (2022). "Theory-grounded Measurement of U.S. Social Stereotypes in English Language Models". ''North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics''.</ref> This model has many implications in predicting behaviour towards stereotyped groups. For example, Koch and colleagues recently proposed that perceived similarity in agency and beliefs increases inter-group cooperation.<ref>Koch, A.; Dorrough, A.; Glöckner, A.; & Imhoff, R. (2020). "The ABC of society: Perceived similarity in agency/socioeconomic success and conservative-progressive beliefs increases intergroup cooperation". ''Journal of Experimental Social Psychology'', 90, 103996. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103996</ref> | ||
==Functions== | ==Functions== | ||
Line 55: | Line 56: | ||
Stereotyping can serve cognitive functions on an interpersonal level, and social functions on an intergroup level.<ref name="Cox et al. (2012)" /><ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> For stereotyping to function on an intergroup level (see social identity approaches: ] and ]), an individual must see themselves as part of a group and being part of that group must also be salient for the individual.<ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> | Stereotyping can serve cognitive functions on an interpersonal level, and social functions on an intergroup level.<ref name="Cox et al. (2012)" /><ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> For stereotyping to function on an intergroup level (see social identity approaches: ] and ]), an individual must see themselves as part of a group and being part of that group must also be salient for the individual.<ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> | ||
Craig McGarty, Russell Spears, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2002) argued that the cognitive functions of stereotyping are best understood in relation to its social functions, and vice versa.<ref name="McGarty Spears et al. (2002)">{{cite book |last1=McGarty |first1=Craig |last3=Yzerbyt |first3=Vincent Y. |last2=Spears |first2=Russel |title=Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups |url= https://archive.org/details/stereotypesasexp00mcga |url-access=limited |date=2002 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=9780521800471 |pages=–199|chapter=Conclusion: stereotypes are selective, variable and contested explanations}}</ref> | Craig McGarty, Russell Spears, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2002) argued that the cognitive functions of stereotyping are best understood in relation to its social functions, and vice versa.<ref name="McGarty Spears et al. (2002)">{{cite book |last1=McGarty |first1=Craig |last3=Yzerbyt |first3=Vincent Y. |last2=Spears |first2=Russel |title=Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups |url= https://archive.org/details/stereotypesasexp00mcga |url-access=limited |date=2002 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=9780521800471 |pages=–199 |chapter=Conclusion: stereotypes are selective, variable and contested explanations}}</ref> | ||
===Cognitive functions=== | ===Cognitive functions=== | ||
Line 72: | Line 73: | ||
=====Explanation purposes===== | =====Explanation purposes===== | ||
] 1873 caricature depicting the stereotypical physical features of a Jewish male]] | ] 1873 caricature depicting the stereotypical physical features of a Jewish male]] | ||
As mentioned previously, stereotypes can be used to explain social events.<ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /><ref name="Haslam et al. (2002)" /> ]<ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> described his observations of how some people found that the antisemitic fabricated contents of '']'' only made sense if Jews have certain characteristics. Therefore, according to Tajfel,<ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> Jews were stereotyped as being evil and yearning for world domination to match the antisemitic "facts" as presented in The Protocols of the Elders of Zion. | As mentioned previously, stereotypes can be used to explain social events.<ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /><ref name="Haslam et al. (2002)" /> ]<ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> described his observations of how some people found that the antisemitic fabricated contents of '']'' only made sense if Jews have certain characteristics. Therefore, according to Tajfel,<ref name="Tajfel (1981)" /> Jews were stereotyped as being evil and yearning for world domination to match the antisemitic "facts" as presented in The Protocols of the Elders of Zion. | ||
Line 85: | Line 86: | ||
====Self-categorization==== | ====Self-categorization==== | ||
Stereotypes can emphasize a person's group membership in two steps: Stereotypes emphasize the person's similarities with ingroup members on relevant dimensions, and also the person's differences from outgroup members on relevant dimensions.<ref name=":1" /> People change the stereotype of their ingroups and outgroups to suit context.<ref name=":1" /> Once an outgroup treats an ingroup member badly, they are more drawn to the members of their own group.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1080/15298868.2012.712753 |volume=12 |title=Comparing Self-stereotyping with In-group-stereotyping and Out-group-stereotyping in Unequal-status Groups: The Case of Gender |date=2013 |journal=Self and Identity |pages=582–596 |last1=Cadinu |first1=Mara |last2=Latrofa |first2=Marcella |last3=Carnaghi |first3=Andrea |
Stereotypes can emphasize a person's group membership in two steps: Stereotypes emphasize the person's similarities with ingroup members on relevant dimensions, and also the person's differences from outgroup members on relevant dimensions.<ref name=":1" /> People change the stereotype of their ingroups and outgroups to suit context.<ref name=":1" /> Once an outgroup treats an ingroup member badly, they are more drawn to the members of their own group.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1080/15298868.2012.712753 |volume=12 |title=Comparing Self-stereotyping with In-group-stereotyping and Out-group-stereotyping in Unequal-status Groups: The Case of Gender |date=2013 |journal=Self and Identity |pages=582–596 |last1=Cadinu |first1=Mara |last2=Latrofa |first2=Marcella |last3=Carnaghi |first3=Andrea |issue=6 |s2cid=145053003}}</ref> This can be seen as members within a group are able to relate to each other though a stereotype because of identical situations. A person can embrace a stereotype to avoid humiliation such as failing a task and blaming it on a stereotype.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2007.01.008 |volume=44 |title=Endorsing a negative in-group stereotype as a self-protective strategy: Sacrificing the group to save the self |date=2008 |journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology |pages=37–49 |last1=Burkley |first1=Melissa |last2=Blanton |first2=Hart}}</ref> | ||
====Social influence and consensus==== | ====Social influence and consensus==== | ||
Line 93: | Line 94: | ||
==Formation== | ==Formation== | ||
Different disciplines give different accounts of how stereotypes develop: Psychologists may focus on an individual's experience with groups, patterns of communication about those groups, and intergroup conflict. As for sociologists, they may focus on the relations among different groups in a social structure. They suggest that stereotypes are the result of conflict, poor parenting, and inadequate mental and emotional development. Once stereotypes have formed, there are two main factors that explain their persistence. First, the cognitive effects of schematic processing (see ]) make it so that when a member of a group behaves as we expect, the behavior confirms and even strengthens existing stereotypes. Second, the affective or emotional aspects of prejudice render logical arguments against stereotypes ineffective in countering the power of emotional responses.<ref>Aronson |
Different disciplines give different accounts of how stereotypes develop: Psychologists may focus on an individual's experience with groups, patterns of communication about those groups, and intergroup conflict. As for sociologists, they may focus on the relations among different groups in a social structure. They suggest that stereotypes are the result of conflict, poor parenting, and inadequate mental and emotional development. Once stereotypes have formed, there are two main factors that explain their persistence. First, the cognitive effects of schematic processing (see ]) make it so that when a member of a group behaves as we expect, the behavior confirms and even strengthens existing stereotypes. Second, the affective or emotional aspects of prejudice render logical arguments against stereotypes ineffective in countering the power of emotional responses.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Aronson |first1=E. |last2=Wilson |first2=T. D. |last3=Akert |first3=R. M. |date=2010 |title=Social Psychology |edition=7th |location=New York |publisher=Pearson}}</ref> | ||
===Correspondence bias=== | ===Correspondence bias=== | ||
Line 99: | Line 100: | ||
''Correspondence bias'' refers to the tendency to ascribe a person's behavior to ] or personality, and to underestimate the extent to which situational factors elicited the behavior. Correspondence bias can play an important role in stereotype formation.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Mackie |first1=Diane M. |last2=Hamilton |first2=David L. |last3=Susskind |first3=Joshua |last4=Rosselli |first4=Francine |editor1-first=C. Neil |editor1-last=MacRae |editor2-first=Charles |editor2-last=Stangor |editor3-first=Miles |editor3-last=Hewstone |title=Stereotypes and Stereotyping |date=1996 |publisher=Guilford Press |location=New York |isbn=9781572300538 |pages=48–49 |chapter=Social Psychological Foundations of Stereotype Formation |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=o2EVqBMpJDEC&pg=PA48}}</ref> | ''Correspondence bias'' refers to the tendency to ascribe a person's behavior to ] or personality, and to underestimate the extent to which situational factors elicited the behavior. Correspondence bias can play an important role in stereotype formation.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Mackie |first1=Diane M. |last2=Hamilton |first2=David L. |last3=Susskind |first3=Joshua |last4=Rosselli |first4=Francine |editor1-first=C. Neil |editor1-last=MacRae |editor2-first=Charles |editor2-last=Stangor |editor3-first=Miles |editor3-last=Hewstone |title=Stereotypes and Stereotyping |date=1996 |publisher=Guilford Press |location=New York |isbn=9781572300538 |pages=48–49 |chapter=Social Psychological Foundations of Stereotype Formation |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=o2EVqBMpJDEC&pg=PA48}}</ref> | ||
For example, in a study by Roguer and Yzerbyt (1999) participants watched a video showing students who were randomly instructed to find arguments either for or against ]. The students that argued in favor of euthanasia came from the same law department or from different departments. Results showed that participants attributed the students' responses to their attitudes although it had been made clear in the video that students had no choice about their position. Participants reported that group membership, i.e., the department that the students belonged to, affected the students' opinions about euthanasia. Law students were perceived to be more in favor of euthanasia than students from different departments despite the fact that a pretest had revealed that subjects had no preexisting expectations about attitudes toward euthanasia and the department that students belong to. The attribution error created the new stereotype that law students are more likely to support euthanasia.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Rogier |first1=Anouk |last2=Yzerbyt |first2=Vincent |title=Social attribution, correspondence bias, and the emergence of stereotypes |date=1999 |journal=Swiss Journal of Psychology |volume=58 |issue=4 |pages=233–240 |doi=10.1024//1421-0185.58.4.233 |url= http://www.psor.ucl.ac.be/personal/yzerbyt/Rogier%20&%20Yzerbyt%20SJP%201999.pdf |access-date=5 April 2013 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20130729071003/http://www.psor.ucl.ac.be/personal/yzerbyt/Rogier%20%26%20Yzerbyt%20SJP%201999.pdf |archive-date=29 July 2013}}</ref> | For example, in a study by Roguer and Yzerbyt (1999) participants watched a video showing students who were randomly instructed to find arguments either for or against ]. The students that argued in favor of euthanasia came from the same law department or from different departments. Results showed that participants attributed the students' responses to their attitudes although it had been made clear in the video that students had no choice about their position. Participants reported that group membership, i.e., the department that the students belonged to, affected the students' opinions about euthanasia. Law students were perceived to be more in favor of euthanasia than students from different departments despite the fact that a pretest had revealed that subjects had no preexisting expectations about attitudes toward euthanasia and the department that students belong to. The attribution error created the new stereotype that law students are more likely to support euthanasia.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Rogier |first1=Anouk |last2=Yzerbyt |first2=Vincent |title=Social attribution, correspondence bias, and the emergence of stereotypes |date=1999 |journal=] |volume=58 |issue=4 |pages=233–240 |doi=10.1024//1421-0185.58.4.233 |url= http://www.psor.ucl.ac.be/personal/yzerbyt/Rogier%20&%20Yzerbyt%20SJP%201999.pdf |access-date=5 April 2013 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20130729071003/http://www.psor.ucl.ac.be/personal/yzerbyt/Rogier%20%26%20Yzerbyt%20SJP%201999.pdf |archive-date=29 July 2013}}</ref> | ||
Nier et al. (2012) found that people who tend to draw dispositional inferences from behavior and ignore situational constraints are more likely to stereotype low-status groups as incompetent and high-status groups as competent. Participants listened to descriptions of two fictitious groups of ]s, one of which was described as being higher in status than the other. In a second study, subjects rated actual groups – the poor and wealthy, women and men – in the United States in terms of their competence. Subjects who scored high on the measure of correspondence bias stereotyped the poor, women, and the fictitious lower-status Pacific Islanders as incompetent whereas they stereotyped the wealthy, men, and the high-status Pacific Islanders as competent. The correspondence bias was a significant predictor of stereotyping even after controlling for other measures that have been linked to beliefs about low status groups, the ] and ].<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nier |first1=Jason A. |last2=Bajaj |first2=Priya |last3=McLean |first3=Meghan C. |last4=Schwartz |first4=Elizabeth |title=Group status, perceptions of agency, and the correspondence bias: Attributional processes in the formation of stereotypes about high and low status groups |date=2012 |journal=] |volume=16 |issue=4 |pages=476–487 |doi=10.1177/1368430212454925 |s2cid=145399213}}</ref> | Nier et al. (2012) found that people who tend to draw dispositional inferences from behavior and ignore situational constraints are more likely to stereotype low-status groups as incompetent and high-status groups as competent. Participants listened to descriptions of two fictitious groups of ]s, one of which was described as being higher in status than the other. In a second study, subjects rated actual groups – the poor and wealthy, women and men – in the United States in terms of their competence. Subjects who scored high on the measure of correspondence bias stereotyped the poor, women, and the fictitious lower-status Pacific Islanders as incompetent whereas they stereotyped the wealthy, men, and the high-status Pacific Islanders as competent. The correspondence bias was a significant predictor of stereotyping even after controlling for other measures that have been linked to beliefs about low status groups, the ] and ].<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nier |first1=Jason A. |last2=Bajaj |first2=Priya |last3=McLean |first3=Meghan C. |last4=Schwartz |first4=Elizabeth |title=Group status, perceptions of agency, and the correspondence bias: Attributional processes in the formation of stereotypes about high and low status groups |date=2012 |journal=] |volume=16 |issue=4 |pages=476–487 |doi=10.1177/1368430212454925 |s2cid=145399213}}</ref> | ||
Based on the anti-public sector bias,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Marvel |first=John D. |date=30 January 2015 |title=Unconscious Bias in Citizens' Evaluations of Public Sector Performance |journal=Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory |pages=muu053 |doi=10.1093/jopart/muu053 |issn=1053-1858 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Döring and Willems (2021)<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Döring |first1=Matthias |last2=Willems |first2=Jurgen |date=22 September 2021 |title=Processing stereotypes: Professionalism confirmed or disconfirmed by sector affiliation? |journal=International Public Management Journal |volume=26 |issue=2 |pages=221–239 |doi=10.1080/10967494.2021.1971125 |s2cid=239261451 |issn=1096-7494 |doi-access=free}}</ref> found that employees in the public sector are considered as less professional compared to employees in the private sector. They build on the assumption that the ] and bureaucratic nature of the public sector spills over in the perception that citizens have about the employees working in the sector. With an experimental vignette study, they analyze how citizens process information on employees' sector affiliation, and integrate non-work role-referencing to test the stereotype confirmation assumption underlying the ]. The results show that sector as well as non-work role-referencing influences perceived employee professionalism but has little effect on the confirmation of particular public sector stereotypes.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Willems |first=Jurgen |date=2020 |title=Public servant stereotypes: It is not (at) all about being lazy, greedy and corrupt |journal=Public Administration |volume=98 |issue=4 |pages=807–823 |doi=10.1111/padm.12686|s2cid=221760839|issn=1467-9299 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Moreover, the results do not confirm a congruity effect of consistent stereotypical information: non-work role-referencing does not aggravate the negative effect of sector affiliation on perceived employee professionalism. | Based on the anti-public sector bias,<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Marvel |first=John D. |date=30 January 2015 |title=Unconscious Bias in Citizens' Evaluations of Public Sector Performance |journal=Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory |pages=muu053 |doi=10.1093/jopart/muu053 |issn=1053-1858 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Döring and Willems (2021)<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Döring |first1=Matthias |last2=Willems |first2=Jurgen |date=22 September 2021 |title=Processing stereotypes: Professionalism confirmed or disconfirmed by sector affiliation? |journal=International Public Management Journal |volume=26 |issue=2 |pages=221–239 |doi=10.1080/10967494.2021.1971125 |s2cid=239261451 |issn=1096-7494 |doi-access=free}}</ref> found that employees in the public sector are considered as less professional compared to employees in the private sector. They build on the assumption that the ] and bureaucratic nature of the public sector spills over in the perception that citizens have about the employees working in the sector. With an experimental vignette study, they analyze how citizens process information on employees' sector affiliation, and integrate non-work role-referencing to test the stereotype confirmation assumption underlying the ]. The results show that sector as well as non-work role-referencing influences perceived employee professionalism but has little effect on the confirmation of particular public sector stereotypes.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Willems |first=Jurgen |date=2020 |title=Public servant stereotypes: It is not (at) all about being lazy, greedy and corrupt |journal=Public Administration |volume=98 |issue=4 |pages=807–823 |doi=10.1111/padm.12686 |s2cid=221760839 |issn=1467-9299 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Moreover, the results do not confirm a congruity effect of consistent stereotypical information: non-work role-referencing does not aggravate the negative effect of sector affiliation on perceived employee professionalism. | ||
===Illusory correlation=== | ===Illusory correlation=== | ||
{{main|Illusory correlation}} | {{main|Illusory correlation}} | ||
Research has shown that stereotypes can develop based on a cognitive mechanism known as illusory correlation – an erroneous inference about the relationship between two events.<ref name="McGarty Yzerbyt et al. (2002)" /><ref name="mullen&johnson">{{cite journal |last1=Mullen |first1=Brian |last2=Johnson |first2=Craig |date=1990 |title=Distinctiveness-based illusory correlations and stereotyping: A meta-analytic integration |journal=] |volume=29 |issue=1 |pages=11–28 |doi=10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00883.x}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Meiser |first1=Thorsten |editor1-first=Klaus |editor1-last=Fiedler |editor2-first=Peter |editor2-last=Justin |title=Information Sampling and Adaptive Cognition |date=2006 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=9780521831598 |pages=183–209 |chapter=Contingency Learning and Biased Group Impressions |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=RMZL_2H8A4kC&pg=PA183}}</ref> If two statistically infrequent events co-occur, observers overestimate the frequency of co-occurrence of these events. The underlying reason is that rare, infrequent events are distinctive and ] and, when paired, become even more so. The heightened salience results in more attention and more effective ], which strengthens the belief that the events are ].<ref name="hamilton&gifford">{{cite journal |last1=Hamilton |first1=David L. |last2=Gifford |first2=Robert K. |date=1976 |title=Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments |journal=] |volume=12 |issue=4 |pages=392–407 |doi=10.1016/S0022-1031(76)80006-6}}</ref><ref name="Berndsen et al. (2002)">{{cite book |last1=Berndsen |first1=Mariëtte |last2=Spears |first2=Russel |last3=van der Pligt |first3=Joop |last4=McGarty |first4=Craig |editor1-last=McGarty |editor1-first=Craig |editor2-last=Yzerbyt |editor2-first=Vincent Y. |editor3-last=Spears |editor3-first=Russel |title=Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups |date=2002 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=9780521800471 |pages=90–110|chapter=Illusory correlation and stereotype formation: making sense of group differences and cognitive biases |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=dkn8dceHRg8C&pg=PA90}}</ref><ref name="stroessner&plaks">{{Cite book |last1=Stroessner |first1=Steven J. |last2=Plaks |first2=Jason E. |chapter=Illusory Correlation and Stereotype Formation: Tracing the Arc of Research Over a Quarter Century |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=DE-cJ8F4rSMC&pg=PA247 |pages=247–259 |editor-first=Gordon B. |editor-last=Moskowitz |title=Cognitive Social Psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |location=Mahwah, New Jersey |date=2001 |isbn=9780805834147}}</ref> | Research has shown that stereotypes can develop based on a cognitive mechanism known as illusory correlation – an erroneous inference about the relationship between two events.<ref name="McGarty Yzerbyt et al. (2002)" /><ref name="mullen&johnson">{{cite journal |last1=Mullen |first1=Brian |last2=Johnson |first2=Craig |date=1990 |title=Distinctiveness-based illusory correlations and stereotyping: A meta-analytic integration |journal=] |volume=29 |issue=1 |pages=11–28 |doi=10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00883.x}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Meiser |first1=Thorsten |editor1-first=Klaus |editor1-last=Fiedler |editor2-first=Peter |editor2-last=Justin |title=Information Sampling and Adaptive Cognition |date=2006 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=9780521831598 |pages=183–209 |chapter=Contingency Learning and Biased Group Impressions |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=RMZL_2H8A4kC&pg=PA183}}</ref> If two statistically infrequent events co-occur, observers overestimate the frequency of co-occurrence of these events. The underlying reason is that rare, infrequent events are distinctive and ] and, when paired, become even more so. The heightened salience results in more attention and more effective ], which strengthens the belief that the events are ].<ref name="hamilton&gifford">{{cite journal |last1=Hamilton |first1=David L. |last2=Gifford |first2=Robert K. |date=1976 |title=Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments |journal=] |volume=12 |issue=4 |pages=392–407 |doi=10.1016/S0022-1031(76)80006-6}}</ref><ref name="Berndsen et al. (2002)">{{cite book |last1=Berndsen |first1=Mariëtte |last2=Spears |first2=Russel |last3=van der Pligt |first3=Joop |last4=McGarty |first4=Craig |editor1-last=McGarty |editor1-first=Craig |editor2-last=Yzerbyt |editor2-first=Vincent Y. |editor3-last=Spears |editor3-first=Russel |title=Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups |date=2002 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=9780521800471 |pages=90–110 |chapter=Illusory correlation and stereotype formation: making sense of group differences and cognitive biases |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=dkn8dceHRg8C&pg=PA90}}</ref><ref name="stroessner&plaks">{{Cite book |last1=Stroessner |first1=Steven J. |last2=Plaks |first2=Jason E. |chapter=Illusory Correlation and Stereotype Formation: Tracing the Arc of Research Over a Quarter Century |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=DE-cJ8F4rSMC&pg=PA247 |pages=247–259 |editor-first=Gordon B. |editor-last=Moskowitz |title=Cognitive Social Psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |location=Mahwah, New Jersey |date=2001 |isbn=9780805834147}}</ref> | ||
In the inter-group context, illusory correlations lead people to misattribute rare behaviors or traits at higher rates to ] members than to majority groups, even when both display the same proportion of the behaviors or traits. ], for instance, are a minority group in the |
In the inter-group context, illusory correlations lead people to misattribute rare behaviors or traits at higher rates to ] members than to majority groups, even when both display the same proportion of the behaviors or traits. ], for instance, are a minority group in the United States and interaction with blacks is a relatively infrequent event for an average ].<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Ajrouch |first1=Kristine J. |last2=Antonucci |first2=Toni C. |last3=Janevic |first3=Mary R. |date=1 March 2001 |title=Social Networks Among Blacks and Whites: The Interaction Between Race and Age |journal=The Journals of Gerontology |volume=56 |issue=2 |pages=S112–S118 |doi=10.1093/geronb/56.2.S112 |pmid=11245365 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Similarly, undesirable behavior (e.g. crime) is statistically less frequent than desirable behavior. Since both events "blackness" and "undesirable behavior" are distinctive in the sense that they are infrequent, the combination of the two leads observers to overestimate the rate of co-occurrence.<ref name="hamilton&gifford" /> Similarly, in workplaces where women are underrepresented and negative behaviors such as errors occur less frequently than positive behaviors, women become more strongly associated with mistakes than men.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Moskowitz |first1=Gordon B. |author-link1=Gordon Moskowitz |title=Social Cognition: Understanding Self and Others |url= https://books.google.com/books?id=_-NLW8Ynvp8C&pg=PA182 |date=2005 |publisher=] |location=New York |isbn=9781593850852 |page=182}}</ref> | ||
In a landmark study, David Hamilton and Richard Gifford (1976) examined the role of illusory correlation in stereotype formation. Subjects were instructed to read descriptions of behaviors performed by members of groups A and B. Negative behaviors outnumbered positive actions and group B was smaller than group A, making negative behaviors and membership in group B relatively infrequent and distinctive. Participants were then asked who had performed a set of actions: a person of group A or group B. Results showed that subjects overestimated the frequency with which both distinctive events, membership in group B and negative behavior, co-occurred, and evaluated group B more negatively. This despite the fact the proportion of positive to negative behaviors was equivalent for both groups and that there was no actual correlation between group membership and behaviors.<ref name="hamilton&gifford" /> Although Hamilton and Gifford found a similar effect for positive behaviors as the infrequent events, a ] review of studies showed that illusory correlation effects are stronger when the infrequent, distinctive information is negative.<ref name="mullen&johnson" /> | In a landmark study, David Hamilton and Richard Gifford (1976) examined the role of illusory correlation in stereotype formation. Subjects were instructed to read descriptions of behaviors performed by members of groups A and B. Negative behaviors outnumbered positive actions and group B was smaller than group A, making negative behaviors and membership in group B relatively infrequent and distinctive. Participants were then asked who had performed a set of actions: a person of group A or group B. Results showed that subjects overestimated the frequency with which both distinctive events, membership in group B and negative behavior, co-occurred, and evaluated group B more negatively. This despite the fact the proportion of positive to negative behaviors was equivalent for both groups and that there was no actual correlation between group membership and behaviors.<ref name="hamilton&gifford" /> Although Hamilton and Gifford found a similar effect for positive behaviors as the infrequent events, a ] review of studies showed that illusory correlation effects are stronger when the infrequent, distinctive information is negative.<ref name="mullen&johnson" /> | ||
Line 133: | Line 134: | ||
The dual-process model of cognitive processing of stereotypes asserts that automatic activation of stereotypes is followed by a controlled processing stage, during which an individual may choose to disregard or ignore the stereotyped information that has been brought to mind.<ref name="faculty.washington.edu" /> | The dual-process model of cognitive processing of stereotypes asserts that automatic activation of stereotypes is followed by a controlled processing stage, during which an individual may choose to disregard or ignore the stereotyped information that has been brought to mind.<ref name="faculty.washington.edu" /> | ||
A number of studies have found that stereotypes are activated automatically. ] (1989), for example, suggested that stereotypes are automatically activated in the presence of a member (or some symbolic equivalent) of a stereotyped group and that the unintentional activation of the stereotype is equally strong for high- and low-prejudice persons. Words related to the cultural stereotype of blacks were presented ]. During an ostensibly unrelated ] task, subjects read a paragraph describing a race-unspecified target person's behaviors and rated the target person on several trait scales. Results showed that participants who received a high proportion of racial words rated the target person in the story as significantly more hostile than participants who were presented with a lower proportion of words related to the stereotype. This effect held true for both high- and low-prejudice subjects (as measured by the Modern Racism Scale). Thus, the racial stereotype was activated even for low-prejudice individuals who did not personally endorse it.<ref name="faculty.washington.edu" /><ref>{{cite book |last1=Devine |first1=Patricia G. |last2=Monteith |first2=Margo J. |editor1-first=Shelly |editor1-last=Chaiken |editor2-first=Yaacov |editor2-last=Trope |title=Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology |date=1999 |publisher=Guilford Press |location=New York |isbn=9781572304215 |pages=341–342 |chapter=Automaticty and Control in Stereotyping |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=5X_auIBx99EC&pg=PA341}}</ref><ref name="Bargh 1994">{{cite book |last1=Bargh |first1=John A. |author-link1=John Bargh |editor1-first=Robert S. |editor1-last=Wyer |editor2-first=Thomas K. |editor2-last=Srull |title=Handbook of Social Cognition |edition=2nd |volume=2 |date=1994 |publisher=Lawrence Earlbaum |location=Hillsdale, New Jersey |isbn=9780805810561 |page= |chapter=The Four Horsemen of Automaticity: Awareness, Intention, Efficiency, Control in Social Cognition |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=5ncW0DyNqVwC&pg=PA21 |url= https://archive.org/details/isbn_9780805810578/page/21}}</ref> Studies using alternative priming methods have shown that the activation of gender and age stereotypes can also be automatic.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Banaji |first1=Mahzarin R. |last2=Hardin |first2=Curtis D. |date=1996 |title=Automatic Stereotyping |journal=] |volume=7 |issue=3 |pages=136–141 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x |s2cid=207858486 |url=http://www.people.fas.harvard.edu/~mrbworks/articles/1996_PsySci.pdf |access-date=12 January 2013 |archive-date=21 May 2013 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130521084033/http://www.people.fas.harvard.edu/~mrbworks/articles/1996_PsySci.pdf |url-status=dead |
A number of studies have found that stereotypes are activated automatically. ] (1989), for example, suggested that stereotypes are automatically activated in the presence of a member (or some symbolic equivalent) of a stereotyped group and that the unintentional activation of the stereotype is equally strong for high- and low-prejudice persons. Words related to the cultural stereotype of blacks were presented ]. During an ostensibly unrelated ] task, subjects read a paragraph describing a race-unspecified target person's behaviors and rated the target person on several trait scales. Results showed that participants who received a high proportion of racial words rated the target person in the story as significantly more hostile than participants who were presented with a lower proportion of words related to the stereotype. This effect held true for both high- and low-prejudice subjects (as measured by the Modern Racism Scale). Thus, the racial stereotype was activated even for low-prejudice individuals who did not personally endorse it.<ref name="faculty.washington.edu" /><ref>{{cite book |last1=Devine |first1=Patricia G. |last2=Monteith |first2=Margo J. |editor1-first=Shelly |editor1-last=Chaiken |editor2-first=Yaacov |editor2-last=Trope |title=Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology |date=1999 |publisher=Guilford Press |location=New York |isbn=9781572304215 |pages=341–342 |chapter=Automaticty and Control in Stereotyping |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=5X_auIBx99EC&pg=PA341}}</ref><ref name="Bargh 1994">{{cite book |last1=Bargh |first1=John A. |author-link1=John Bargh |editor1-first=Robert S. |editor1-last=Wyer |editor2-first=Thomas K. |editor2-last=Srull |title=Handbook of Social Cognition |edition=2nd |volume=2 |date=1994 |publisher=Lawrence Earlbaum |location=Hillsdale, New Jersey |isbn=9780805810561 |page= |chapter=The Four Horsemen of Automaticity: Awareness, Intention, Efficiency, Control in Social Cognition |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=5ncW0DyNqVwC&pg=PA21 |url= https://archive.org/details/isbn_9780805810578/page/21}}</ref> Studies using alternative priming methods have shown that the activation of gender and age stereotypes can also be automatic.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Banaji |first1=Mahzarin R. |last2=Hardin |first2=Curtis D. |date=1996 |title=Automatic Stereotyping |journal=] |volume=7 |issue=3 |pages=136–141 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x |s2cid=207858486 |url= http://www.people.fas.harvard.edu/~mrbworks/articles/1996_PsySci.pdf |access-date=12 January 2013 |archive-date=21 May 2013 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20130521084033/http://www.people.fas.harvard.edu/~mrbworks/articles/1996_PsySci.pdf |url-status=dead}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Perdue |first1=Charles W. |last2=Gurtman |first2=Michael B. |date=1990 |title=Evidence for the automaticity of ageism |journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology |volume=26 |issue=3 |pages=199–216 |doi=10.1016/0022-1031(90)90035-K}}</ref> | ||
Subsequent research suggested that the relation between category activation and stereotype activation was more complex.<ref name="Bargh 1994" /><ref>{{cite book |last1=Brown |first1=Rupert |title=Prejudice: Its Social Psychology |url= https://books.google.com/books?id=PygYKbRoZjcC&pg=PA88 |edition=2nd |date=2010 |publisher=] |location=Oxford |isbn=9781405113069 |pages=88}}</ref> Lepore and Brown (1997), for instance, noted that the words used in Devine's study were both neutral category labels (e.g., "Blacks") and stereotypic attributes (e.g., "lazy"). They argued that if only the neutral category labels were presented, people high and low in prejudice would respond differently. In a design similar to Devine's, Lepore and Brown ] the category of African-Americans using labels such as "blacks" and "West Indians" and then assessed the differential activation of the associated stereotype in the subsequent impression-formation task. They found that high-prejudice participants increased their ratings of the target person on the negative stereotypic dimensions and decreased them on the positive dimension whereas low-prejudice subjects tended in the opposite direction. The results suggest that the level of prejudice and stereotype endorsement affects people's judgements when the category – and not the stereotype per se – is primed.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Lepore |first1=Lorella |last2=Brown |first2=Rupert |date=1997 |title=Category and Stereotype Activation: Is Prejudice Inevitable? |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=72 |issue=2 |pages=275–287 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.275 |url= http://www.atkinson.yorku.ca/~jsteele/PDF/Optional%20Readings/Lepore_Brown_JPSP_1997.pdf |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20140102191944/http://www.atkinson.yorku.ca/~jsteele/PDF/Optional%20Readings/Lepore_Brown_JPSP_1997.pdf |archive-date=2 January 2014}}</ref> | Subsequent research suggested that the relation between category activation and stereotype activation was more complex.<ref name="Bargh 1994" /><ref>{{cite book |last1=Brown |first1=Rupert |title=Prejudice: Its Social Psychology |url= https://books.google.com/books?id=PygYKbRoZjcC&pg=PA88 |edition=2nd |date=2010 |publisher=] |location=Oxford |isbn=9781405113069 |pages=88}}</ref> Lepore and Brown (1997), for instance, noted that the words used in Devine's study were both neutral category labels (e.g., "Blacks") and stereotypic attributes (e.g., "lazy"). They argued that if only the neutral category labels were presented, people high and low in prejudice would respond differently. In a design similar to Devine's, Lepore and Brown ] the category of African-Americans using labels such as "blacks" and "West Indians" and then assessed the differential activation of the associated stereotype in the subsequent impression-formation task. They found that high-prejudice participants increased their ratings of the target person on the negative stereotypic dimensions and decreased them on the positive dimension whereas low-prejudice subjects tended in the opposite direction. The results suggest that the level of prejudice and stereotype endorsement affects people's judgements when the category – and not the stereotype per se – is primed.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Lepore |first1=Lorella |last2=Brown |first2=Rupert |date=1997 |title=Category and Stereotype Activation: Is Prejudice Inevitable? |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=72 |issue=2 |pages=275–287 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.275 |url= http://www.atkinson.yorku.ca/~jsteele/PDF/Optional%20Readings/Lepore_Brown_JPSP_1997.pdf |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20140102191944/http://www.atkinson.yorku.ca/~jsteele/PDF/Optional%20Readings/Lepore_Brown_JPSP_1997.pdf |archive-date=2 January 2014}}</ref> | ||
Research has shown that people can be trained to activate ] information and thereby reduce the automatic activation of negative stereotypes. In a study by Kawakami et al. (2000), for example, participants were presented with a category label and taught to respond "No" to stereotypic traits and "Yes" to nonstereotypic traits. After this training period, subjects showed reduced stereotype activation.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kawakami |first1=Kerry |last2=Dovidio |first2=John F. |last3=Moll |first3=Jasper |last4=Hermsen |first4=Sander |last5=Russin |first5=Abby |date=2000 |title=Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=78 |issue=5 |pages=871–888|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871 |pmid=10821195}}</ref><ref name="Gawronski et al. 2008">{{cite journal |first1=Bertram |last1=Gawronski |first2=Roland |last2=Deutsch |first3=Sawsan |last3=Mbirkou |first4=Beate |last4=Seibt |first5=Fritz |last5=Strack |date=2008 |title=When 'Just Say No' is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation |journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology |volume=44 |issue=2 |pages=370–377 |doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2006.12.004}}</ref> This effect is based on the learning of new and more ]s rather than the negation of already existing ones.<ref name="Gawronski et al. 2008" /> | Research has shown that people can be trained to activate ] information and thereby reduce the automatic activation of negative stereotypes. In a study by Kawakami et al. (2000), for example, participants were presented with a category label and taught to respond "No" to stereotypic traits and "Yes" to nonstereotypic traits. After this training period, subjects showed reduced stereotype activation.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kawakami |first1=Kerry |last2=Dovidio |first2=John F. |last3=Moll |first3=Jasper |last4=Hermsen |first4=Sander |last5=Russin |first5=Abby |date=2000 |title=Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=78 |issue=5 |pages=871–888 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871 |pmid=10821195}}</ref><ref name="Gawronski et al. 2008">{{cite journal |first1=Bertram |last1=Gawronski |first2=Roland |last2=Deutsch |first3=Sawsan |last3=Mbirkou |first4=Beate |last4=Seibt |first5=Fritz |last5=Strack |date=2008 |title=When 'Just Say No' is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation |journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology |volume=44 |issue=2 |pages=370–377 |doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2006.12.004}}</ref> This effect is based on the learning of new and more ]s rather than the negation of already existing ones.<ref name="Gawronski et al. 2008" /> | ||
===Automatic behavioral outcomes=== | ===Automatic behavioral outcomes=== | ||
Empirical evidence suggests that stereotype activation can automatically influence social behavior.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Wheeler |first1=S. Christian |last2=Petty |first2=Richard E. |title=The Effects of Stereotype Activation on Behavior: A Review of Possible Mechanisms |date=2001 |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=127 |issue=6 |pages=797–826 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.797 |pmid=11726072 |url= http://www.communicationcache.com/uploads/1/0/8/8/10887248/the_effects_of_stereotype_activation_on_behavior_-_a_review_of_possible_mechanisms.pdf |access-date=10 June 2013}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book |editor1-first=Mark R. |editor1-last=Leary |editor2-first=June Price |editor2-last=Tangney |title=Handbook of self and identity |edition=2nd |location=New York |publisher=Guilford Press |date=2012 |isbn=9781462503056 |pages=164–165 |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=VukSQuVMQy0C&pg=PA164 |chapter=Implicit self and identity |last1=Devos |first1=Thierry |last2=Huynh |first2=Que-Lam |last3=Banaji |first3=Mahzarin D.}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book |editor1-first=Joseph P. |editor1-last=Forgas |editor2-first=Kipling D. |editor2-last=Williams |title=Social influence: direct and indirect processes |date=2001 |location=Philadelphia |publisher=] |chapter=Automatic social influence: The perception-behavior links as an explanatory mechanism for behavior matching |last=Dijksterhuis |first=Ap |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=RfGl17y_yCUC&pg=PA99 |pages=99–100 |isbn=9781841690391}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book |editor-first=Manuel |editor-last=London |title=How People Evaluate Others in Organizations |date=2001 |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum |location=Mahwah, New Jersey |isbn=9780805836127 |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=oqHDiUbhrBkC&pg=PA2012 |page= |chapter=Causes and Consequences of Stereotypes in Organizations |last1=Operario |first1=Din |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |url= https://archive.org/details/howpeopleevaluat00manu/page/2012}}</ref> For example, ], Chen, and Burrows (1996) activated the stereotype of the elderly among half of their participants by administering a scrambled-sentence test where participants saw words related to age stereotypes. Subjects primed with the stereotype walked significantly more slowly than the control group (although the test did not include any words specifically referring to slowness), thus acting in a way that the stereotype suggests that elderly people will act. And the stereotype of the elder will affect the subjective perception of them through depression.<ref>{{Cite book|doi=10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_489 |chapter=Alzheimer's Dementia |title=Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology |date=2011 |last1=Tschanz |first1=Joann T. |last2=Andersen |first2=Aaron |pages=99–105 |isbn=9780387799476}}</ref> In another experiment, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows also found that because the stereotype about blacks includes the notion of aggression, subliminal exposure to black faces increased the likelihood that randomly selected white college students reacted with more aggression and hostility than participants who subconsciously viewed a white face.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Bargh |first1=John A. |last2=Chen |first2=Mark |last3=Burrows |first3=Lara |title=Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action |date=1996 |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=71 |issue=2 |pages=230–244 |url= http://www.yale.edu/acmelab/articles/bargh_chen_burrows_1996.pdf |access-date=10 June 2013 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230 |pmid=8765481 |citeseerx=10.1.1.320.8563 |s2cid=6654763 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20131009182408/http://yale.edu/acmelab/articles/bargh_chen_burrows_1996.pdf |archive-date=9 October 2013}}</ref> Similarly, Correll et al. (2002) showed that activated stereotypes about blacks can influence people's behavior. In a series of experiments, black and white participants played a |
Empirical evidence suggests that stereotype activation can automatically influence social behavior.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Wheeler |first1=S. Christian |last2=Petty |first2=Richard E. |title=The Effects of Stereotype Activation on Behavior: A Review of Possible Mechanisms |date=2001 |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=127 |issue=6 |pages=797–826 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.797 |pmid=11726072 |url= http://www.communicationcache.com/uploads/1/0/8/8/10887248/the_effects_of_stereotype_activation_on_behavior_-_a_review_of_possible_mechanisms.pdf |access-date=10 June 2013}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book |editor1-first=Mark R. |editor1-last=Leary |editor2-first=June Price |editor2-last=Tangney |title=Handbook of self and identity |edition=2nd |location=New York |publisher=Guilford Press |date=2012 |isbn=9781462503056 |pages=164–165 |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=VukSQuVMQy0C&pg=PA164 |chapter=Implicit self and identity |last1=Devos |first1=Thierry |last2=Huynh |first2=Que-Lam |last3=Banaji |first3=Mahzarin D.}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book |editor1-first=Joseph P. |editor1-last=Forgas |editor2-first=Kipling D. |editor2-last=Williams |title=Social influence: direct and indirect processes |date=2001 |location=Philadelphia |publisher=] |chapter=Automatic social influence: The perception-behavior links as an explanatory mechanism for behavior matching |last=Dijksterhuis |first=Ap |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=RfGl17y_yCUC&pg=PA99 |pages=99–100 |isbn=9781841690391}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book |editor-first=Manuel |editor-last=London |title=How People Evaluate Others in Organizations |date=2001 |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum |location=Mahwah, New Jersey |isbn=9780805836127 |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=oqHDiUbhrBkC&pg=PA2012 |page= |chapter=Causes and Consequences of Stereotypes in Organizations |last1=Operario |first1=Din |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |url= https://archive.org/details/howpeopleevaluat00manu/page/2012}}</ref> For example, ], Chen, and Burrows (1996) activated the stereotype of the elderly among half of their participants by administering a scrambled-sentence test where participants saw words related to age stereotypes. Subjects primed with the stereotype walked significantly more slowly than the control group (although the test did not include any words specifically referring to slowness), thus acting in a way that the stereotype suggests that elderly people will act. And the stereotype of the elder will affect the subjective perception of them through depression.<ref>{{Cite book |doi=10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_489 |chapter=Alzheimer's Dementia |title=Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology |date=2011 |last1=Tschanz |first1=Joann T. |last2=Andersen |first2=Aaron |pages=99–105 |isbn=9780387799476}}</ref> In another experiment, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows also found that because the stereotype about blacks includes the notion of aggression, subliminal exposure to black faces increased the likelihood that randomly selected white college students reacted with more aggression and hostility than participants who subconsciously viewed a white face.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Bargh |first1=John A. |last2=Chen |first2=Mark |last3=Burrows |first3=Lara |title=Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action |date=1996 |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=71 |issue=2 |pages=230–244 |url= http://www.yale.edu/acmelab/articles/bargh_chen_burrows_1996.pdf |access-date=10 June 2013 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230 |pmid=8765481 |citeseerx=10.1.1.320.8563 |s2cid=6654763 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20131009182408/http://yale.edu/acmelab/articles/bargh_chen_burrows_1996.pdf |archive-date=9 October 2013}}</ref> Similarly, Correll et al. (2002) showed that activated stereotypes about blacks can influence people's behavior. In a series of experiments, black and white participants played a video game, in which a black or white person was shown holding a gun or a harmless object (e.g., a mobile phone). Participants had to decide as quickly as possible whether to shoot the target. When the target person was armed, both black and white participants were faster in deciding to shoot the target when he was black than when he was white. When the target was unarmed, the participants avoided shooting him more quickly when he was white. Time pressure made the shooter bias even more pronounced.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Correll |first1=Joshua |last2=Park |first2=Bernadette |last3=Judd |first3=Charles M. |last4=Wittenbrink |first4=Bernd |title=The Police Officer's Dilemma: Using Ethnicity to Disambiguate Potentially Threatening Individuals |date=2002 |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=83 |issue=6 |pages=1314–1329 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314 |citeseerx=10.1.1.466.7243 |pmid=12500813}}</ref> | ||
==Accuracy== | ==Accuracy== | ||
Line 149: | Line 150: | ||
Research on the role of ] in the formation of stereotypes suggests that stereotypes can develop because of incorrect inferences about the relationship between two events (e.g., membership in a social group and bad or good attributes). This means that at least some stereotypes are inaccurate.<ref name="mullen&johnson" /><ref name="hamilton&gifford" /><ref name="stroessner&plaks" /><ref name="McConnell et al. (1994)">{{cite journal |last1=McConnell |first1=Allen R. |last2=Sherman |first2=Steven J. |last3=Hamilton |first3=David L. |date=1994 |title=Illusory correlation in the perception of groups: an extension of the distinctiveness-based account |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=67 |issue=3 |pages=414–429 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.414 |pmid=7965600 |url= http://allenmcconnell.net/pdfs/edbe-JPSP-1994.pdf}}</ref> | Research on the role of ] in the formation of stereotypes suggests that stereotypes can develop because of incorrect inferences about the relationship between two events (e.g., membership in a social group and bad or good attributes). This means that at least some stereotypes are inaccurate.<ref name="mullen&johnson" /><ref name="hamilton&gifford" /><ref name="stroessner&plaks" /><ref name="McConnell et al. (1994)">{{cite journal |last1=McConnell |first1=Allen R. |last2=Sherman |first2=Steven J. |last3=Hamilton |first3=David L. |date=1994 |title=Illusory correlation in the perception of groups: an extension of the distinctiveness-based account |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=67 |issue=3 |pages=414–429 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.414 |pmid=7965600 |url= http://allenmcconnell.net/pdfs/edbe-JPSP-1994.pdf}}</ref> | ||
A 1995 book by Yueh-Ting Lee et al. argued that stereotypes are sometimes accurate.<ref>{{cite book |title=Stereotype Accuracy: Toward Appreciating Group Differences |publisher=American Psychological Association |date=September 1995 |isbn=9781557983077 |editor1-link=Yueh-Ting Lee |editor1-first=Yueh-Ting |editor1-last=Lee |editor2-first=Lee J. |editor2-last=Jussim |editor3-first=Clark R. |editor3-last=McCauley}}</ref> Similarly, a 2015 study by Jussim et al. reviewed four studies of racial stereotypes, and seven studies of gender stereotypes regarding demographic characteristics, academic achievement, personality and behavior, and argued that some aspects of ethnic and gender stereotypes are accurate while stereotypes concerning political affiliation and nationality are much less accurate.<ref name="Jussim">{{cite book |last1=Jussim |first1=Lee |last2=Cain |first2=Thomas R. |last3=Crawford |first3=Jarret T. |last4=Harber |first4=Kent |last5=Cohen |first5=Florette |title=Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination |url= https://archive.org/details/handbookprejudic00nels |url-access=limited |chapter=The unbearable accuracy of stereotypes |publisher=Psychology Press |location=New York |date=2009 |isbn=9780805859522 |editor-first=Todd D. |editor-last=Nelson |pages=–227}}</ref> |
A 1995 book by Yueh-Ting Lee et al. argued that stereotypes are sometimes accurate.<ref>{{cite book |title=Stereotype Accuracy: Toward Appreciating Group Differences |publisher=American Psychological Association |date=September 1995 |isbn=9781557983077 |editor1-link=Yueh-Ting Lee |editor1-first=Yueh-Ting |editor1-last=Lee |editor2-first=Lee J. |editor2-last=Jussim |editor3-first=Clark R. |editor3-last=McCauley}}</ref> Similarly, a 2015 study by Jussim et al. reviewed four studies of racial stereotypes, and seven studies of gender stereotypes regarding demographic characteristics, academic achievement, personality and behavior, and argued that some aspects of ethnic and gender stereotypes are accurate while stereotypes concerning political affiliation and nationality are much less accurate.<ref name="Jussim">{{cite book |last1=Jussim |first1=Lee |last2=Cain |first2=Thomas R. |last3=Crawford |first3=Jarret T. |last4=Harber |first4=Kent |last5=Cohen |first5=Florette |title=Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination |url= https://archive.org/details/handbookprejudic00nels |url-access=limited |chapter=The unbearable accuracy of stereotypes |publisher=Psychology Press |location=New York |date=2009 |isbn=9780805859522 |editor-first=Todd D. |editor-last=Nelson |pages=–227}}</ref> | ||
A 2005 study by Terracciano et al. found that stereotypic beliefs about nationality do not reflect the actual personality traits of people from different cultures.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Terracciano |first1=A. |last2=Abdel-Khalek |first2=A. M. |last3=Adám |first3=N. |last4=Adamovová |first4=L. |last5=Ahn |first5=C. K. |last6=Ahn |first6=H. N. |last7=Alansari |first7=B. M. |last8=Alcalay |first8=L. |last9=Allik |first9=J. |title=National Character Does Not Reflect Mean Personality Trait Levels in 49 Cultures |journal=] |date=2005 |volume=310 |pages=96–100 |doi=10.1126/science.1117199 |pmid=16210536 |pmc=2775052 |issue=5745 |bibcode=2005Sci...310...96T}}</ref> | A 2005 study by Terracciano et al. found that stereotypic beliefs about nationality do not reflect the actual personality traits of people from different cultures.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Terracciano |first1=A. |last2=Abdel-Khalek |first2=A. M. |last3=Adám |first3=N. |last4=Adamovová |first4=L. |last5=Ahn |first5=C. K. |last6=Ahn |first6=H. N. |last7=Alansari |first7=B. M. |last8=Alcalay |first8=L. |last9=Allik |first9=J. |title=National Character Does Not Reflect Mean Personality Trait Levels in 49 Cultures |journal=] |date=2005 |volume=310 |pages=96–100 |doi=10.1126/science.1117199 |pmid=16210536 |pmc=2775052 |issue=5745 |bibcode=2005Sci...310...96T}}</ref> | ||
Line 161: | Line 162: | ||
Attributive ambiguity refers to the uncertainty that members of stereotyped groups experience in interpreting the causes of others' behavior toward them. Stereotyped individuals who receive negative ] can ] it either to personal shortcomings, such as lack of ability or poor effort, or the evaluator's stereotypes and prejudice toward their social group. Alternatively, positive feedback can either be attributed to personal merit or discounted as a form of ] or ].<ref name="zemore-fiske-kim">{{cite book |last1=Zemore |first1=Sarah E. |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |last3=Kim |first3=Hyun-Jeong |editor1-first=Thomas |editor1-last=Eckes |editor2-first=Hanns Martin |editor2-last=Trautner |title=The Developmental Social Psychology of Gender |date=2000 |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |location=Mahwah, New Jersey |isbn=9780585300658 |pages=229–230 |chapter=Gender Stereotypes and the Dynamics of Social Interaction |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=yJ43_5tJGycC&pg=PA229}}</ref><ref name="crocker-major-steele">{{cite book |last1=Crocker |first1=Jennifer |last2=Major |first2=Brenda |last3=Stelle |first3=Claude |editor1-first=Daniel T. |editor1-last=Gilbert |editor2-first=Susan T. |editor2-last=Fiske |editor3-first=Gardner |editor3-last=Lindzey |title=The Handbook of Social Psychology |edition=4th |volume=2 |date=1998 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=9780195213768 |pages=519–521 |chapter=Social Stigma |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=w27pSuHLnLYC&pg=PA519}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Whiteley |first1=Bernard E. |last2=Kite |first2=Mary E. |title=The Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination |url= https://books.google.com/books?id=mXSJEjl4uZYC&pg=PA428 |edition=2nd |date=2010 |publisher=Wadsworth / Cengage Learning |location=Belmont, California |isbn=9780495599647 |pages=428–435}}</ref> | Attributive ambiguity refers to the uncertainty that members of stereotyped groups experience in interpreting the causes of others' behavior toward them. Stereotyped individuals who receive negative ] can ] it either to personal shortcomings, such as lack of ability or poor effort, or the evaluator's stereotypes and prejudice toward their social group. Alternatively, positive feedback can either be attributed to personal merit or discounted as a form of ] or ].<ref name="zemore-fiske-kim">{{cite book |last1=Zemore |first1=Sarah E. |last2=Fiske |first2=Susan T. |last3=Kim |first3=Hyun-Jeong |editor1-first=Thomas |editor1-last=Eckes |editor2-first=Hanns Martin |editor2-last=Trautner |title=The Developmental Social Psychology of Gender |date=2000 |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |location=Mahwah, New Jersey |isbn=9780585300658 |pages=229–230 |chapter=Gender Stereotypes and the Dynamics of Social Interaction |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=yJ43_5tJGycC&pg=PA229}}</ref><ref name="crocker-major-steele">{{cite book |last1=Crocker |first1=Jennifer |last2=Major |first2=Brenda |last3=Stelle |first3=Claude |editor1-first=Daniel T. |editor1-last=Gilbert |editor2-first=Susan T. |editor2-last=Fiske |editor3-first=Gardner |editor3-last=Lindzey |title=The Handbook of Social Psychology |edition=4th |volume=2 |date=1998 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=9780195213768 |pages=519–521 |chapter=Social Stigma |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=w27pSuHLnLYC&pg=PA519}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Whiteley |first1=Bernard E. |last2=Kite |first2=Mary E. |title=The Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination |url= https://books.google.com/books?id=mXSJEjl4uZYC&pg=PA428 |edition=2nd |date=2010 |publisher=Wadsworth / Cengage Learning |location=Belmont, California |isbn=9780495599647 |pages=428–435}}</ref> | ||
] et al. (1991) showed that when black participants were evaluated by a white person who was aware of their race, black subjects mistrusted the feedback, attributing negative feedback to the evaluator's stereotypes and positive feedback to the evaluator's desire to appear unbiased. When the black participants' race was unknown to the evaluator, they were more accepting of the feedback.<ref name="Crocker et al.">{{cite journal |last1=Crocker |first1=Jennifer |last2=Voelkl |first2=Kristin |last3=Testa |first3=Maria |last4=Major |first4=Brenda |date=1991 |title=Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity |journal=] |volume=60 |issue=2 |pages=218–228|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.218}}</ref> | ] et al. (1991) showed that when black participants were evaluated by a white person who was aware of their race, black subjects mistrusted the feedback, attributing negative feedback to the evaluator's stereotypes and positive feedback to the evaluator's desire to appear unbiased. When the black participants' race was unknown to the evaluator, they were more accepting of the feedback.<ref name="Crocker et al.">{{cite journal |last1=Crocker |first1=Jennifer |last2=Voelkl |first2=Kristin |last3=Testa |first3=Maria |last4=Major |first4=Brenda |date=1991 |title=Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity |journal=] |volume=60 |issue=2 |pages=218–228 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.218}}</ref> | ||
Attributional ambiguity has been shown to affect a person's ]. When they receive positive evaluations, stereotyped individuals are uncertain of whether they really deserved their success and, consequently, they find it difficult to take credit for their achievements. In the case of negative feedback, ambiguity has been shown to have a protective effect on self-esteem as it allows people to assign blame to external causes. Some studies, however, have found that this effect only holds when stereotyped individuals can be absolutely certain that their negative outcomes are due to the evaluators's prejudice. If any room for uncertainty remains, stereotyped individuals tend to blame themselves.<ref name="crocker-major-steele" /> | Attributional ambiguity has been shown to affect a person's ]. When they receive positive evaluations, stereotyped individuals are uncertain of whether they really deserved their success and, consequently, they find it difficult to take credit for their achievements. In the case of negative feedback, ambiguity has been shown to have a protective effect on self-esteem as it allows people to assign blame to external causes. Some studies, however, have found that this effect only holds when stereotyped individuals can be absolutely certain that their negative outcomes are due to the evaluators's prejudice. If any room for uncertainty remains, stereotyped individuals tend to blame themselves.<ref name="crocker-major-steele" /> | ||
Line 168: | Line 169: | ||
===Stereotype threat=== | ===Stereotype threat=== | ||
⚫ | ] | ||
{{Main|Stereotype threat}} | {{Main|Stereotype threat}} | ||
⚫ | ] | ||
Stereotype threat occurs when people are aware of a negative stereotype about their social group and experience anxiety or concern that they might confirm the stereotype.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Quinn |first1=Diane M. |last2=Kallen |first2=Rachel W. |last3=Spencer |first3=Steven J. |editor1-first=Miles |editor1-last=Hewstone |editor2-first=Peter |editor2-last=Glick |editor3-first=John F. |editor3-last=Dovidio |editor4-first=Victoria M. |editor4-last=Esses |title=The SAGE Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping and Discrimination |date=2010 |publisher=] |location=Thousand Oaks, California |isbn=9781412934534 |pages=379–394 |chapter=Stereotype Threat}}</ref> Stereotype threat has been shown to undermine performance in a variety of domains.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Inzlicht |first1=Michael |last2=Tullett |first2=Alexa M. |last3=Gutsell |first3=Jennifer N. |editor1-first=Michael |editor1-last=Inzlicht |editor2-last=Schmader |editor2-first=Toni |title=Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application |date=2012 |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=New York |isbn=9780199732449 |page=108 |chapter=Stereotype Threat Spillover: The Short- and Long-term Effects of Coping with Threats to Social Identity |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=o1JBcAv3f14C&pg=PA108}}</ref><ref name=AronsonBook>{{cite book |last1=Aronson |first1=Joshua |last2=Stelle |first2=Claude M. |editor1-first=Andrew J. |editor1-last=Elliot |editor2-first=Carol S. |editor2-last=Dweck |title=Handbook of Competence and Motivation |date=2005 |publisher=] |location=New York |isbn=9781593851231 |pages=436, 443 |chapter=Chapter 24: Stereotypes and the Fragility of Academic Competence, Motivation, and Self-Concept |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=B14TMHRtYBcC&pg=PA436}}</ref> | Stereotype threat occurs when people are aware of a negative stereotype about their social group and experience anxiety or concern that they might confirm the stereotype.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Quinn |first1=Diane M. |last2=Kallen |first2=Rachel W. |last3=Spencer |first3=Steven J. |editor1-first=Miles |editor1-last=Hewstone |editor2-first=Peter |editor2-last=Glick |editor3-first=John F. |editor3-last=Dovidio |editor4-first=Victoria M. |editor4-last=Esses |title=The SAGE Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping and Discrimination |date=2010 |publisher=] |location=Thousand Oaks, California |isbn=9781412934534 |pages=379–394 |chapter=Stereotype Threat}}</ref> Stereotype threat has been shown to undermine performance in a variety of domains.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Inzlicht |first1=Michael |last2=Tullett |first2=Alexa M. |last3=Gutsell |first3=Jennifer N. |editor1-first=Michael |editor1-last=Inzlicht |editor2-last=Schmader |editor2-first=Toni |title=Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application |date=2012 |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=New York |isbn=9780199732449 |page=108 |chapter=Stereotype Threat Spillover: The Short- and Long-term Effects of Coping with Threats to Social Identity |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=o1JBcAv3f14C&pg=PA108}}</ref><ref name=AronsonBook>{{cite book |last1=Aronson |first1=Joshua |last2=Stelle |first2=Claude M. |editor1-first=Andrew J. |editor1-last=Elliot |editor2-first=Carol S. |editor2-last=Dweck |title=Handbook of Competence and Motivation |date=2005 |publisher=] |location=New York |isbn=9781593851231 |pages=436, 443 |chapter=Chapter 24: Stereotypes and the Fragility of Academic Competence, Motivation, and Self-Concept |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=B14TMHRtYBcC&pg=PA436}}</ref> | ||
] and Joshua Aronson conducted the first experiments showing that stereotype threat can depress intellectual performance on ]s. In one study, they found that black college students performed worse than white students on a verbal test when the task was framed as a measure of intelligence. When it was not presented in that manner, the performance gap narrowed. Subsequent experiments showed that framing the test as diagnostic of intellectual ability made black students more aware of negative stereotypes about their group, which in turn impaired their performance.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Steele |first1=Claude M. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |title=Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=69 |issue=5 |pages=797–811 |date=November 1995 |pmid=7473032 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 |s2cid=4665022 |url= http://users.nber.org/~sewp/events/2005.01.14/Bios+Links/Good-rec2-Steele_&_Aronson_95.pdf}}</ref> Stereotype threat effects have been demonstrated for an array of social groups in many different arenas, including not only academics but also sports,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Stone |first1=Jeff |last2=Lynch |first2=Christian I. |last3=Sjomeling |first3=Mike |last4=Darley |first4=John M. |title=Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=77 |issue=6 |date=1999 |pages=1213–1227 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213 |citeseerx=10.1.1.687.5039 |s2cid=14568298}}</ref> ]<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Maass |first1=Anne |last2=D'Ettole |first2=Claudio |last3=Cadinu |first3=Mara |title=Checkmate? The role of gender stereotypes in the ultimate intellectual sport |journal=] |volume=38 |issue=2 |date=2008 |pages=231–245 |doi=10.1002/ejsp.440 |s2cid=144308852 |url= http://clarksvillechessclub.org/pdf%20files/The%20role%20of%20gender%20stereotypes%20in%20chess.pdf |access-date=5 September 2012 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20121120220830/http://clarksvillechessclub.org/pdf%20files/The%20role%20of%20gender%20stereotypes%20in%20chess.pdf |archive-date=20 November 2012}}</ref> and business.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gupta |first1=V. K. |last2=Bhawe |first2=N. M. |title=The Influence of Proactive Personality and Stereotype Threat on Women's Entrepreneurial Intentions |journal=] |volume=13 |issue=4 |date=2007 |pages=73–85 |doi=10.1177/10717919070130040901|s2cid=145318243}}</ref> | ] and Joshua Aronson conducted the first experiments showing that stereotype threat can depress intellectual performance on ]s. In one study, they found that black college students performed worse than white students on a verbal test when the task was framed as a measure of intelligence. When it was not presented in that manner, the performance gap narrowed. Subsequent experiments showed that framing the test as diagnostic of intellectual ability made black students more aware of negative stereotypes about their group, which in turn impaired their performance.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Steele |first1=Claude M. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |title=Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=69 |issue=5 |pages=797–811 |date=November 1995 |pmid=7473032 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 |s2cid=4665022 |url= http://users.nber.org/~sewp/events/2005.01.14/Bios+Links/Good-rec2-Steele_&_Aronson_95.pdf}}</ref> Stereotype threat effects have been demonstrated for an array of social groups in many different arenas, including not only academics but also sports,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Stone |first1=Jeff |last2=Lynch |first2=Christian I. |last3=Sjomeling |first3=Mike |last4=Darley |first4=John M. |title=Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=77 |issue=6 |date=1999 |pages=1213–1227 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213 |citeseerx=10.1.1.687.5039 |s2cid=14568298}}</ref> ]<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Maass |first1=Anne |last2=D'Ettole |first2=Claudio |last3=Cadinu |first3=Mara |title=Checkmate? The role of gender stereotypes in the ultimate intellectual sport |journal=] |volume=38 |issue=2 |date=2008 |pages=231–245 |doi=10.1002/ejsp.440 |s2cid=144308852 |url= http://clarksvillechessclub.org/pdf%20files/The%20role%20of%20gender%20stereotypes%20in%20chess.pdf |access-date=5 September 2012 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20121120220830/http://clarksvillechessclub.org/pdf%20files/The%20role%20of%20gender%20stereotypes%20in%20chess.pdf |archive-date=20 November 2012}}</ref> and business.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gupta |first1=V. K. |last2=Bhawe |first2=N. M. |title=The Influence of Proactive Personality and Stereotype Threat on Women's Entrepreneurial Intentions |journal=] |volume=13 |issue=4 |date=2007 |pages=73–85 |doi=10.1177/10717919070130040901 |s2cid=145318243}}</ref> | ||
Some researchers have suggested that stereotype threat should not be interpreted as a factor in real-life performance gaps, and have raised the possibility of ].<ref name="Ganley2013">{{cite journal | |
Some researchers have suggested that stereotype threat should not be interpreted as a factor in real-life performance gaps, and have raised the possibility of ].<ref name="Ganley2013">{{cite journal |last1=Ganley |first1=C. M. |last2=Mingle |first2=L. A. |last3=Ryan |first3=A. M. |last4=Ryan |first4=K. |last5=Vasilyeva |first5=M. |last6=Perry |first6=M. |title=An examination of stereotype threat effects on girls' mathematics performance |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=49 |issue=10 |pages=1886–1897 |date=October 2013 |pmid=23356523 |doi=10.1037/a0031412 |url= https://dl.dropboxusercontent.com/u/85192141/2013-ganley.pdf |url-status=dead |citeseerx=10.1.1.353.4436 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20140719005546/https://dl.dropboxusercontent.com/u/85192141/2013-ganley.pdf |archive-date=19 July 2014}}</ref><ref name="Stoet2012">{{Cite journal |last1=Stoet |first1=G. |last2=Geary |first2=D. C. |doi=10.1037/a0026617 |title=Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement? |journal=Review of General Psychology |volume=16 |pages=93–102 |date=2012 |s2cid=145724069}} {{Webarchive|url= https://web.archive.org/web/20160112130459/http://volition.gla.ac.uk/~stoet/pdf/Stoet-Geary-RGP2012.pdf |date=12 January 2016}}</ref><ref name="Flore2014">{{cite journal |last1=Flore |first1=Paulette C. |last2=Wicherts |first2=Jelte M. |title=Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis |journal=Journal of School Psychology |volume=53 |issue=1 |date=2014 |pages=25–44 |issn=0022-4405 |doi=10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002 |pmid=25636259 |s2cid=206516995}}</ref> Other critics have focused on correcting what they claim are misconceptions of early studies showing a large effect.<ref name="Sackett2004a">{{cite journal |last1=Sackett |first1=P. R. |last2=Hardison |first2=C. M. |last3=Cullen |first3=M. J. |title=On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American-White differences on cognitive tests |journal=The American Psychologist |volume=59 |issue=1 |pages=7–13 |date=January 2004 |pmid=14736315 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7 |url= http://www2.uni-jena.de/svw/igc/studies/ss03/sackitt_hardison_cullen_2004.pdf |url-status=dead |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20130404150510/http://www2.uni-jena.de/svw/igc/studies/ss03/sackitt_hardison_cullen_2004.pdf |archive-date=4 April 2013}}</ref> However, ] and systematic reviews have shown significant evidence for the effects of stereotype threat, though the phenomenon defies over-simplistic characterization.<ref name=":3">{{cite journal |last1=Pennington |first1=C. R. |last2=Heim |first2=D. |last3=Levy |first3=A. R. |last4=Larkin |first4=D. T. |date=11 January 2016 |title=Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators |journal=PLOS ONE |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=e0146487 |bibcode=2016PLoSO..1146487P |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0146487 |pmc=4713435 |pmid=26752551 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name=":42">{{cite journal |last1=Nguyen |first1=H. H. |last2=Ryan |first2=A. M. |date=November 2008 |title=Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=93 |issue=6 |pages=1314–1334 |doi=10.1037/a0012702 |pmid=19025250|s2cid=36769821 }}</ref><ref name=":52">{{cite journal |last1=Walton |first1=G. M. |last2=Spencer |first2=S. J. |date=September 2009 |title=Latent ability: grades and test scores systematically underestimate the intellectual ability of negatively stereotyped students |journal=Psychological Science |volume=20 |issue=9 |pages=1132–1139 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x |pmid=19656335 |s2cid=25810191 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name=":62">{{Cite journal |last1=Gentile |first1=A. |last2=Boca |first2=S. |last3=Giammusso |first3=I. |date=1 November 2018 |title='You play like a Woman!' Effects of gender stereotype threat on Women's performance in physical and sport activities: A meta-analysis |journal=Psychology of Sport and Exercise |volume=39 |pages=95–103 |doi=10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.07.013 |issn=1469-0292 |s2cid=149490634}}</ref><ref name=":72">{{cite journal |last1=Lamont |first1=R. A. |last2=Swift |first2=H. J. |last3=Abrams |first3=D. |date=March 2015 |title=A review and meta-analysis of age-based stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage |journal=Psychology and Aging |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=180–193 |doi=10.1037/a0038586 |pmc=4360754 |pmid=25621742}}</ref><ref name=":22">{{cite journal |last1=Picho |first1=K. |last2=Rodriguez |first2=A. |last3=Finnie |first3=L. |date=May 2013 |title=Exploring the moderating role of context on the mathematics performance of females under stereotype threat: a meta-analysis |journal=The Journal of Social Psychology |volume=153 |issue=3 |pages=299–333 |doi=10.1080/00224545.2012.737380 |pmid=23724702 |s2cid=45950675}}</ref><ref name=":142">{{cite journal |last1=Liu |first1=S. |last2=Liu |first2=P. |last3=Wang |first3=M. |last4=Zhang |first4=B. |date=June 2021 |title=Effectiveness of stereotype threat interventions: A meta-analytic review |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=106 |issue=6 |pages=921–949 |doi=10.1037/apl0000770 |pmid=32772526 |s2cid=221098319}}</ref><ref name=":132">{{cite journal |last1=VanLandingham |first1=H. |last2=Ellison |first2=R. L. |last3=Laique |first3=A. |last4=Cladek |first4=A. |last5=Khan |first5=H. |last6=Gonzalez |first6=C. |last7=Dunn |first7=M. R. |date=February 2022 |title=A scoping review of stereotype threat for BIPOC: Cognitive effects and intervention strategies for the field of neuropsychology |journal=The Clinical Neuropsychologist |volume=36 |issue=2 |pages=503–522 |doi=10.1080/13854046.2021.1947388 |pmid=34233577 |s2cid=235769090}}</ref> | ||
===Self-fulfilling prophecy=== | ===Self-fulfilling prophecy=== | ||
Line 187: | Line 188: | ||
A 2005 study by J. Thomas Kellow and Brett D. Jones looked at the effects of self-fulfilling prophecy on African American and Caucasian high school freshman students. Both white and black students were informed that their test performance would be predictive of their performance on a statewide, high stakes ]. They were also told that historically, white students had outperformed black students on the test. This knowledge created a self-fulfilling prophecy in both the white and black students, where the white students scored statistically significantly higher than the African American students on the test. The ] of underperforming on standardized tests affected the African American students in this study.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kellow |first=Thomas |date=February 2008 |title=The Effects of Stereotypes on the Achievement Gap: Reexamining the Academic Performance of African American High School Students |journal=Journal of Black Psychology |volume=34 |pages=94–120 |doi=10.1177/0095798407310537 |citeseerx=10.1.1.553.9188 |s2cid=145490359}}</ref> | A 2005 study by J. Thomas Kellow and Brett D. Jones looked at the effects of self-fulfilling prophecy on African American and Caucasian high school freshman students. Both white and black students were informed that their test performance would be predictive of their performance on a statewide, high stakes ]. They were also told that historically, white students had outperformed black students on the test. This knowledge created a self-fulfilling prophecy in both the white and black students, where the white students scored statistically significantly higher than the African American students on the test. The ] of underperforming on standardized tests affected the African American students in this study.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kellow |first=Thomas |date=February 2008 |title=The Effects of Stereotypes on the Achievement Gap: Reexamining the Academic Performance of African American High School Students |journal=Journal of Black Psychology |volume=34 |pages=94–120 |doi=10.1177/0095798407310537 |citeseerx=10.1.1.553.9188 |s2cid=145490359}}</ref> | ||
In ], there is a popular stereotype which represents members of the profession as being humorless, introspective ].<ref>Friedman |
In ], there is a popular stereotype which represents members of the profession as being humorless, introspective ].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Friedman |first1=A. L. |last2=Lyne |first2=S. R. |date=2001 |title=The beancounter stereotype: Towards a general model of stereotype generation |journal=Critical Perspectives on Accounting |volume=12 |issue=4 |pages=423–451|doi=10.1006/cpac.2000.0451 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Jeacle |first1=I. |last2=Miley |first2=F. |last3=Read |first3=A. |date=2012 |title=Jokes in popular culture: The characterisation of the accountant |journal=Accounting, Auditing & Accountability Journal}}</ref> | ||
===Discrimination and prejudice === | ===Discrimination and prejudice === | ||
Because stereotypes simplify and justify social reality, they have potentially powerful effects on how people perceive and treat one another.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Banaji |first1=Mahzarin R. |author-link1=Mahzarin Banaji |editor1-first=Neil |editor1-last=Smelser |editor2-first=Paul |editor2-last=Baltes |title=International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences |date=2002 |publisher=] |location=New York |isbn=9780080430768 |doi=10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01754-X |pages=15100–15104 |chapter=The Social Psychology of Stereotypes}}</ref> As a result, stereotypes can lead to |
Because stereotypes simplify and justify social reality, they have potentially powerful effects on how people perceive and treat one another.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Banaji |first1=Mahzarin R. |author-link1=Mahzarin Banaji |editor1-first=Neil |editor1-last=Smelser |editor2-first=Paul |editor2-last=Baltes |title=International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences |date=2002 |publisher=] |location=New York |isbn=9780080430768 |doi=10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01754-X |pages=15100–15104 |chapter=The Social Psychology of Stereotypes}}</ref> As a result, stereotypes can lead to discrimination in labor markets and other domains.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Fiske |first1=Susan T. |last2=Lee |first2=Tiane L. |editor1-first=Arthur P |editor1-last=Brief |title=Diversity at Work |date=2008 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=New York |isbn=9780521860307 |pages=13–52 |chapter=Stereotypes and prejudice create workplace discrimination |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=8edJmBsyRHwC&pg=PA13}}</ref> For example, Tilcsik (2011) has found that employers who seek job applicants with stereotypically male ] traits are particularly likely to engage in discrimination against gay men, suggesting that discrimination on the basis of ] is partly rooted in specific stereotypes and that these stereotypes loom large in many labor markets.<ref name=Tilcsik/> Agerström and Rooth (2011) showed that automatic ] stereotypes captured by the ] can predict real hiring discrimination against the obese.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Agerström |first1=Jens |last2=Rooth |first2=Dan-Olof |date=2011 |title=The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=96 |issue=4 |pages=790–805 |doi=10.1037/a0021594 |pmid=21280934}}</ref> Similarly, experiments suggest that gender stereotypes play an important role in judgments that affect ] decisions.<ref name=":22"/><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Rudman |first1=Laurie A. |last2=Glick |first2=Peter |date=2001 |title=Prescriptive Gender Stereotypes and Backlash toward Agentic Women |journal=] |volume=57 |issue=4 |pages=743–762 |doi=10.1111/0022-4537.00239 |hdl=2027.42/146421 |s2cid=54219902 |url= https://wesfiles.wesleyan.edu/courses/PSYC-309-clwilkins/Week3/Rudman.Glick.2001.pdf |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20121106121608/https://wesfiles.wesleyan.edu/courses/PSYC-309-clwilkins/Week3/Rudman.Glick.2001.pdf |archive-date=6 November 2012 |hdl-access=free}}</ref> | ||
Stereotypes can cause ] prejudice. For example, scientists and activists have warned that the use of the stereotype "Nigerian Prince" for referring to ]mers is racist, i.e. "reducing ] to a nation of scammers and fraudulent princes, as some people still do online, is a stereotype that needs to be called out".<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Yékú |first=James |date=9 September 2020 |title=Anti-Afropolitan ethics and the performative politics of online scambaiting |url= https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02533952.2020.1813943 |journal=Social Dynamics |volume=46 |issue=2 |pages=240–258 |doi=10.1080/02533952.2020.1813943 |s2cid=222232833 |issn=0253-3952}}</ref> | Stereotypes can cause ] prejudice. For example, scientists and activists have warned that the use of the stereotype "Nigerian Prince" for referring to ]mers is racist, i.e. "reducing ] to a nation of scammers and fraudulent princes, as some people still do online, is a stereotype that needs to be called out".<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Yékú |first=James |date=9 September 2020 |title=Anti-Afropolitan ethics and the performative politics of online scambaiting |url= https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02533952.2020.1813943 |journal=Social Dynamics |volume=46 |issue=2 |pages=240–258 |doi=10.1080/02533952.2020.1813943 |s2cid=222232833 |issn=0253-3952}}</ref> | ||
Line 198: | Line 199: | ||
Stereotypes can affect self-evaluations and lead to self-stereotyping.<ref name="Cox et al. (2012)" /><ref>{{cite book |last1=Sinclair |first1=Stacey |last2=Huntsinger |first2=Jeff |editor1-first=Shana |editor1-last=Levin |editor2-first=Colette |editor2-last=Van Laar |title=Stigma and Group Inequality: Social Psychological Perspectives |series=Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology |date=2006 |publisher=] |location=Mahwah, New Jersey |isbn=9780805844153 |page=239 |chapter=The Interpersonal Basis of Self-Stereotyping |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=7WtgXfECza8C&pg=PA239}}</ref> For instance, Correll (2001, 2004) found that specific stereotypes (e.g., the stereotype that women have lower mathematical ability) affect women's and men's evaluations of their abilities (e.g., in math and science), such that men assess their own task ability higher than women performing at the same level.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Correll |first1=Shelley J. |date=2001 |title=Gender and the career choice process: The role of biased self-assessments |journal=American Journal of Sociology |volume=106 |issue=6 |pages=1691–1730 |doi=10.1086/321299 |s2cid=142863258 |url= http://www.chaire-crsng-inal.fsg.ulaval.ca/fileadmin/docs/documents/Article/Gender_and_career_choice_process_2001.pdf |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20110912043942/http://www.chaire-crsng-inal.fsg.ulaval.ca/fileadmin/docs/documents/Article/Gender_and_career_choice_process_2001.pdf |archive-date=12 September 2011}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Correll |first1=Shelley J. |date=2004 |title=Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations |journal=] |volume=69 |issue=1 |pages=93–113 |doi=10.1177/000312240406900106 |url= http://people.uncw.edu/maumem/soc500/Correll2004.pdf |citeseerx=10.1.1.520.8370 |s2cid=8735336}}</ref> Similarly, a study by Sinclair et al. (2006) has shown that Asian American women rated their math ability more favorably when their ethnicity and the relevant stereotype that Asian Americans excel in math was made salient. In contrast, they rated their math ability less favorably when their gender and the corresponding stereotype of women's inferior math skills was made salient. Sinclair et al. found, however, that the effect of stereotypes on self-evaluations is ] by the degree to which close people in someone's life endorse these stereotypes. People's self-stereotyping can increase or decrease depending on whether close others view them in stereotype-consistent or inconsistent manner.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Sinclair |first1=Stacey |last2=Hardin |first2=Curtis D. |last3=Lowery |first3=Brian S. |title=Self-Stereotyping in the Context of Multiple Social Identities |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=2006 |volume=90 |issue=4 |pages=529–542 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.529 |url= http://psych.princeton.edu/psychology/research/sinclair/pubs/self%20stereo%20and%20multiple%20identities.PDF |pmid=16649853 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20140827125537/http://psych.princeton.edu/psychology/research/sinclair/pubs/self%20stereo%20and%20multiple%20identities.PDF |archive-date=27 August 2014}}</ref> | Stereotypes can affect self-evaluations and lead to self-stereotyping.<ref name="Cox et al. (2012)" /><ref>{{cite book |last1=Sinclair |first1=Stacey |last2=Huntsinger |first2=Jeff |editor1-first=Shana |editor1-last=Levin |editor2-first=Colette |editor2-last=Van Laar |title=Stigma and Group Inequality: Social Psychological Perspectives |series=Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology |date=2006 |publisher=] |location=Mahwah, New Jersey |isbn=9780805844153 |page=239 |chapter=The Interpersonal Basis of Self-Stereotyping |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=7WtgXfECza8C&pg=PA239}}</ref> For instance, Correll (2001, 2004) found that specific stereotypes (e.g., the stereotype that women have lower mathematical ability) affect women's and men's evaluations of their abilities (e.g., in math and science), such that men assess their own task ability higher than women performing at the same level.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Correll |first1=Shelley J. |date=2001 |title=Gender and the career choice process: The role of biased self-assessments |journal=American Journal of Sociology |volume=106 |issue=6 |pages=1691–1730 |doi=10.1086/321299 |s2cid=142863258 |url= http://www.chaire-crsng-inal.fsg.ulaval.ca/fileadmin/docs/documents/Article/Gender_and_career_choice_process_2001.pdf |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20110912043942/http://www.chaire-crsng-inal.fsg.ulaval.ca/fileadmin/docs/documents/Article/Gender_and_career_choice_process_2001.pdf |archive-date=12 September 2011}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Correll |first1=Shelley J. |date=2004 |title=Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations |journal=] |volume=69 |issue=1 |pages=93–113 |doi=10.1177/000312240406900106 |url= http://people.uncw.edu/maumem/soc500/Correll2004.pdf |citeseerx=10.1.1.520.8370 |s2cid=8735336}}</ref> Similarly, a study by Sinclair et al. (2006) has shown that Asian American women rated their math ability more favorably when their ethnicity and the relevant stereotype that Asian Americans excel in math was made salient. In contrast, they rated their math ability less favorably when their gender and the corresponding stereotype of women's inferior math skills was made salient. Sinclair et al. found, however, that the effect of stereotypes on self-evaluations is ] by the degree to which close people in someone's life endorse these stereotypes. People's self-stereotyping can increase or decrease depending on whether close others view them in stereotype-consistent or inconsistent manner.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Sinclair |first1=Stacey |last2=Hardin |first2=Curtis D. |last3=Lowery |first3=Brian S. |title=Self-Stereotyping in the Context of Multiple Social Identities |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=2006 |volume=90 |issue=4 |pages=529–542 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.529 |url= http://psych.princeton.edu/psychology/research/sinclair/pubs/self%20stereo%20and%20multiple%20identities.PDF |pmid=16649853 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20140827125537/http://psych.princeton.edu/psychology/research/sinclair/pubs/self%20stereo%20and%20multiple%20identities.PDF |archive-date=27 August 2014}}</ref> | ||
Stereotyping can also play a central role in depression, when people have negative self-stereotypes about themselves |
Stereotyping can also play a central role in depression, when people have negative self-stereotypes about themselves. According to ], ], ], and Hollon (2012).,<ref name="Cox et al. (2012)" /> stereotyping can also play a central role in depression, which is characterized by negative ]s. Stereotypes and self-schemas are the same type of cognitive structure, therefore, they suggest that an integrated perspective of prejudice and depression provides useful insight on how stereotypes are acquired. Negative stereotypes are set in motion within the Source, who conveys the prejudice towards the Target, which in turn will lead the Target to suffer from depression. Members of stigmatized groups may internalize the negative evaluation of their group and develop depression. People may also show prejudice internalization through self-stereotyping because of negative childhood experiences such as verbal and physical abuse. This depression that is caused by prejudice (i.e., "deprejudice") can be related to group membership (e.g., Me–Gay–Bad) or not (e.g., Me–Bad). If someone holds prejudicial beliefs about a stigmatized group and then becomes a member of that group, they may internalize their prejudice and develop depression. People may also show prejudice internalization through self-stereotyping because of negative childhood experiences such as verbal and physical abuse.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Sachs |first1=Nicole M. |last2=Veysey |first2=Bonita M. |last3=Rivera |first3=Luis M. |date=14 November 2017 |title=Implicit Social Cognitive Processes Underlying Victim Self and Identity: Evidence With College-Aged Adults |journal=Journal of Interpersonal Violence |volume=36 |issue=3–4 |pages=1256–1282 |doi=10.1177/0886260517741625 |pmid=29294984 |s2cid=206565963}}</ref> | ||
===Substitute for observations === | ===Substitute for observations === | ||
Line 206: | Line 207: | ||
==Role in art and culture== | ==Role in art and culture== | ||
] lighting a powder keg and swinging a bottle. Published in '']'', 1871.]] | ] lighting a powder keg and swinging a bottle. Published in '']'', 1871.]] | ||
Stereotypes are common in various cultural |
Stereotypes are common in various cultural media, where they take the form of dramatic ]s. The instantly recognizable nature of stereotypes mean that they are effective in advertising and situation comedy.<ref>{{Cite web |url= http://tedb.byu.edu/?page_id=35 |title=Lesson 2 – Stock Characters {{!}} BYU Theatre Education Database|website=tedb.byu.edu|access-date=30 March 2018}}</ref> Alexander Fedorov (2015) proposed a concept of media stereotypes analysis. This concept refers to identification and analysis of stereotypical images of people, ideas, events, stories, themes, etc. in media context.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Fedorov |first=Alexander |title=Media Stereotypes Analysis in the Classroom at the Student Audience |journal=European Journal of Contemporary Education |date=2015 |volume=12 |issue=2 |pages=158–162 |doi=10.13187/ejced.2015.12.158 |doi-access=free}}</ref> | ||
The characters that do appear in movies greatly affect how people worldwide perceive gender relations, race, and cultural communities. Because approximately 85% of worldwide ticket sales are directed toward Hollywood movies, the American movie industry has been greatly responsible for portraying characters of different cultures and diversity to fit into stereotypical categories.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Lee |first=Kevin |date=January 2008 |title='The Little State Department': Hollywood and the MPAA's Influence on U.S. Trade Relations |url= https://scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1671&context=njilb |journal=Northwestern Journal of International Law & Business |volume=28 |issue=2}}</ref> This has led to the spread and persistence of gender, racial, ethnic, and cultural stereotypes seen in the movies.<sup>]]</sup> | The characters that do appear in movies greatly affect how people worldwide perceive gender relations, race, and cultural communities. Because approximately 85% of worldwide ticket sales are directed toward Hollywood movies, the American movie industry has been greatly responsible for portraying characters of different cultures and diversity to fit into stereotypical categories.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Lee |first=Kevin |date=January 2008 |title='The Little State Department': Hollywood and the MPAA's Influence on U.S. Trade Relations |url= https://scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1671&context=njilb |journal=Northwestern Journal of International Law & Business |volume=28 |issue=2}}</ref> This has led to the spread and persistence of gender, racial, ethnic, and cultural stereotypes seen in the movies.<sup>]]</sup> | ||
For example, ] are usually portrayed as ruthless agents, brutal mobsters and villains in ] movies.<ref>{{cite news |title=Will the cliche of the 'Russian baddie' ever leave our screens? |url= https://www.theguardian.com/tv-and-radio/2017/jul/10/will-cliche-russian-baddie-ever-leave-screens-james-norton-mcmafia |work=The Guardian |date=10 July 2017}}</ref><ref>"." Fox News. 14 October 2014</ref><ref>{{cite news |title=5 Hollywood Villains That Prove Russian Stereotypes Are Hard to Kill |url= https://www.themoscowtimes.com/2015/08/09/5-hollywood-villains-that-prove-russian-stereotypes-are-hard-to-kill-a48849 |work=The Moscow Times |date=9 August 2015}}</ref> According to Russian American professor ], "You can't even turn the TV on and go to the movies without reference to Russians as horrible."<ref>{{cite news |title=Hollywood stereotypes: Why are Russians the bad guys? |url= https://www.bbc.com/culture/article/20141106-why-are-russians-always-bad-guys |work=BBC News |date=5 November 2014}}</ref> The portrayals of |
For example, ] are usually portrayed as ruthless agents, brutal mobsters and villains in ] movies.<ref>{{cite news |title=Will the cliche of the 'Russian baddie' ever leave our screens? |url= https://www.theguardian.com/tv-and-radio/2017/jul/10/will-cliche-russian-baddie-ever-leave-screens-james-norton-mcmafia |work=The Guardian |date=10 July 2017}}</ref><ref>"." Fox News. 14 October 2014</ref><ref>{{cite news |title=5 Hollywood Villains That Prove Russian Stereotypes Are Hard to Kill |url= https://www.themoscowtimes.com/2015/08/09/5-hollywood-villains-that-prove-russian-stereotypes-are-hard-to-kill-a48849 |work=The Moscow Times |date=9 August 2015}}</ref> According to Russian American professor ], "You can't even turn the TV on and go to the movies without reference to Russians as horrible."<ref>{{cite news |title=Hollywood stereotypes: Why are Russians the bad guys? |url= https://www.bbc.com/culture/article/20141106-why-are-russians-always-bad-guys |work=BBC News |date=5 November 2014}}</ref> The portrayals of Latin Americans in film and print media are restricted to a narrow set of characters. Latin Americans are largely depicted as sexualized figures such as the Latino ] or the Latina vixen, ] members, (illegal) immigrants, or entertainers. By comparison, they are rarely portrayed as working professionals, business leaders or politicians.<ref name=":22"/> | ||
In ], there are several Latin American stereotypes that have historically been used. Some examples are El Bandido, the Halfbreed Harlot, The Male Buffoon, The Female Clown, The Latin Lover, The Dark Lady, The Wise Old Man, and The Poor Peon. Many Hispanic characters in Hollywood films consists of one or more of these basic stereotypes, but it has been rare to view Latin American actors representing characters outside of this stereotypical criteria.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Berg |first=Charles |date=Summer 1990 |title=Stereotyping in films in general and of the Hispanic in Particular |journal=The Howard Journal of Communications |volume=2 |issue=3 |pages=294–296 |doi=10.1080/10646179009359721}}</ref> | In ], there are several Latin American stereotypes that have historically been used. Some examples are El Bandido, the Halfbreed Harlot, The Male Buffoon, The Female Clown, The Latin Lover, The Dark Lady, The Wise Old Man, and The Poor Peon. Many Hispanic characters in Hollywood films consists of one or more of these basic stereotypes, but it has been rare to view Latin American actors representing characters outside of this stereotypical criteria.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Berg |first=Charles |date=Summer 1990 |title=Stereotyping in films in general and of the Hispanic in Particular |journal=The Howard Journal of Communications |volume=2 |issue=3 |pages=294–296 |doi=10.1080/10646179009359721}}</ref> | ||
Media stereotypes of women first emerged in the early 20th century. Various stereotypic depictions or "types" of women appeared in magazines, including Victorian ideals of femininity, the ], the ], the ], and the ].<sup>]]</sup><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Coltrane |first1=Scott |last2=Adams |first2=Michele |title=Work–Family Imagery and Gender Stereotypes:Television and the Reproduction of Difference |journal=Journal of Vocational Behavior |volume=50}}</ref> | Media stereotypes of women first emerged in the early 20th century. Various stereotypic depictions or "types" of women appeared in magazines, including Victorian ideals of femininity, the ], the ], the ], and the ].<sup>]]</sup><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Coltrane |first1=Scott |last2=Adams |first2=Michele |date=April 1997 |title=Work–Family Imagery and Gender Stereotypes: Television and the Reproduction of Difference |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0001879196915754 |url-access=subscription |journal=Journal of Vocational Behavior |volume=50 |issue=2 |pages=323–347 |doi=10.1006/jvbe.1996.1575}}</ref> | ||
Stereotypes are also common in video games, with women being portrayed as stereotypes such as the "]" or as ]s (see ]).<ref>{{Cite |
Stereotypes are also common in video games, with women being portrayed as stereotypes such as the "]" or as ]s (see ]).<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Mou |first1=Yi |last2=Peng |first2=Wei |date=January 2008 |chapter=Gender and Racial Stereotypes in Popular Video Games |chapter-url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/314671823_Gender_and_Racial_Stereotypes_in_Popular_Video_Games |title=Handbook of Research on Effective Electronic Gaming in Education |location=Hershey, Pa. |publisher=Information Science Reference |doi=10.4018/9781599048086.ch053 |isbn=9781599048086 |oclc=755871149}} Book: {{Doi|10.4018/978-1-59904-808-6}}.</ref> Studies show that minorities are portrayed most often in stereotypical roles such as athletes and gangsters<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Burgess |first1=Melinda |last2=Dill |first2=Karen |date=15 September 2011 |title=Playing With Prejudice: The Prevalence and Consequences of Racial Stereotypes in Video Games |journal=Media Psychology |volume=14 |issue=3 |pages=289–311 |doi=10.1080/15213269.2011.596467 |s2cid=1416833}}</ref> {{crossreference|(see ])}}. | ||
In |
In literature and art, stereotypes are ]d or predictable characters or situations. Throughout history, storytellers have drawn from stereotypical characters and situations to immediately connect the audience with new tales.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Auracher |first1=Jan |last2=Hirose |first2=Akiko |date=2017 |title=The Influence of Reader's Stereotypes on the Assessment of Fictional Characters |jstor=10.5325/complitstudies.54.4.0795 |journal=Comparative Literature Studies |volume=54 |issue=4 |pages=795–823 |doi=10.5325/complitstudies.54.4.0795 |s2cid=148778808}}</ref> | ||
===Role in sports === | ===Role in sports === | ||
Line 232: | Line 233: | ||
==See also== | ==See also== | ||
⚫ | {{Portal|Psychology|Society}} | ||
{{Col-begin}} | |||
{{Div col|colwidth=20em}} | |||
{{Col-break}} | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
Line 254: | Line 255: | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
Line 262: | Line 264: | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
⚫ | {{Div col end}} | ||
;Gender | ;Gender | ||
Line 267: | Line 270: | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
{{Col-break}} | |||
⚫ | {{Portal|Psychology|Society | ||
}} | |||
===Examples of stereotypes=== | ===Examples of stereotypes=== | ||
<!---♦♦♦ Please keep the list in alphabetical order ♦♦♦---> | <!---♦♦♦ Please keep the list in alphabetical order ♦♦♦---> | ||
;Cultural and ethnic | ; Cultural and ethnic | ||
{{Div col|colwidth=20em}} | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
Line 294: | Line 294: | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
{{Div col end}} | |||
;Sexuality related | ; Sexuality related | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
;Other | ; Other | ||
* ]s | * ]s | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
⚫ | {{col |
||
==References== | ==References== | ||
{{ |
{{Reflist}} | ||
==Further reading== | ==Further reading== | ||
{{Scholia|topic}} | {{Scholia|topic}} | ||
⚫ | * {{Cite web |url= http://beta.in-mind.org/node/126 |title=Are Stereotypes True? |website=Beta.In-Mind.org |access-date=25 July 2018 |archive-date=24 July 2010 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20100724145638/http://beta.in-mind.org/node/126 |url-status=dead}} | ||
⚫ | * {{ |
||
* {{ |
* {{Cite book |last1=Crawford |first1=M. |last2=Unger |first2=R. |date=2004 |title=Women and Gender: A Feminist Psychology |location=New York |publisher=McGraw Hill |pages=45–49}} | ||
* {{Cite book |last1=Ewen |first1=Stuart |last2=Ewen |first2=Elizabeth |title=Typecasting: On the Arts and Sciences of Human Inequality |location=New York |publisher=Seven Stories Press |date=2006}} | |||
* A Major Resource: Constantly updated and archived | |||
⚫ | * {{Cite journal |last1=Hilton |first1=James L. |last2=von Hippel |first2=William |date=1996 |title=Stereotypes |journal=Annual Review of Psychology |volume=47 |issue=1 |pages=237–271 |doi=10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.237 |pmid=15012482}} | ||
* {{Cite journal |last=Regenberg |first=Nina |date=2007 |title=Are Blonds Really Dumb? |journal=The Inquisitive Mind |issue=3 |url= http://beta.in-mind.org/issue-3/are-blonds-really-dumb |access-date=4 May 2010 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20110726174639/http://beta.in-mind.org/issue-3/are-blonds-really-dumb |archive-date=26 July 2011}} | * {{Cite journal |last=Regenberg |first=Nina |date=2007 |title=Are Blonds Really Dumb? |journal=The Inquisitive Mind |issue=3 |url= http://beta.in-mind.org/issue-3/are-blonds-really-dumb |access-date=4 May 2010 |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20110726174639/http://beta.in-mind.org/issue-3/are-blonds-really-dumb |archive-date=26 July 2011}} | ||
* | |||
⚫ | * {{ |
||
⚫ | * {{Cite journal |last1=Spitzer |first1=B. L. |last2=Henderson |first2=K. |last3=Zavian |first3=M. T. |date=1999 |title=Gender Differences in Population Versus Media Body Sizes: A Comparison over Four Decades |doi=10.1023/a:1018836029738 |journal=Sex Roles |volume=40 |issue=7/8 |pages=545–565 |s2cid=55674520}} | ||
* {{ |
* {{Cite book |last=Turner |first=Chris |author-link=Chris Turner (author) |date=2004 |title=Planet Simpson: How a Cartoon Masterpiece Documented an Era and Defined a Generation |title-link=Planet Simpson |others=Foreword by ] |location=Toronto |publisher=] |oclc=55682258 |isbn=9780679313182}} | ||
* {{cite book |last1=Crawford |first1=M. |last2=Unger |first2=R. |date=2004 |title=Women and Gender: A Feminist Psychology |publisher=McGraw Hill |location=New York |pages=45–49}} | |||
⚫ | * {{ |
||
==External links== | ==External links== | ||
{{Commons category}} | |||
{{Wiktionary}} | |||
* with social psychologists ] and Mike North about the stereotyping of older people | * with social psychologists ] and Mike North about the stereotyping of older people | ||
* |
* —Lecture by ] sociologist Shelley Correll on 21 October 2010 | ||
* Stereotyping | * —Social Psychology Network | ||
* |
* —Media Smarts, Canada's Centre for Digital and Media Literacy | ||
* |
* at AHealthCareer | ||
* |
* —] by ] | ||
{{SocialPsychology}} | {{SocialPsychology}} | ||
Line 334: | Line 333: | ||
{{Stock characters}} | {{Stock characters}} | ||
{{Discrimination}} | {{Discrimination}} | ||
{{Authority control}} | {{Authority control}} | ||
] | ] | ||
⚫ | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
⚫ | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
⚫ | ] | ||
⚫ | ] |
Latest revision as of 17:36, 15 December 2024
Generalized but fixed and oversimplified image or idea of a particular type of person or thing For other uses, see Stereotype (disambiguation). Not to be confused with Stereotypy.
In social psychology, a stereotype is a generalized belief about a particular category of people. It is an expectation that people might have about every person of a particular group. The type of expectation can vary; it can be, for example, an expectation about the group's personality, preferences, appearance or ability. Stereotypes are often overgeneralized, inaccurate, and resistant to new information. A stereotype does not necessarily need to be a negative assumption. They may be positive, neutral, or negative.
Explicit stereotypes
An explicit stereotype refers to stereotypes that one is aware that one holds, and is aware that one is using to judge people. If person A is making judgments about a particular person B from group G, and person A has an explicit stereotype for group G, their decision bias can be partially mitigated using conscious control; however, attempts to offset bias due to conscious awareness of a stereotype often fail at being truly impartial, due to either underestimating or overestimating the amount of bias being created by the stereotype.
Implicit stereotypes
Main article: Implicit stereotypeImplicit stereotypes are those that lay on individuals' subconsciousness, that they have no control or awareness of. "Implicit stereotypes are built based on two concepts, associative networks in semantic (knowledge) memory and automatic activation". Implicit stereotypes are automatic and involuntary associations that people make between a social group and a domain or attribute. For example, one can have beliefs that women and men are equally capable of becoming successful electricians but at the same time many can associate electricians more with men than women.
In social psychology, a stereotype is any thought widely adopted about specific types of individuals or certain ways of behaving intended to represent the entire group of those individuals or behaviors as a whole. These thoughts or beliefs may or may not accurately reflect reality. Within psychology and across other disciplines, different conceptualizations and theories of stereotyping exist, at times sharing commonalities, as well as containing contradictory elements. Even in the social sciences and some sub-disciplines of psychology, stereotypes are occasionally reproduced and can be identified in certain theories, for example, in assumptions about other cultures.
Etymology
The term stereotype comes from the French adjective stéréotype and derives from the Greek words στερεός (stereos), 'firm, solid' and τύπος (typos), 'impression', hence 'solid impression on one or more ideas/theories'.
The term was first used in the printing trade in 1798 by Firmin Didot, to describe a printing plate that duplicated any typography. The duplicate printing plate, or the stereotype, is used for printing instead of the original.
Outside of printing, the first reference to stereotype in English was in 1850, as a noun that meant 'image perpetuated without change'. However, it was not until 1922 that stereotype was first used in the modern psychological sense by American journalist Walter Lippmann in his work Public Opinion.
Relationship with other types of intergroup attitudes
Stereotypes, prejudice, racism, and discrimination are understood as related but different concepts. Stereotypes are regarded as the most cognitive component and often occurs without conscious awareness, whereas prejudice is the affective component of stereotyping and discrimination is one of the behavioral components of prejudicial reactions. In this tripartite view of intergroup attitudes, stereotypes reflect expectations and beliefs about the members of groups perceived as different from one's own, prejudice represents the emotional response, and discrimination refers to actions.
Although related, the three concepts can exist independently of each other. According to Daniel Katz and Kenneth Braly, stereotyping leads to racial prejudice when people emotionally react to the name of a group, ascribe characteristics to members of that group, and then evaluate those characteristics.
Possible prejudicial effects of stereotypes are:
- Justification of ill-founded prejudices or ignorance
- Unwillingness to rethink one's attitudes and behavior
- Preventing some people of stereotyped groups from entering or succeeding in activities or fields
Content
Stereotype content refers to the attributes that people think characterize a group. Studies of stereotype content examine what people think of others, rather than the reasons and mechanisms involved in stereotyping.
Early theories of stereotype content proposed by social psychologists such as Gordon Allport assumed that stereotypes of outgroups reflected uniform antipathy. For instance, Katz and Braly argued in their classic 1933 study that ethnic stereotypes were uniformly negative.
By contrast, a newer model of stereotype content theorizes that stereotypes are frequently ambivalent and vary along two dimensions: warmth and competence. Warmth and competence are respectively predicted by lack of competition and status. Groups that do not compete with the in-group for the same resources (e.g., college space) are perceived as warm, whereas high-status (e.g., economically or educationally successful) groups are considered competent. The groups within each of the four combinations of high and low levels of warmth and competence elicit distinct emotions. The model explains the phenomenon that some out-groups are admired but disliked, whereas others are liked but disrespected. This model was empirically tested on a variety of national and international samples and was found to reliably predict stereotype content.
An even more recent model of stereotype content called the agency–beliefs–communion (ABC) model suggested that methods to study warmth and competence in the stereotype content model (SCM) were missing a crucial element, that being, stereotypes of social groups are often spontaneously generated. Experiments on the SCM usually ask participants to rate traits according to warmth and competence but this does not allow participants to use any other stereotype dimensions. The ABC model, proposed by Koch and colleagues in 2016 is an estimate of how people spontaneously stereotype U.S social groups of people using traits. Koch et al. conducted several studies asking participants to list groups and sort them according to their similarity. Using statistical techniques, they revealed three dimensions that explained the similarity ratings. These three dimensions were agency (A), beliefs (B), and communion (C). Agency is associated with reaching goals, standing out and socio-economic status and is related to competence in the SCM, with some examples of traits including poor and wealthy, powerful and powerless, low status and high status. Beliefs is associated with views on the world, morals and conservative-progressive beliefs with some examples of traits including traditional and modern, religious and science-oriented or conventional and alternative. Finally, communion is associated with connecting with others and fitting in and is similar to warmth from the SCM, with some examples of traits including trustworthy and untrustworthy, cold and warm and repellent and likeable. According to research using this model, there is a curvilinear relationship between agency and communion. For example, if a group is high or low in the agency dimension then they may be seen as un-communal, whereas groups that are average in agency are seen as more communal. This model has many implications in predicting behaviour towards stereotyped groups. For example, Koch and colleagues recently proposed that perceived similarity in agency and beliefs increases inter-group cooperation.
Functions
Early studies suggested that stereotypes were only used by rigid, repressed, and authoritarian people. This idea has been refuted by contemporary studies that suggest the ubiquity of stereotypes and it was suggested to regard stereotypes as collective group beliefs, meaning that people who belong to the same social group share the same set of stereotypes. Modern research asserts that full understanding of stereotypes requires considering them from two complementary perspectives: as shared within a particular culture/subculture and as formed in the mind of an individual person.
Relationship between cognitive and social functions
Stereotyping can serve cognitive functions on an interpersonal level, and social functions on an intergroup level. For stereotyping to function on an intergroup level (see social identity approaches: social identity theory and self-categorization theory), an individual must see themselves as part of a group and being part of that group must also be salient for the individual.
Craig McGarty, Russell Spears, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2002) argued that the cognitive functions of stereotyping are best understood in relation to its social functions, and vice versa.
Cognitive functions
Stereotypes can help make sense of the world. They are a form of categorization that helps to simplify and systematize information. Thus, information is more easily identified, recalled, predicted, and reacted to. Stereotypes are categories of objects or people. Between stereotypes, objects or people are as different from each other as possible. Within stereotypes, objects or people are as similar to each other as possible.
Gordon Allport has suggested possible answers to why people find it easier to understand categorized information. First, people can consult a category to identify response patterns. Second, categorized information is more specific than non-categorized information, as categorization accentuates properties that are shared by all members of a group. Third, people can readily describe objects in a category because objects in the same category have distinct characteristics. Finally, people can take for granted the characteristics of a particular category because the category itself may be an arbitrary grouping.
A complementary perspective theorizes how stereotypes function as time- and energy-savers that allow people to act more efficiently. Yet another perspective suggests that stereotypes are people's biased perceptions of their social contexts. In this view, people use stereotypes as shortcuts to make sense of their social contexts, and this makes a person's task of understanding his or her world less cognitively demanding.
Social functions
Social categorization
In the following situations, the overarching purpose of stereotyping is for people to put their collective self (their in-group membership) in a positive light:
- when stereotypes are used for explaining social events
- when stereotypes are used for justifying activities of one's own group (ingroup) to another group (outgroup)
- when stereotypes are used for differentiating the ingroup as positively distinct from outgroups
Explanation purposes
As mentioned previously, stereotypes can be used to explain social events. Henri Tajfel described his observations of how some people found that the antisemitic fabricated contents of The Protocols of the Elders of Zion only made sense if Jews have certain characteristics. Therefore, according to Tajfel, Jews were stereotyped as being evil and yearning for world domination to match the antisemitic "facts" as presented in The Protocols of the Elders of Zion.
Justification purposes
People create stereotypes of an outgroup to justify the actions that their in-group has committed (or plans to commit) towards that outgroup. For example, according to Tajfel, Europeans stereotyped African, Indian, and Chinese people as being incapable of achieving financial advances without European help. This stereotype was used to justify European colonialism in Africa, India, and China.
Intergroup differentiation
An assumption is that people want their ingroup to have a positive image relative to outgroups, and so people want to differentiate their ingroup from relevant outgroups in a desirable way. If an outgroup does not affect the ingroup's image, then from an image preservation point of view, there is no point for the ingroup to be positively distinct from that outgroup.
People can actively create certain images for relevant outgroups by stereotyping. People do so when they see that their ingroup is no longer as clearly and/or as positively differentiated from relevant outgroups, and they want to restore the intergroup differentiation to a state that favours the ingroup.
Self-categorization
Stereotypes can emphasize a person's group membership in two steps: Stereotypes emphasize the person's similarities with ingroup members on relevant dimensions, and also the person's differences from outgroup members on relevant dimensions. People change the stereotype of their ingroups and outgroups to suit context. Once an outgroup treats an ingroup member badly, they are more drawn to the members of their own group. This can be seen as members within a group are able to relate to each other though a stereotype because of identical situations. A person can embrace a stereotype to avoid humiliation such as failing a task and blaming it on a stereotype.
Social influence and consensus
Stereotypes are an indicator of ingroup consensus. When there are intragroup disagreements over stereotypes of the ingroup and/or outgroups, ingroup members take collective action to prevent other ingroup members from diverging from each other.
John C. Turner proposed in 1987 that if ingroup members disagree on an outgroup stereotype, then one of three possible collective actions follow: First, ingroup members may negotiate with each other and conclude that they have different outgroup stereotypes because they are stereotyping different subgroups of an outgroup (e.g., Russian gymnasts versus Russian boxers). Second, ingroup members may negotiate with each other, but conclude that they are disagreeing because of categorical differences amongst themselves. Accordingly, in this context, it is better to categorise ingroup members under different categories (e.g., Democrats versus Republican) than under a shared category (e.g., American). Finally, ingroup members may influence each other to arrive at a common outgroup stereotype.
Formation
Different disciplines give different accounts of how stereotypes develop: Psychologists may focus on an individual's experience with groups, patterns of communication about those groups, and intergroup conflict. As for sociologists, they may focus on the relations among different groups in a social structure. They suggest that stereotypes are the result of conflict, poor parenting, and inadequate mental and emotional development. Once stereotypes have formed, there are two main factors that explain their persistence. First, the cognitive effects of schematic processing (see schema) make it so that when a member of a group behaves as we expect, the behavior confirms and even strengthens existing stereotypes. Second, the affective or emotional aspects of prejudice render logical arguments against stereotypes ineffective in countering the power of emotional responses.
Correspondence bias
Main article: Correspondence biasCorrespondence bias refers to the tendency to ascribe a person's behavior to disposition or personality, and to underestimate the extent to which situational factors elicited the behavior. Correspondence bias can play an important role in stereotype formation.
For example, in a study by Roguer and Yzerbyt (1999) participants watched a video showing students who were randomly instructed to find arguments either for or against euthanasia. The students that argued in favor of euthanasia came from the same law department or from different departments. Results showed that participants attributed the students' responses to their attitudes although it had been made clear in the video that students had no choice about their position. Participants reported that group membership, i.e., the department that the students belonged to, affected the students' opinions about euthanasia. Law students were perceived to be more in favor of euthanasia than students from different departments despite the fact that a pretest had revealed that subjects had no preexisting expectations about attitudes toward euthanasia and the department that students belong to. The attribution error created the new stereotype that law students are more likely to support euthanasia.
Nier et al. (2012) found that people who tend to draw dispositional inferences from behavior and ignore situational constraints are more likely to stereotype low-status groups as incompetent and high-status groups as competent. Participants listened to descriptions of two fictitious groups of Pacific Islanders, one of which was described as being higher in status than the other. In a second study, subjects rated actual groups – the poor and wealthy, women and men – in the United States in terms of their competence. Subjects who scored high on the measure of correspondence bias stereotyped the poor, women, and the fictitious lower-status Pacific Islanders as incompetent whereas they stereotyped the wealthy, men, and the high-status Pacific Islanders as competent. The correspondence bias was a significant predictor of stereotyping even after controlling for other measures that have been linked to beliefs about low status groups, the just-world fallacy and social dominance orientation.
Based on the anti-public sector bias, Döring and Willems (2021) found that employees in the public sector are considered as less professional compared to employees in the private sector. They build on the assumption that the red-tape and bureaucratic nature of the public sector spills over in the perception that citizens have about the employees working in the sector. With an experimental vignette study, they analyze how citizens process information on employees' sector affiliation, and integrate non-work role-referencing to test the stereotype confirmation assumption underlying the representativeness heuristic. The results show that sector as well as non-work role-referencing influences perceived employee professionalism but has little effect on the confirmation of particular public sector stereotypes. Moreover, the results do not confirm a congruity effect of consistent stereotypical information: non-work role-referencing does not aggravate the negative effect of sector affiliation on perceived employee professionalism.
Illusory correlation
Main article: Illusory correlationResearch has shown that stereotypes can develop based on a cognitive mechanism known as illusory correlation – an erroneous inference about the relationship between two events. If two statistically infrequent events co-occur, observers overestimate the frequency of co-occurrence of these events. The underlying reason is that rare, infrequent events are distinctive and salient and, when paired, become even more so. The heightened salience results in more attention and more effective encoding, which strengthens the belief that the events are correlated.
In the inter-group context, illusory correlations lead people to misattribute rare behaviors or traits at higher rates to minority group members than to majority groups, even when both display the same proportion of the behaviors or traits. Black people, for instance, are a minority group in the United States and interaction with blacks is a relatively infrequent event for an average white American. Similarly, undesirable behavior (e.g. crime) is statistically less frequent than desirable behavior. Since both events "blackness" and "undesirable behavior" are distinctive in the sense that they are infrequent, the combination of the two leads observers to overestimate the rate of co-occurrence. Similarly, in workplaces where women are underrepresented and negative behaviors such as errors occur less frequently than positive behaviors, women become more strongly associated with mistakes than men.
In a landmark study, David Hamilton and Richard Gifford (1976) examined the role of illusory correlation in stereotype formation. Subjects were instructed to read descriptions of behaviors performed by members of groups A and B. Negative behaviors outnumbered positive actions and group B was smaller than group A, making negative behaviors and membership in group B relatively infrequent and distinctive. Participants were then asked who had performed a set of actions: a person of group A or group B. Results showed that subjects overestimated the frequency with which both distinctive events, membership in group B and negative behavior, co-occurred, and evaluated group B more negatively. This despite the fact the proportion of positive to negative behaviors was equivalent for both groups and that there was no actual correlation between group membership and behaviors. Although Hamilton and Gifford found a similar effect for positive behaviors as the infrequent events, a meta-analytic review of studies showed that illusory correlation effects are stronger when the infrequent, distinctive information is negative.
Hamilton and Gifford's distinctiveness-based explanation of stereotype formation was subsequently extended. A 1994 study by McConnell, Sherman, and Hamilton found that people formed stereotypes based on information that was not distinctive at the time of presentation, but was considered distinctive at the time of judgement. Once a person judges non-distinctive information in memory to be distinctive, that information is re-encoded and re-represented as if it had been distinctive when it was first processed.
Common environment
One explanation for why stereotypes are shared is that they are the result of a common environment that stimulates people to react in the same way.
The problem with the 'common environment' is that explanation in general is that it does not explain how shared stereotypes can occur without direct stimuli. Research since the 1930s suggested that people are highly similar with each other in how they describe different racial and national groups, although those people have no personal experience with the groups they are describing.
Socialization and upbringing
Another explanation says that people are socialised to adopt the same stereotypes. Some psychologists believe that although stereotypes can be absorbed at any age, stereotypes are usually acquired in early childhood under the influence of parents, teachers, peers, and the media.
If stereotypes are defined by social values, then stereotypes only change as per changes in social values. The suggestion that stereotype content depends on social values reflects Walter Lippman's argument in his 1922 publication that stereotypes are rigid because they cannot be changed at will.
Studies emerging since the 1940s refuted the suggestion that stereotype contents cannot be changed at will. Those studies suggested that one group's stereotype of another group would become more or less positive depending on whether their intergroup relationship had improved or degraded. Intergroup events (e.g., World War II, Persian Gulf conflicts) often changed intergroup relationships. For example, after WWII, Black American students held a more negative stereotype of people from countries that were the United States's WWII enemies. If there are no changes to an intergroup relationship, then relevant stereotypes do not change.
Intergroup relations
According to a third explanation, shared stereotypes are neither caused by the coincidence of common stimuli, nor by socialisation. This explanation posits that stereotypes are shared because group members are motivated to behave in certain ways, and stereotypes reflect those behaviours. It is important to note from this explanation that stereotypes are the consequence, not the cause, of intergroup relations. This explanation assumes that when it is important for people to acknowledge both their ingroup and outgroup, they will emphasise their difference from outgroup members, and their similarity to ingroup members. International migration creates more opportunities for intergroup relations, but the interactions do not always disconfirm stereotypes. They are also known to form and maintain them.
Activation
The dual-process model of cognitive processing of stereotypes asserts that automatic activation of stereotypes is followed by a controlled processing stage, during which an individual may choose to disregard or ignore the stereotyped information that has been brought to mind.
A number of studies have found that stereotypes are activated automatically. Patricia Devine (1989), for example, suggested that stereotypes are automatically activated in the presence of a member (or some symbolic equivalent) of a stereotyped group and that the unintentional activation of the stereotype is equally strong for high- and low-prejudice persons. Words related to the cultural stereotype of blacks were presented subliminally. During an ostensibly unrelated impression-formation task, subjects read a paragraph describing a race-unspecified target person's behaviors and rated the target person on several trait scales. Results showed that participants who received a high proportion of racial words rated the target person in the story as significantly more hostile than participants who were presented with a lower proportion of words related to the stereotype. This effect held true for both high- and low-prejudice subjects (as measured by the Modern Racism Scale). Thus, the racial stereotype was activated even for low-prejudice individuals who did not personally endorse it. Studies using alternative priming methods have shown that the activation of gender and age stereotypes can also be automatic.
Subsequent research suggested that the relation between category activation and stereotype activation was more complex. Lepore and Brown (1997), for instance, noted that the words used in Devine's study were both neutral category labels (e.g., "Blacks") and stereotypic attributes (e.g., "lazy"). They argued that if only the neutral category labels were presented, people high and low in prejudice would respond differently. In a design similar to Devine's, Lepore and Brown primed the category of African-Americans using labels such as "blacks" and "West Indians" and then assessed the differential activation of the associated stereotype in the subsequent impression-formation task. They found that high-prejudice participants increased their ratings of the target person on the negative stereotypic dimensions and decreased them on the positive dimension whereas low-prejudice subjects tended in the opposite direction. The results suggest that the level of prejudice and stereotype endorsement affects people's judgements when the category – and not the stereotype per se – is primed.
Research has shown that people can be trained to activate counterstereotypic information and thereby reduce the automatic activation of negative stereotypes. In a study by Kawakami et al. (2000), for example, participants were presented with a category label and taught to respond "No" to stereotypic traits and "Yes" to nonstereotypic traits. After this training period, subjects showed reduced stereotype activation. This effect is based on the learning of new and more positive stereotypes rather than the negation of already existing ones.
Automatic behavioral outcomes
Empirical evidence suggests that stereotype activation can automatically influence social behavior. For example, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996) activated the stereotype of the elderly among half of their participants by administering a scrambled-sentence test where participants saw words related to age stereotypes. Subjects primed with the stereotype walked significantly more slowly than the control group (although the test did not include any words specifically referring to slowness), thus acting in a way that the stereotype suggests that elderly people will act. And the stereotype of the elder will affect the subjective perception of them through depression. In another experiment, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows also found that because the stereotype about blacks includes the notion of aggression, subliminal exposure to black faces increased the likelihood that randomly selected white college students reacted with more aggression and hostility than participants who subconsciously viewed a white face. Similarly, Correll et al. (2002) showed that activated stereotypes about blacks can influence people's behavior. In a series of experiments, black and white participants played a video game, in which a black or white person was shown holding a gun or a harmless object (e.g., a mobile phone). Participants had to decide as quickly as possible whether to shoot the target. When the target person was armed, both black and white participants were faster in deciding to shoot the target when he was black than when he was white. When the target was unarmed, the participants avoided shooting him more quickly when he was white. Time pressure made the shooter bias even more pronounced.
Accuracy
Stereotypes can be efficient shortcuts and sense-making tools. They can, however, keep people from processing new or unexpected information about each individual, thus biasing the impression formation process. Early researchers believed that stereotypes were inaccurate representations of reality. A series of pioneering studies in the 1930s found no empirical support for widely held racial stereotypes. By the mid-1950s, Gordon Allport wrote that, "It is possible for a stereotype to grow in defiance of all evidence."
Research on the role of illusory correlations in the formation of stereotypes suggests that stereotypes can develop because of incorrect inferences about the relationship between two events (e.g., membership in a social group and bad or good attributes). This means that at least some stereotypes are inaccurate.
A 1995 book by Yueh-Ting Lee et al. argued that stereotypes are sometimes accurate. Similarly, a 2015 study by Jussim et al. reviewed four studies of racial stereotypes, and seven studies of gender stereotypes regarding demographic characteristics, academic achievement, personality and behavior, and argued that some aspects of ethnic and gender stereotypes are accurate while stereotypes concerning political affiliation and nationality are much less accurate.
A 2005 study by Terracciano et al. found that stereotypic beliefs about nationality do not reflect the actual personality traits of people from different cultures.
In a 1973 paper, Marlene MacKie argued that while stereotypes are inaccurate, this is a definition rather than empirical claim – stereotypes were simply defined as inaccurate, even though the supposed inaccuracy of stereotypes was treated as though it was an empirical discovery.
Effects
Attributional ambiguity
Main article: Attributional ambiguityAttributive ambiguity refers to the uncertainty that members of stereotyped groups experience in interpreting the causes of others' behavior toward them. Stereotyped individuals who receive negative feedback can attribute it either to personal shortcomings, such as lack of ability or poor effort, or the evaluator's stereotypes and prejudice toward their social group. Alternatively, positive feedback can either be attributed to personal merit or discounted as a form of sympathy or pity.
Crocker et al. (1991) showed that when black participants were evaluated by a white person who was aware of their race, black subjects mistrusted the feedback, attributing negative feedback to the evaluator's stereotypes and positive feedback to the evaluator's desire to appear unbiased. When the black participants' race was unknown to the evaluator, they were more accepting of the feedback.
Attributional ambiguity has been shown to affect a person's self-esteem. When they receive positive evaluations, stereotyped individuals are uncertain of whether they really deserved their success and, consequently, they find it difficult to take credit for their achievements. In the case of negative feedback, ambiguity has been shown to have a protective effect on self-esteem as it allows people to assign blame to external causes. Some studies, however, have found that this effect only holds when stereotyped individuals can be absolutely certain that their negative outcomes are due to the evaluators's prejudice. If any room for uncertainty remains, stereotyped individuals tend to blame themselves.
Attributional ambiguity can also make it difficult to assess one's skills because performance-related evaluations are mistrusted or discounted. Moreover, it can lead to the belief that one's efforts are not directly linked to the outcomes, thereby depressing one's motivation to succeed.
Stereotype threat
Main article: Stereotype threatStereotype threat occurs when people are aware of a negative stereotype about their social group and experience anxiety or concern that they might confirm the stereotype. Stereotype threat has been shown to undermine performance in a variety of domains.
Claude M. Steele and Joshua Aronson conducted the first experiments showing that stereotype threat can depress intellectual performance on standardized tests. In one study, they found that black college students performed worse than white students on a verbal test when the task was framed as a measure of intelligence. When it was not presented in that manner, the performance gap narrowed. Subsequent experiments showed that framing the test as diagnostic of intellectual ability made black students more aware of negative stereotypes about their group, which in turn impaired their performance. Stereotype threat effects have been demonstrated for an array of social groups in many different arenas, including not only academics but also sports, chess and business.
Some researchers have suggested that stereotype threat should not be interpreted as a factor in real-life performance gaps, and have raised the possibility of publication bias. Other critics have focused on correcting what they claim are misconceptions of early studies showing a large effect. However, meta-analyses and systematic reviews have shown significant evidence for the effects of stereotype threat, though the phenomenon defies over-simplistic characterization.
Self-fulfilling prophecy
Main article: Self-fulfilling prophecyStereotypes lead people to expect certain actions from members of social groups. These stereotype-based expectations may lead to self-fulfilling prophecies, in which one's inaccurate expectations about a person's behavior, through social interaction, prompt that person to act in stereotype-consistent ways, thus confirming one's erroneous expectations and validating the stereotype.
Word, Zanna, and Cooper (1974) demonstrated the effects of stereotypes in the context of a job interview. White participants interviewed black and white subjects who, prior to the experiments, had been trained to act in a standardized manner. Analysis of the videotaped interviews showed that black job applicants were treated differently: They received shorter amounts of interview time and less eye contact; interviewers made more speech errors (e.g., stutters, sentence incompletions, incoherent sounds) and physically distanced themselves from black applicants. In a second experiment, trained interviewers were instructed to treat applicants, all of whom were white, like the whites or blacks had been treated in the first experiment. As a result, applicants treated like the blacks of the first experiment behaved in a more nervous manner and received more negative performance ratings than interviewees receiving the treatment previously afforded to whites.
A 1977 study by Snyder, Tanke, and Berscheid found a similar pattern in social interactions between men and women. Male undergraduate students were asked to talk to female undergraduates, whom they believed to be physically attractive or unattractive, on the phone. The conversations were taped and analysis showed that men who thought that they were talking to an attractive woman communicated in a more positive and friendlier manner than men who believed that they were talking to unattractive women. This altered the women's behavior: Female subjects who, unknowingly to them, were perceived to be physically attractive behaved in a friendly, likeable, and sociable manner in comparison with subjects who were regarded as unattractive.
A 2005 study by J. Thomas Kellow and Brett D. Jones looked at the effects of self-fulfilling prophecy on African American and Caucasian high school freshman students. Both white and black students were informed that their test performance would be predictive of their performance on a statewide, high stakes standardized test. They were also told that historically, white students had outperformed black students on the test. This knowledge created a self-fulfilling prophecy in both the white and black students, where the white students scored statistically significantly higher than the African American students on the test. The stereotype threat of underperforming on standardized tests affected the African American students in this study.
In accountancy, there is a popular stereotype which represents members of the profession as being humorless, introspective beancounters.
Discrimination and prejudice
Because stereotypes simplify and justify social reality, they have potentially powerful effects on how people perceive and treat one another. As a result, stereotypes can lead to discrimination in labor markets and other domains. For example, Tilcsik (2011) has found that employers who seek job applicants with stereotypically male heterosexual traits are particularly likely to engage in discrimination against gay men, suggesting that discrimination on the basis of sexual orientation is partly rooted in specific stereotypes and that these stereotypes loom large in many labor markets. Agerström and Rooth (2011) showed that automatic obesity stereotypes captured by the Implicit Association Test can predict real hiring discrimination against the obese. Similarly, experiments suggest that gender stereotypes play an important role in judgments that affect hiring decisions.
Stereotypes can cause racist prejudice. For example, scientists and activists have warned that the use of the stereotype "Nigerian Prince" for referring to Advance-fee scammers is racist, i.e. "reducing Nigeria to a nation of scammers and fraudulent princes, as some people still do online, is a stereotype that needs to be called out".
Self-stereotyping
Main article: Self-stereotypingStereotypes can affect self-evaluations and lead to self-stereotyping. For instance, Correll (2001, 2004) found that specific stereotypes (e.g., the stereotype that women have lower mathematical ability) affect women's and men's evaluations of their abilities (e.g., in math and science), such that men assess their own task ability higher than women performing at the same level. Similarly, a study by Sinclair et al. (2006) has shown that Asian American women rated their math ability more favorably when their ethnicity and the relevant stereotype that Asian Americans excel in math was made salient. In contrast, they rated their math ability less favorably when their gender and the corresponding stereotype of women's inferior math skills was made salient. Sinclair et al. found, however, that the effect of stereotypes on self-evaluations is mediated by the degree to which close people in someone's life endorse these stereotypes. People's self-stereotyping can increase or decrease depending on whether close others view them in stereotype-consistent or inconsistent manner.
Stereotyping can also play a central role in depression, when people have negative self-stereotypes about themselves. According to Cox, Abramson, Devine, and Hollon (2012)., stereotyping can also play a central role in depression, which is characterized by negative self-schemas. Stereotypes and self-schemas are the same type of cognitive structure, therefore, they suggest that an integrated perspective of prejudice and depression provides useful insight on how stereotypes are acquired. Negative stereotypes are set in motion within the Source, who conveys the prejudice towards the Target, which in turn will lead the Target to suffer from depression. Members of stigmatized groups may internalize the negative evaluation of their group and develop depression. People may also show prejudice internalization through self-stereotyping because of negative childhood experiences such as verbal and physical abuse. This depression that is caused by prejudice (i.e., "deprejudice") can be related to group membership (e.g., Me–Gay–Bad) or not (e.g., Me–Bad). If someone holds prejudicial beliefs about a stigmatized group and then becomes a member of that group, they may internalize their prejudice and develop depression. People may also show prejudice internalization through self-stereotyping because of negative childhood experiences such as verbal and physical abuse.
Substitute for observations
Stereotypes are traditional and familiar symbol clusters, expressing a more or less complex idea in a convenient way. They are often simplistic pronouncements about gender, racial, ethnic, and cultural backgrounds and they can become a source of misinformation and delusion. For example, in a school when students are confronted with the task of writing a theme, they think in terms of literary associations, often using stereotypes picked up from books, films, and magazines that they have read or viewed.
The danger in stereotyping lies not in its existence, but in the fact that it can become a substitute for observation and a misinterpretation of a cultural identity. Promoting information literacy is a pedagogical approach that can effectively combat the entrenchment of stereotypes. The necessity for using information literacy to separate multicultural "fact from fiction" is well illustrated with examples from literature and media.
Role in art and culture
Stereotypes are common in various cultural media, where they take the form of dramatic stock characters. The instantly recognizable nature of stereotypes mean that they are effective in advertising and situation comedy. Alexander Fedorov (2015) proposed a concept of media stereotypes analysis. This concept refers to identification and analysis of stereotypical images of people, ideas, events, stories, themes, etc. in media context.
The characters that do appear in movies greatly affect how people worldwide perceive gender relations, race, and cultural communities. Because approximately 85% of worldwide ticket sales are directed toward Hollywood movies, the American movie industry has been greatly responsible for portraying characters of different cultures and diversity to fit into stereotypical categories. This has led to the spread and persistence of gender, racial, ethnic, and cultural stereotypes seen in the movies.
For example, Russians are usually portrayed as ruthless agents, brutal mobsters and villains in Hollywood movies. According to Russian American professor Nina L. Khrushcheva, "You can't even turn the TV on and go to the movies without reference to Russians as horrible." The portrayals of Latin Americans in film and print media are restricted to a narrow set of characters. Latin Americans are largely depicted as sexualized figures such as the Latino macho or the Latina vixen, gang members, (illegal) immigrants, or entertainers. By comparison, they are rarely portrayed as working professionals, business leaders or politicians.
In Hollywood films, there are several Latin American stereotypes that have historically been used. Some examples are El Bandido, the Halfbreed Harlot, The Male Buffoon, The Female Clown, The Latin Lover, The Dark Lady, The Wise Old Man, and The Poor Peon. Many Hispanic characters in Hollywood films consists of one or more of these basic stereotypes, but it has been rare to view Latin American actors representing characters outside of this stereotypical criteria.
Media stereotypes of women first emerged in the early 20th century. Various stereotypic depictions or "types" of women appeared in magazines, including Victorian ideals of femininity, the New Woman, the Gibson Girl, the femme fatale, and the Flapper.
Stereotypes are also common in video games, with women being portrayed as stereotypes such as the "damsel in distress" or as sexual objects (see Gender representation in video games). Studies show that minorities are portrayed most often in stereotypical roles such as athletes and gangsters (see Race and video games).
In literature and art, stereotypes are clichéd or predictable characters or situations. Throughout history, storytellers have drawn from stereotypical characters and situations to immediately connect the audience with new tales.
Role in sports
Female athletes encounter various pressures and stereotypes, which have significant psychological consequences. These stereotypes give rise to challenges in athletes' lives, including diminished self-esteem, leading to more profound psychological impacts.
Female athletes have made considerable strides in overcoming obstacles. They have transitioned from being unable to compete competitively due to biological misconceptions to having equal opportunities as male athletes, thanks to Title IX. Today, there is greater societal acceptance of female athletes. However, the intersection of being a female athlete adds additional pressures. Not only are they expected to excel in competition, but they are also required to conform to societal expectations of femininity. Furthermore, female athletes often face scrutiny and criticism regarding their appearance compared to non-athletic women.
Young athletes, in particular, confront an intensified amount of pressure, leading some to quit sports because it is no longer enjoyable and the implications of being a young female athlete become overwhelming. They are unfairly labeled as gay or delicate and subjected to derogatory comments such as "like a girl." Additionally, they grapple with body image concerns that can give rise to severe health issues. Even specific sports contribute to the scrutiny female athletes face, with criticism directed at the uniforms required for competition.
The proliferation of stereotypes in women's sports has resulted in a decline in female participation. These social stigmas, including being labeled as gay or delicate, and the expectation to play in a manner deemed "like a girl," have contributed to body image issues, eating disorders, and depression among numerous female athletes.
See also
- Archetype
- Attribute substitution
- Attribution bias
- Base rate fallacy
- Cognitive bias
- Conjunction fallacy (Linda problem)
- Counterstereotype (antonym)
- Echo chamber (media)
- Ethnocentrism
- Face-ism
- Filter Bubble
- Habitus (sociology)
- Implicit stereotype
- In-group favoritism
- Labelling
- Labeling theory
- Negativity effect
- Out-group homogeneity
- Role
- Role reversal
- Role suction
- Scapegoating
- Similarity (philosophy)
- Statistical syllogism
- Stigma management
- Stigmatization
- Stock character
- Trait ascription bias
- Women-are-wonderful effect
- Gender
Examples of stereotypes
- Cultural and ethnic
- Ethnic stereotype
- List of ethnic slurs
- Stereotypes of African Americans
- Stereotypes of Africans
- Stereotypes of Americans
- Stereotypes of Arabs and Muslims in the United States
- Stereotypes of Argentines
- Stereotypes of the British
- Stereotypes of Canadians
- Stereotypes of French people
- Stereotypes of Germans
- Stereotypes of groups within the United States
- Stereotypes of Hispanic and Latino Americans in the United States
- Stereotypes of Indigenous peoples of Canada and the United States
- Stereotypes of Japanese people
- Stereotypes of Jews
- Stereotypes of Russians
- Stereotypes of South Asians
- Stereotypes of East and Southeast Asians
- Sexuality related
- Other
References
- Critchfield, Austi (15 November 2017). "This is Why Doughnuts Are Associated With Cops". Spoon University. Archived from the original on 19 November 2017. Retrieved 2 November 2020.
The funny thing is, the whole cop and doughnuts thing is completely out of date – today, an officer could just as easily swing through a McDonald's drive through as he could a Krispy Kreme. Yet, the stereotype endures, even though police aren't seen at doughnut shops in nearly the numbers they used to have been. In a way, it's become a stereotype of itself, which is pretty meta.
- Cardwell, Mike (1999). Dictionary of Psychology. Chicago: Fitzroy Dearborn. ISBN 9781579580643.
- Meyer, David G. (2008). Intuition: Its Powers and Perils. Yale University Press. pp. 114–115. ISBN 9780300130270.
- "Frequently Asked Questions". Implicit.Harvard.edu. Retrieved 14 April 2018.
- ^ Hinton, Perry (1 September 2017). "Implicit stereotypes and the predictive brain: Cognition and culture in "biased" person perception". Palgrave Communications. 3 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1057/palcomms.2017.86. S2CID 54036730.
- ^ McGarty, Craig; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y.; Spears, Russel (2002). "Social, cultural and cognitive factors in stereotype formation" (PDF). Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge University Press. pp. 1–15. ISBN 9780521800471.
- Judd, Charles M.; Park, Bernadette (1993). "Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes". Psychological Review. 100 (1): 109–128. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.1.109. PMID 8426877.
- ^ Cox, William T. L.; Abramson, Lyn Y.; Devine, Patricia G.; Hollon, Steven D. (September 2012). "Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Depression: The Integrated Perspective". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 7 (5): 427–449. doi:10.1177/1745691612455204. PMID 26168502. S2CID 1512121.
- Chakkarath, Pradeep (March 2010). "Stereotypes in social psychology: The 'West-East' differentiation as a reflection of Western traditions of thought". Psychological Studies. 55 (1): 18–25. doi:10.1007/s12646-010-0002-9. S2CID 144061506.
- Liddell, Henry George; Scott, Robert. "στερεός". A Greek-English Lexicon – via Perseus Digital Library.
- Liddell, Henry George; Scott, Robert. "τύπος". A Greek-English Lexicon – via Perseus Digital Library.
- Harper, Douglas. "stereotype – Origin and meaning". Online Etymology Dictionary. Retrieved 22 October 2017.
- Kleg, Milton (1993). Hate, Prejudice and Racism. Albany: State University of New York Press. pp. 135–137. ISBN 9780585054919.
- Vega, Tanzina. "Working while brown: What discrimination looks like now". CNNMoney. Retrieved 26 March 2018.
- ^ Fiske, Susan T. (1998). "Stereotyping, ⁰Prejudice, and Discrimination". In Gilbert, Daniel T.; Fiske, Susan T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). The Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. 2 (4th ed.). Boston, Massachusetts: McGraw-Hill. p. 357. ISBN 9780195213768.
- ^ Denmark, Florence L. (2010). "Prejudice and Discrimination". In Weiner, Irving B.; Craigheaid, W. Edward (eds.). The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. Vol. 3 (4th ed.). Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley. p. 1277. ISBN 9780470479216.
- ^ Katz, Daniel; Braly, Kenneth W. (1935). "Racial prejudice and racial stereotypes". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 30 (2): 175–193. doi:10.1037/h0059800.
- ^ Oakes, P. J.; Haslam, S. A.; Turner, J. C. (1994). Stereotyping and Social Reality. Oxford: Blackwell.
- ^ Devine, Patricia G. (1989). "Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their Automatic and Controlled Components". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 56 (1): 5–18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5. S2CID 33975027.
- ^ Tajfel, Henri (1981). "Social stereotypes and social groups". In Turner, John C.; Giles, Howard (eds.). Intergroup behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 144–167. ISBN 9780631117117.
- ^ Tilcsik, András (2011). "Pride and Prejudice: Employment Discrimination against Openly Gay Men in the United States". American Journal of Sociology. 117 (2): 586–626. doi:10.1086/661653. hdl:1807/34998. PMID 22268247. S2CID 23542996.
- ^ Operario, Don; Fiske, Susan T. (2003). "Stereotypes: Content, Structures, Processes, and Context". In Brown, Rupert; Gaertner, Samuel L. (eds.). Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. Malden, Massachusetts: Blackwell. pp. 22–44. ISBN 9781405106542.
- ^ Fiske, Susan T.; Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Glick, Peter; Xu, Jun (2002). "A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow From Perceived Status and Competition" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 82 (6): 878–902. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.320.4001. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878. PMID 12051578. S2CID 17057403.
- ^ Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Fiske, Susan T. (2002). "Doddering But Dear: Process, Content, and Function in Stereotyping of Older Persons". In Nelson, Todd D. (ed.). Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice against Older Persons. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. pp. 7–8. ISBN 9780262140775.
- Dovidio, John F.; Gaertner, Samuel L. (2010). "Intergroup Bias". In Susan T., Fiske; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. 2 (5th ed.). Hooboken, New Jersey: John Wiley. p. 1085. ISBN 9780470137475.
- Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Fiske, Susan T.; Kwan, Virginia S. Y.; Glick, Peter; Demoulin, Stéphanie; Leyens, Jacques-Philippe; Bond, Michael Harris; Croizet, Jean-Claude; Ellemers, Naomi; Sleebos, Ed; Htun, Tin Tin; Kim, Hyun-Jeong; Maio, Greg; Perry, Judi; Petkova, Kristina; Todorov, Valery; Rodríguez-Bailón, Rosa; Morales, Elena; Moya, Miguel; Palacios, Marisol; Smith, Vanessa; Perez, Rolando; Vala, Jorge; Ziegler, Rene (March 2009). "Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences". British Journal of Social Psychology. 48 (1): 1–33. doi:10.1348/014466608X314935. PMC 3912751. PMID 19178758.
- ^ Koch, A.; Imhoff, R.; Dotsch, R.; Unkelbach, C.; & Alves, H. (2016). "The ABC of Stereotypes About Groups: Agency/Socioeconomic Success, Conservative-Progressive Beliefs, and Communion". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 675–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000046
- "Groups' warmth is a personal matter: Understanding consensus on stereotype dimensions reconciles adversarial models of social evaluation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 89, 103995. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103995
- Koch, A.; Yzerbyt, V.; Abele, A.; Ellemers, N.; & Fiske, S. T. (2021). "Chapter One – Social evaluation: Comparing models across interpersonal, intragroup, intergroup, several-group, and many-group contexts". In: Gawronski, B. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 63, pp. 1–68). Academic Press. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2020.11.001
- Dragon, P.; Holland, R.; Szumowska, E.; Suska, A.; Kossowska, M.; & Wigboldus, D. (2022). "Explaining the relationship between agency and communion by means of lay personality theories and connectionist modelling". https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/g847b"
- Cao, Y. T.; Sotnikova, A.; Daum'e, H.; Rudinger, R.; & Zou, L. X. (2022). "Theory-grounded Measurement of U.S. Social Stereotypes in English Language Models". North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics.
- Koch, A.; Dorrough, A.; Glöckner, A.; & Imhoff, R. (2020). "The ABC of society: Perceived similarity in agency/socioeconomic success and conservative-progressive beliefs increases intergroup cooperation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 90, 103996. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103996
- Macrae, C. N.; Stangor, C.; Hewstone, M., eds. (1995). Stereotypes and Stereotyping. p. 4.
- McGarty, Craig; Spears, Russel; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y. (2002). "Conclusion: stereotypes are selective, variable and contested explanations". Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge University Press. pp. 186–199. ISBN 9780521800471.
- ^ Allport, Gordon W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley. p. 189. ISBN 9780201001754.
- ^ Haslam, S. Alexander; Turner, John C.; Oakes, Penelope J.; Reynolds, Katherine J.; Doosje, Bertjan (2002). "From personal pictures in the head to collective tools in the world: How shared stereotypes allow groups to represent and change social reality". Stereotypes as Explanations. pp. 157–185. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511489877.009. ISBN 9780521804820.
- Cadinu, Mara; Latrofa, Marcella; Carnaghi, Andrea (2013). "Comparing Self-stereotyping with In-group-stereotyping and Out-group-stereotyping in Unequal-status Groups: The Case of Gender". Self and Identity. 12 (6): 582–596. doi:10.1080/15298868.2012.712753. S2CID 145053003.
- Burkley, Melissa; Blanton, Hart (2008). "Endorsing a negative in-group stereotype as a self-protective strategy: Sacrificing the group to save the self". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 44: 37–49. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.01.008.
- Aronson, E.; Wilson, T. D.; Akert, R. M. (2010). Social Psychology (7th ed.). New York: Pearson.
- Mackie, Diane M.; Hamilton, David L.; Susskind, Joshua; Rosselli, Francine (1996). "Social Psychological Foundations of Stereotype Formation". In MacRae, C. Neil; Stangor, Charles; Hewstone, Miles (eds.). Stereotypes and Stereotyping. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 48–49. ISBN 9781572300538.
- Rogier, Anouk; Yzerbyt, Vincent (1999). "Social attribution, correspondence bias, and the emergence of stereotypes" (PDF). Swiss Journal of Psychology. 58 (4): 233–240. doi:10.1024//1421-0185.58.4.233. Archived from the original (PDF) on 29 July 2013. Retrieved 5 April 2013.
- Nier, Jason A.; Bajaj, Priya; McLean, Meghan C.; Schwartz, Elizabeth (2012). "Group status, perceptions of agency, and the correspondence bias: Attributional processes in the formation of stereotypes about high and low status groups". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 16 (4): 476–487. doi:10.1177/1368430212454925. S2CID 145399213.
- Marvel, John D. (30 January 2015). "Unconscious Bias in Citizens' Evaluations of Public Sector Performance". Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory: muu053. doi:10.1093/jopart/muu053. ISSN 1053-1858.
- Döring, Matthias; Willems, Jurgen (22 September 2021). "Processing stereotypes: Professionalism confirmed or disconfirmed by sector affiliation?". International Public Management Journal. 26 (2): 221–239. doi:10.1080/10967494.2021.1971125. ISSN 1096-7494. S2CID 239261451.
- Willems, Jurgen (2020). "Public servant stereotypes: It is not (at) all about being lazy, greedy and corrupt". Public Administration. 98 (4): 807–823. doi:10.1111/padm.12686. ISSN 1467-9299. S2CID 221760839.
- ^ Mullen, Brian; Johnson, Craig (1990). "Distinctiveness-based illusory correlations and stereotyping: A meta-analytic integration". British Journal of Social Psychology. 29 (1): 11–28. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00883.x.
- Meiser, Thorsten (2006). "Contingency Learning and Biased Group Impressions". In Fiedler, Klaus; Justin, Peter (eds.). Information Sampling and Adaptive Cognition. Cambridge University Press. pp. 183–209. ISBN 9780521831598.
- ^ Hamilton, David L.; Gifford, Robert K. (1976). "Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 12 (4): 392–407. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(76)80006-6.
- ^ Berndsen, Mariëtte; Spears, Russel; van der Pligt, Joop; McGarty, Craig (2002). "Illusory correlation and stereotype formation: making sense of group differences and cognitive biases". In McGarty, Craig; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y.; Spears, Russel (eds.). Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge University Press. pp. 90–110. ISBN 9780521800471.
- ^ Stroessner, Steven J.; Plaks, Jason E. (2001). "Illusory Correlation and Stereotype Formation: Tracing the Arc of Research Over a Quarter Century". In Moskowitz, Gordon B. (ed.). Cognitive Social Psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 247–259. ISBN 9780805834147.
- Ajrouch, Kristine J.; Antonucci, Toni C.; Janevic, Mary R. (1 March 2001). "Social Networks Among Blacks and Whites: The Interaction Between Race and Age". The Journals of Gerontology. 56 (2): S112–S118. doi:10.1093/geronb/56.2.S112. PMID 11245365.
- Moskowitz, Gordon B. (2005). Social Cognition: Understanding Self and Others. New York: Guilford Press. p. 182. ISBN 9781593850852.
- ^ McConnell, Allen R.; Sherman, Steven J.; Hamilton, David L. (1994). "Illusory correlation in the perception of groups: an extension of the distinctiveness-based account" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (3): 414–429. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.414. PMID 7965600.
- ^ Katz, Daniel; Braley, Kenneth (1933). "Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 28 (3): 280–290. doi:10.1037/h0074049.
- Meenes, Max (1943). "A Comparison of Racial Stereotypes of 1935 and 1942". Journal of Social Psychology. 17 (2): 327–336. doi:10.1080/00224545.1943.9712287.
- Haslam, S. Alexander; Turner, John C.; Oakes, Penelope J.; McGarty, Craig; Hayes, Brett K. (1992). "Context-dependent variation in social stereotyping 1: The effects of intergroup relations as mediated by social change and frame of reference". European Journal of Social Psychology. 22 (1): 3–20. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420220104.
- Palmer, Wayne (2019). "International Migration and Stereotype Formation: Indonesian Migrants in Hong Kong". Journal of International Migration and Integration. 21 (3): 731–744. doi:10.1007/s12134-019-00680-1. ISSN 1488-3473. S2CID 195363006.
- Devine, Patricia G.; Monteith, Margo J. (1999). "Automaticty and Control in Stereotyping". In Chaiken, Shelly; Trope, Yaacov (eds.). Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 341–342. ISBN 9781572304215.
- ^ Bargh, John A. (1994). "The Four Horsemen of Automaticity: Awareness, Intention, Efficiency, Control in Social Cognition". In Wyer, Robert S.; Srull, Thomas K. (eds.). Handbook of Social Cognition. Vol. 2 (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum. p. 21. ISBN 9780805810561.
- Banaji, Mahzarin R.; Hardin, Curtis D. (1996). "Automatic Stereotyping" (PDF). Psychological Science. 7 (3): 136–141. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x. S2CID 207858486. Archived from the original (PDF) on 21 May 2013. Retrieved 12 January 2013.
- Perdue, Charles W.; Gurtman, Michael B. (1990). "Evidence for the automaticity of ageism". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 26 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(90)90035-K.
- Brown, Rupert (2010). Prejudice: Its Social Psychology (2nd ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. p. 88. ISBN 9781405113069.
- Lepore, Lorella; Brown, Rupert (1997). "Category and Stereotype Activation: Is Prejudice Inevitable?" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (2): 275–287. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.275. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 January 2014.
- Kawakami, Kerry; Dovidio, John F.; Moll, Jasper; Hermsen, Sander; Russin, Abby (2000). "Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (5): 871–888. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871. PMID 10821195.
- ^ Gawronski, Bertram; Deutsch, Roland; Mbirkou, Sawsan; Seibt, Beate; Strack, Fritz (2008). "When 'Just Say No' is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 44 (2): 370–377. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.12.004.
- Wheeler, S. Christian; Petty, Richard E. (2001). "The Effects of Stereotype Activation on Behavior: A Review of Possible Mechanisms" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 127 (6): 797–826. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.797. PMID 11726072. Retrieved 10 June 2013.
- Devos, Thierry; Huynh, Que-Lam; Banaji, Mahzarin D. (2012). "Implicit self and identity". In Leary, Mark R.; Tangney, June Price (eds.). Handbook of self and identity (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. pp. 164–165. ISBN 9781462503056.
- Dijksterhuis, Ap (2001). "Automatic social influence: The perception-behavior links as an explanatory mechanism for behavior matching". In Forgas, Joseph P.; Williams, Kipling D. (eds.). Social influence: direct and indirect processes. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. pp. 99–100. ISBN 9781841690391.
- Operario, Din; Fiske, Susan T. (2001). "Causes and Consequences of Stereotypes in Organizations". In London, Manuel (ed.). How People Evaluate Others in Organizations. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. p. 2012. ISBN 9780805836127.
- Tschanz, Joann T.; Andersen, Aaron (2011). "Alzheimer's Dementia". Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. pp. 99–105. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_489. ISBN 9780387799476.
- Bargh, John A.; Chen, Mark; Burrows, Lara (1996). "Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 71 (2): 230–244. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.320.8563. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230. PMID 8765481. S2CID 6654763. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 October 2013. Retrieved 10 June 2013.
- Correll, Joshua; Park, Bernadette; Judd, Charles M.; Wittenbrink, Bernd (2002). "The Police Officer's Dilemma: Using Ethnicity to Disambiguate Potentially Threatening Individuals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 83 (6): 1314–1329. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.466.7243. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314. PMID 12500813.
- Lee, Yueh-Ting; Jussim, Lee J.; McCauley, Clark R., eds. (September 1995). Stereotype Accuracy: Toward Appreciating Group Differences. American Psychological Association. ISBN 9781557983077.
- Jussim, Lee; Cain, Thomas R.; Crawford, Jarret T.; Harber, Kent; Cohen, Florette (2009). "The unbearable accuracy of stereotypes". In Nelson, Todd D. (ed.). Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 199–227. ISBN 9780805859522.
- Terracciano, A.; Abdel-Khalek, A. M.; Adám, N.; Adamovová, L.; Ahn, C. K.; Ahn, H. N.; Alansari, B. M.; Alcalay, L.; Allik, J. (2005). "National Character Does Not Reflect Mean Personality Trait Levels in 49 Cultures". Science. 310 (5745): 96–100. Bibcode:2005Sci...310...96T. doi:10.1126/science.1117199. PMC 2775052. PMID 16210536.
- Mackie, Marlene. "Arriving at "truth" by definition: The case of stereotype inaccuracy." Social problems 20, no. 4 (1973): 431–447.
- ^ Zemore, Sarah E.; Fiske, Susan T.; Kim, Hyun-Jeong (2000). "Gender Stereotypes and the Dynamics of Social Interaction". In Eckes, Thomas; Trautner, Hanns Martin (eds.). The Developmental Social Psychology of Gender. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 229–230. ISBN 9780585300658.
- ^ Crocker, Jennifer; Major, Brenda; Stelle, Claude (1998). "Social Stigma". In Gilbert, Daniel T.; Fiske, Susan T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). The Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. 2 (4th ed.). Oxford University Press. pp. 519–521. ISBN 9780195213768.
- Whiteley, Bernard E.; Kite, Mary E. (2010). The Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination (2nd ed.). Belmont, California: Wadsworth / Cengage Learning. pp. 428–435. ISBN 9780495599647.
- Crocker, Jennifer; Voelkl, Kristin; Testa, Maria; Major, Brenda (1991). "Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60 (2): 218–228. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.218.
- Osborne, Jason W. (2007). "Linking Stereotype Threat and Anxiety". Educational Psychology. 27 (1): 135–154. doi:10.1080/01443410601069929. S2CID 144555070.
- Quinn, Diane M.; Kallen, Rachel W.; Spencer, Steven J. (2010). "Stereotype Threat". In Hewstone, Miles; Glick, Peter; Dovidio, John F.; Esses, Victoria M. (eds.). The SAGE Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping and Discrimination. Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications. pp. 379–394. ISBN 9781412934534.
- Inzlicht, Michael; Tullett, Alexa M.; Gutsell, Jennifer N. (2012). "Stereotype Threat Spillover: The Short- and Long-term Effects of Coping with Threats to Social Identity". In Inzlicht, Michael; Schmader, Toni (eds.). Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 108. ISBN 9780199732449.
- Aronson, Joshua; Stelle, Claude M. (2005). "Chapter 24: Stereotypes and the Fragility of Academic Competence, Motivation, and Self-Concept". In Elliot, Andrew J.; Dweck, Carol S. (eds.). Handbook of Competence and Motivation. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 436, 443. ISBN 9781593851231.
- Steele, Claude M.; Aronson, Joshua (November 1995). "Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 69 (5): 797–811. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797. PMID 7473032. S2CID 4665022.
- Stone, Jeff; Lynch, Christian I.; Sjomeling, Mike; Darley, John M. (1999). "Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 77 (6): 1213–1227. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.687.5039. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213. S2CID 14568298.
- Maass, Anne; D'Ettole, Claudio; Cadinu, Mara (2008). "Checkmate? The role of gender stereotypes in the ultimate intellectual sport" (PDF). European Journal of Social Psychology. 38 (2): 231–245. doi:10.1002/ejsp.440. S2CID 144308852. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 November 2012. Retrieved 5 September 2012.
- Gupta, V. K.; Bhawe, N. M. (2007). "The Influence of Proactive Personality and Stereotype Threat on Women's Entrepreneurial Intentions". Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies. 13 (4): 73–85. doi:10.1177/10717919070130040901. S2CID 145318243.
- Ganley, C. M.; Mingle, L. A.; Ryan, A. M.; Ryan, K.; Vasilyeva, M.; Perry, M. (October 2013). "An examination of stereotype threat effects on girls' mathematics performance" (PDF). Developmental Psychology. 49 (10): 1886–1897. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.353.4436. doi:10.1037/a0031412. PMID 23356523. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 July 2014.
- Stoet, G.; Geary, D. C. (2012). "Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement?". Review of General Psychology. 16: 93–102. doi:10.1037/a0026617. S2CID 145724069. Pdf. Archived 12 January 2016 at the Wayback Machine
- Flore, Paulette C.; Wicherts, Jelte M. (2014). "Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis". Journal of School Psychology. 53 (1): 25–44. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002. ISSN 0022-4405. PMID 25636259. S2CID 206516995.
- Sackett, P. R.; Hardison, C. M.; Cullen, M. J. (January 2004). "On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American-White differences on cognitive tests" (PDF). The American Psychologist. 59 (1): 7–13. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7. PMID 14736315. Archived from the original (PDF) on 4 April 2013.
- Pennington, C. R.; Heim, D.; Levy, A. R.; Larkin, D. T. (11 January 2016). "Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators". PLOS ONE. 11 (1): e0146487. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1146487P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146487. PMC 4713435. PMID 26752551.
- Nguyen, H. H.; Ryan, A. M. (November 2008). "Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 93 (6): 1314–1334. doi:10.1037/a0012702. PMID 19025250. S2CID 36769821.
- Walton, G. M.; Spencer, S. J. (September 2009). "Latent ability: grades and test scores systematically underestimate the intellectual ability of negatively stereotyped students". Psychological Science. 20 (9): 1132–1139. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x. PMID 19656335. S2CID 25810191.
- Gentile, A.; Boca, S.; Giammusso, I. (1 November 2018). "'You play like a Woman!' Effects of gender stereotype threat on Women's performance in physical and sport activities: A meta-analysis". Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 39: 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.07.013. ISSN 1469-0292. S2CID 149490634.
- Lamont, R. A.; Swift, H. J.; Abrams, D. (March 2015). "A review and meta-analysis of age-based stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage". Psychology and Aging. 30 (1): 180–193. doi:10.1037/a0038586. PMC 4360754. PMID 25621742.
- ^ Picho, K.; Rodriguez, A.; Finnie, L. (May 2013). "Exploring the moderating role of context on the mathematics performance of females under stereotype threat: a meta-analysis". The Journal of Social Psychology. 153 (3): 299–333. doi:10.1080/00224545.2012.737380. PMID 23724702. S2CID 45950675.
- Liu, S.; Liu, P.; Wang, M.; Zhang, B. (June 2021). "Effectiveness of stereotype threat interventions: A meta-analytic review". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 106 (6): 921–949. doi:10.1037/apl0000770. PMID 32772526. S2CID 221098319.
- VanLandingham, H.; Ellison, R. L.; Laique, A.; Cladek, A.; Khan, H.; Gonzalez, C.; Dunn, M. R. (February 2022). "A scoping review of stereotype threat for BIPOC: Cognitive effects and intervention strategies for the field of neuropsychology". The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 36 (2): 503–522. doi:10.1080/13854046.2021.1947388. PMID 34233577. S2CID 235769090.
- Kassin, Saul M.; Fein, Steven; Markus, Hazel Rose (2011). Social psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, California: Wadsworth / Cengage Learning. p. 172. ISBN 9780495812401.
- Brown, Rupert (2010). Prejudice: Its Social Psychology (2nd ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 94–97. ISBN 9781405113069.
- Chen, Mark; Bargh, John A. (1997). "Nonconscious Behavioral Confirmation Processes: The Self-Fulfilling Consequences of Automatic Stereotype Activation" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 33 (5): 541–560. doi:10.1006/jesp.1997.1329. Archived from the original (PDF) on 22 May 2013. Retrieved 5 April 2013.
- Word, Carl O.; Zanna, Mark P.; Cooper, Joel (1974). "The nonverbal mediation of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 10 (2): 109–120. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(74)90059-6.
- Snyder, Mark; Tanke, Elizabeth D.; Berscheid, Ellen (1977). "Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 35 (9): 656–666. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.656. Archived from the original (PDF) on 17 June 2012.
- Kellow, Thomas (February 2008). "The Effects of Stereotypes on the Achievement Gap: Reexamining the Academic Performance of African American High School Students". Journal of Black Psychology. 34: 94–120. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.553.9188. doi:10.1177/0095798407310537. S2CID 145490359.
- Friedman, A. L.; Lyne, S. R. (2001). "The beancounter stereotype: Towards a general model of stereotype generation". Critical Perspectives on Accounting. 12 (4): 423–451. doi:10.1006/cpac.2000.0451.
- Jeacle, I.; Miley, F.; Read, A. (2012). "Jokes in popular culture: The characterisation of the accountant". Accounting, Auditing & Accountability Journal.
- Banaji, Mahzarin R. (2002). "The Social Psychology of Stereotypes". In Smelser, Neil; Baltes, Paul (eds.). International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. New York: Pergamon. pp. 15100–15104. doi:10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01754-X. ISBN 9780080430768.
- Fiske, Susan T.; Lee, Tiane L. (2008). "Stereotypes and prejudice create workplace discrimination". In Brief, Arthur P (ed.). Diversity at Work. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 13–52. ISBN 9780521860307.
- Agerström, Jens; Rooth, Dan-Olof (2011). "The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination". Journal of Applied Psychology. 96 (4): 790–805. doi:10.1037/a0021594. PMID 21280934.
- Rudman, Laurie A.; Glick, Peter (2001). "Prescriptive Gender Stereotypes and Backlash toward Agentic Women" (PDF). Journal of Social Issues. 57 (4): 743–762. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00239. hdl:2027.42/146421. S2CID 54219902. Archived from the original (PDF) on 6 November 2012.
- Yékú, James (9 September 2020). "Anti-Afropolitan ethics and the performative politics of online scambaiting". Social Dynamics. 46 (2): 240–258. doi:10.1080/02533952.2020.1813943. ISSN 0253-3952. S2CID 222232833.
- Sinclair, Stacey; Huntsinger, Jeff (2006). "The Interpersonal Basis of Self-Stereotyping". In Levin, Shana; Van Laar, Colette (eds.). Stigma and Group Inequality: Social Psychological Perspectives. Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. p. 239. ISBN 9780805844153.
- Correll, Shelley J. (2001). "Gender and the career choice process: The role of biased self-assessments" (PDF). American Journal of Sociology. 106 (6): 1691–1730. doi:10.1086/321299. S2CID 142863258. Archived from the original (PDF) on 12 September 2011.
- Correll, Shelley J. (2004). "Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations" (PDF). American Sociological Review. 69 (1): 93–113. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.520.8370. doi:10.1177/000312240406900106. S2CID 8735336.
- Sinclair, Stacey; Hardin, Curtis D.; Lowery, Brian S. (2006). "Self-Stereotyping in the Context of Multiple Social Identities" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 90 (4): 529–542. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.529. PMID 16649853. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 August 2014.
- Sachs, Nicole M.; Veysey, Bonita M.; Rivera, Luis M. (14 November 2017). "Implicit Social Cognitive Processes Underlying Victim Self and Identity: Evidence With College-Aged Adults". Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 36 (3–4): 1256–1282. doi:10.1177/0886260517741625. PMID 29294984. S2CID 206565963.
- Hayakawa, S. I. (1950). "Recognizing Stereotypes as Substitutes for Thought". ETC: A Review of General Semantics. 7 (3): 208–210. JSTOR 42581302.
- Taylor, Rhonda Harris; Patterson, Lotsee (December 2000). "Using information literacy to promote critical thinking". Teacher Librarian. 28 (2): 9–14. OCLC 425340563.
- "Lesson 2 – Stock Characters | BYU Theatre Education Database". tedb.byu.edu. Retrieved 30 March 2018.
- Fedorov, Alexander (2015). "Media Stereotypes Analysis in the Classroom at the Student Audience". European Journal of Contemporary Education. 12 (2): 158–162. doi:10.13187/ejced.2015.12.158.
- Lee, Kevin (January 2008). "'The Little State Department': Hollywood and the MPAA's Influence on U.S. Trade Relations". Northwestern Journal of International Law & Business. 28 (2).
- "Will the cliche of the 'Russian baddie' ever leave our screens?". The Guardian. 10 July 2017.
- "Russian film industry and Hollywood uneasy with one another." Fox News. 14 October 2014
- "5 Hollywood Villains That Prove Russian Stereotypes Are Hard to Kill". The Moscow Times. 9 August 2015.
- "Hollywood stereotypes: Why are Russians the bad guys?". BBC News. 5 November 2014.
- Berg, Charles (Summer 1990). "Stereotyping in films in general and of the Hispanic in Particular". The Howard Journal of Communications. 2 (3): 294–296. doi:10.1080/10646179009359721.
- Coltrane, Scott; Adams, Michele (April 1997). "Work–Family Imagery and Gender Stereotypes: Television and the Reproduction of Difference". Journal of Vocational Behavior. 50 (2): 323–347. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1996.1575.
- Mou, Yi; Peng, Wei (January 2008). "Gender and Racial Stereotypes in Popular Video Games". Handbook of Research on Effective Electronic Gaming in Education. Hershey, Pa.: Information Science Reference. doi:10.4018/9781599048086.ch053. ISBN 9781599048086. OCLC 755871149. Book: doi:10.4018/978-1-59904-808-6.
- Burgess, Melinda; Dill, Karen (15 September 2011). "Playing With Prejudice: The Prevalence and Consequences of Racial Stereotypes in Video Games". Media Psychology. 14 (3): 289–311. doi:10.1080/15213269.2011.596467. S2CID 1416833.
- Auracher, Jan; Hirose, Akiko (2017). "The Influence of Reader's Stereotypes on the Assessment of Fictional Characters". Comparative Literature Studies. 54 (4): 795–823. doi:10.5325/complitstudies.54.4.0795. JSTOR 10.5325/complitstudies.54.4.0795. S2CID 148778808.
- ^ Wartel, Jennifer (5 May 2021). "Stereotypes in Women's Sports and their Psychological Effects". Academic Festival.
Further reading
Scholia has a topic profile for Stereotype.- "Are Stereotypes True?". Beta.In-Mind.org. Archived from the original on 24 July 2010. Retrieved 25 July 2018.
- Crawford, M.; Unger, R. (2004). Women and Gender: A Feminist Psychology. New York: McGraw Hill. pp. 45–49.
- Ewen, Stuart; Ewen, Elizabeth (2006). Typecasting: On the Arts and Sciences of Human Inequality. New York: Seven Stories Press.
- Hilton, James L.; von Hippel, William (1996). "Stereotypes". Annual Review of Psychology. 47 (1): 237–271. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.237. PMID 15012482.
- Regenberg, Nina (2007). "Are Blonds Really Dumb?". The Inquisitive Mind (3). Archived from the original on 26 July 2011. Retrieved 4 May 2010.
- Stereotype & Society
- Spitzer, B. L.; Henderson, K.; Zavian, M. T. (1999). "Gender Differences in Population Versus Media Body Sizes: A Comparison over Four Decades". Sex Roles. 40 (7/8): 545–565. doi:10.1023/a:1018836029738. S2CID 55674520.
- Turner, Chris (2004). Planet Simpson: How a Cartoon Masterpiece Documented an Era and Defined a Generation. Foreword by Douglas Coupland. Toronto: Random House Canada. ISBN 9780679313182. OCLC 55682258.
External links
- Interview with social psychologists Susan Fiske and Mike North about the stereotyping of older people
- "How gender stereotypes influence emerging career aspirations"—Lecture by Stanford University sociologist Shelley Correll on 21 October 2010
- Stereotyping—Social Psychology Network
- "Stereotypes"—Media Smarts, Canada's Centre for Digital and Media Literacy
- Age and Health based stereotyping at AHealthCareer
- "The Danger of a Single Story"—TEDTalk by Chimamanda Adichie
Social psychology | ||
---|---|---|
Interpersonal relationships | ||
Conflict | ||
Social influence | ||
Group dynamics | ||
Aggression | ||
Altruism | ||
Culture | ||
Self-concept | ||
Attribution |
Stereotypes in the United States | |
---|---|
Ethnic & national |
|
Regional & social |
Stock characters | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
| |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
|