Revision as of 22:41, 5 October 2007 editValjean (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Extended confirmed users, IP block exemptions, Pending changes reviewers, Rollbackers95,282 edits grammar← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 01:27, 18 November 2024 edit undo2601:447:c600:4ce0:a11f:a5f1:5fb7:8139 (talk) ceTags: Mobile edit Mobile app edit iOS app edit App section source | ||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Short description|Claimed form of extrasensory perception}} | |||
{{TotallyDisputed}} | |||
{{About|the alleged extrasensory ability|the album|Clairvoyance (album){{!}}''Clairvoyance'' (album)|the book|Clairvoyance (book){{!}}''Clairvoyance'' (book)}} | |||
{{Infobox Paranormalterms | |||
{{redirect|Clairvoyant|other uses|Clairvoyant (disambiguation)}} | |||
|Image_Name = Ganzfeld.jpg | |||
{{Use mdy dates|date=June 2013}} | |||
|Image_Caption = An ] aiming to stimulate clairvoyance. | |||
{{Paranormal|main}} | |||
|Usage = Terminology | |||
] to explain clairvoyance<ref>{{cite book |title=Les Miroirs Magiques |author=Paul Sédir |url=https://commons.wikimedia.org/File:Paul_Sedir_-_Les_Miroirs_Magiques_-_1907.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190403140616/https://commons.wikimedia.org/File:Paul_Sedir_-_Les_Miroirs_Magiques_-_1907.pdf |archive-date=2019-04-03 |url-status=live |location=Paris |series=Librairie Générale des Sciences Occultes |edition=3rd |year=1907 |page=22}}</ref>]] | |||
|Name = Clairvoyance | |||
|Origin = | |||
|Short = | |||
|Additional_Names = | |||
|Definition = The transference of information about an object, location or contemporary physical event through Psi. | |||
|Characteristics = An individual who can gather information from an external contemporary source that is hidden from their traditional senses by distance or physical barriers. | |||
|Extra_Title = | |||
|Extra_Column = | |||
|See_Also = ],<br>],<br>],] | |||
}}'''Clairvoyance''' (from 17th century ] ''Clair'' meaning "clear" and ''voyant'' meaning "seeing") is a form of ] involving the transference of information about an object, location or physical event through means other than the known human ].<ref>http://parapsych.org/glossary_a_d.html#c FAQ of the Parapsychological Association retrieved August 17, 2007</ref><ref>http://parapsych.org/glossary_e_k.html#e FAQ of the Parapsychological Association on EXTRASENSORY PERCEPTION (ESP), retrieved August 17, 2007</ref> A person said to have the ability of clairvoyance is referred to as a ''clairvoyant''. | |||
'''Clairvoyance''' ({{IPAc-en|k|l|ɛər|ˈ|v|ɔɪ|.|ə|n|s}}; {{etymology|fr|{{wikt-lang|fr|clair}}|clear||{{wikt-lang|fr|voyance}}|vision}}) is the claimed ability to acquire information that would be considered impossible to get through scientifically proven sensations, thus classified as ], or "sixth sense".<ref>{{cite Merriam-Webster|clairvoyance |quote=1: the power or faculty of discerning objects not present to the senses<br>2: ability to perceive matters beyond the range of ordinary perception: penetration |access-date=2022-02-22}} {{Cite web |url=http://mw1.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/clairvoyance |title=Clairvoyance - Definition and More from the Free Merriam-Webster Dictionary |access-date=October 6, 2007 |archive-date=February 27, 2012 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120227103952/http://mw1.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/clairvoyance |url-status=bot: unknown }}</ref><ref>{{cite encyclopedia |url=https://www.britannica.com/ebc/article-9364105 |title=clairvoyance |encyclopedia=] |url-status=dead |access-date=2007-10-07 |postscript=. The ESP entry includes clairvoyance. |archive-date=February 18, 2008 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080218091840/http://www.britannica.com/ebc/article-9364105 }}</ref> Any person who is claimed to have such ability is said to be a '''clairvoyant''' ({{IPAc-en|k|l|ɛər|ˈ|v|ɔɪ|.|ə|n|t}})<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.oxfordlearnersdictionaries.com/definition/english/clairvoyant_1 |title=clairvoyant |website=Oxford Learners Dictionaries |access-date=2017-04-08}}</ref> ({{gloss|one who sees clearly}}). | |||
Claims for the existence of ] ] abilities such as clairvoyance are highly controversial. ] explores this possibility, but no evidence for paranormal phenomena has gained wide acceptance within the mainstream ]. | |||
Claims for the existence of ] and ] abilities such as clairvoyance have not been supported by scientific evidence.<ref name=skepdic1>]. (2003). . Retrieved 2014-04-30.</ref> ] explores this possibility, but the existence of the paranormal is not accepted by the ].<ref> | |||
* ]. (1983). ''Treatise on Basic Philosophy: Volume 6: Epistemology & Methodology II: Understanding the World''. Springer. p. 226. {{ISBN|90-277-1635-8}} "Despite being several thousand years old, and having attracted a large number of researchers over the past hundred years, we owe no single firm finding to parapsychology: no hard data on telepathy, clairvoyance, precognition, or psychokinesis." | |||
* ]. (1990). ''Physics and Psychics: The Search for a World Beyond the Senses''. Prometheus Books. p. 166. {{ISBN|0-87975-575-X}} "The bottom line is simple: science is based on consensus, and at present a scientific consensus that psychic phenomena exist is still not established." | |||
* Zechmeister, Eugene; Johnson, James. (1992). ''Critical Thinking: A Functional Approach''. Brooks/Cole Pub. Co. p. 115. {{ISBN|0534165966}} "There exists no good scientific evidence for the existence of paranormal phenomena such as ESP. To be acceptable to the scientific community, evidence must be both valid and reliable." | |||
* ]. (2003). ''Pseudoscience and the Paranormal''. Prometheus Books. p. 144. {{ISBN|1-57392-979-4}} "It is important to realize that, in one hundred years of parapsychological investigations, there has never been a single adequate demonstration of the reality of any psi phenomenon."</ref> The scientific community widely considers parapsychology, including the study of clairvoyance, a ].<ref>{{cite web|url=http://dictionary.reference.com/browse/pseudoscience |title=Dictionary.com "Pseudoscience" |publisher=Dictionary.reference.com |access-date=September 22, 2012}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/pseudo-science/ |title=Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy "Science and Pseudo-Science" |publisher=Plato.stanford.edu |date=September 3, 2008 |access-date=September 22, 2012}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.quackwatch.com/01QuackeryRelatedTopics/russian.html |title=Science Needs to Combat Pseudoscience: A Statement by 32 Russian Scientists and Philosophers |publisher=Quackwatch.com |date=July 17, 1998 |access-date=September 22, 2012}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.csj.org/studyindex/studycrthk/study_pseddoscience/study_factscnfiction5.htm |title=International Cultic Studies Association "Science Fiction in Pseudoscience" |publisher=Csj.org |access-date=September 22, 2012 |archive-date=August 12, 2018 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180812231115/http://www.csj.org/studyindex/studycrthk/study_pseddoscience/study_factscnfiction5.htm |url-status=dead }}</ref><ref> | |||
* {{citation|last1=Gross|first1=Paul R|last2=Levitt|first2=Norman|last3=Lewis|first3=Martin W|year=1996|title=The Flight from Science and Reason|publisher=New York Academy of Sciences|page=|isbn=978-0801856761|quote=The overwhelming majority of scientists consider parapsychology, by whatever name, to be pseudoscience.|url=https://archive.org/details/flightfromscienc0000unse_w3d8/page/565}} | |||
* {{citation|last=Friedlander|first=Michael W|year=1998|title=At the Fringes of Science|publisher=Westview Press|page=119|isbn=978-0-8133-2200-1|quote=Parapsychology has failed to gain general scientific acceptance even for its improved methods and claimed successes, and it is still treated with a lopsided ambivalence among the scientific community. Most scientists write it off as pseudoscience unworthy of their time.}} | |||
* {{citation|last1=Pigliucci|first1=Massimo|last2=Boudry|first2=Maarten|year=2013|title=Philosophy of Pseudoscience: Reconsidering the Demarcation Problem|publisher=University Of Chicago Press|page=158|isbn=978-0-226-05196-3|quote=Many observers refer to the field as a 'pseudoscience'. When mainstream scientists say that the field of parapsychology is not scientific, they mean that no satisfying naturalistic cause-and-effect explanation for these supposed effects has yet been proposed and that the field's experiments cannot be consistently replicated.|hdl=1854/LU-3161824}}</ref><ref name="Cordón">{{Cite book |author=Cordón, Luis A. |title=Popular Psychology: An Encyclopedia |publisher=] |location=Westport, Conn |year=2005 |page= |isbn=978-0-313-32457-4 |quote=The essential problem is that a large portion of the scientific community, including most research psychologists, regards parapsychology as a pseudoscience, due largely to its failure to move beyond null results in the way science usually does. Ordinarily, when experimental evidence fails repeatedly to support a hypothesis, that hypothesis is abandoned. Within parapsychology, however, more than a century of experimentation has failed even to conclusively demonstrate the mere existence of paranormal phenomenon, yet parapsychologists continue to pursue that elusive goal. |url=https://archive.org/details/popularpsycholog0000cord/page/182 }}</ref> | |||
==Usage== | ==Usage== | ||
Pertaining to the ability of clear-sightedness, clairvoyance refers to the paranormal ability to see persons and events that are distant in time or space. It can be divided into roughly three classes: ], the ability to perceive or predict future events, ], the ability to see past events, and ], the perception of contemporary events happening outside the range of normal perception.<ref>]. (2001). ''The Encyclopedia of Occultism & Parapsychology''. p. 297. Gale Group, Detroit. {{ISBN|978-0810385702}}.</ref> | |||
==In history and religion== | |||
Within ], clairvoyance is used exclusively to refer to the transfer of information that is both contemporary to, and hidden from, the clairvoyant. It is differentiated from ] in that the information is said to be gained directly from an external physical source, rather than being transferred from the mind of one individual to another. <ref name=parasoc1> — ] (2007-04-27)</ref> | |||
Throughout history, there have been numerous places and times in which people have claimed themselves, or others, to be clairvoyant. | |||
In several religions, stories of certain individuals being able to see things far removed from their immediate sensory perception are commonplace, especially within pagan religions where ]s were used. ] often involved some degree of clairvoyance, especially when future events were predicted. This ability has sometimes been attributed to a higher power rather than to the person performing it. | |||
Outside of parapsychology, clairvoyance is often used to refer to other forms of ], most commonly the perception of events that have occurred in the past, or which will occur in the future (known as ] and ] respectively), <ref name=skepdic1>Carrol, Robert (2003), "" - ''Skeptics Dictionary'', Wiley, ISBN 0471272426</ref><ref name=parasoc1/>, or to refer to communications with the dead (see ]). | |||
===Christianity=== | |||
Clairvoyace is related to ], although the term "remote viewing" itself is not as widely applicable to clairvoyance because it refers to a specific controlled process. | |||
A number of ] were said to be able to see or know things that were far removed from their immediate sensory perception as a kind of gift from God, including ], ] and ] in ] and ], ] and ] in ]. ] in the Gospels is also recorded as able to know things far removed from his immediate human perception. Some Christians today also share the same claim.{{Citation needed|date=August 2024}} | |||
===Jainism=== | |||
==Status of clairvoyance== | |||
{{Main|Jain epistemology}} | |||
In ], clairvoyance is regarded as one of the five kinds of knowledge. The beings of hell and heaven (]) are said to possess clairvoyance by birth. According to Jain text ], "this kind of knowledge has been called ''avadhi'' as it ascertains matter in downward range or knows objects within limits".{{sfn|S. A. Jain|1992|p=16}} | |||
===Anthroposophy=== | |||
Within the field of parapsychology, there is a consensus that some instances of clairvoyance are verifiable. <ref name=parasoc2>"", FAQ - Parapsychological Association (])</ref><ref name=parasoc3>"", FAQ - Parapsychological Association (])</ref>. There is also a measured level of belief from amongst the general public, with the portion of the US population who believe in clairvoyance varying between 1/4 and 1/3 over the last 15 years. | |||
], famous as a clairvoyant himself,<ref name = CorrDoc>Steiner, ''Correspondence and Documents 1901–1925'', 1988, p. 9. {{ISBN|0880102071}}</ref><ref name="Ruse2018">{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=D1RuDwAAQBAJ&pg=PA97|title=The Problem of War: Darwinism, Christianity, and Their Battle to Understand Human Conflict|last=Ruse|first=Michael|year= 2018|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0-19-086757-7|page=97}}</ref> claimed that it is easy for a clairvoyant to confuse their own emotional and spiritual being with the objective spiritual world.<ref>Rudolf Steiner, ''Errors in Spiritual Investigation: Meeting the Guardian of the Threshold'', A Lecture Berlin, March 6, 1913, Bn 62; GA 62; CW 62, Mercury Press, Spring Valley, New York, 1983, https://wn.rsarchive.org/Lectures/19130306p01.html Quote: "He therefore must learn above all else to know himself, so that when he is able to confront a spiritual outer world in the same way as he confronts an objective being he can distinguish himself from what is truth. If he does not learn to delimit himself in this way, he will always confuse that which is only within him, that which is only his subjective experience, with the spiritual world picture; he can never arrive at a real grasp of spiritual reality."</ref><ref>Rudolf Steiner ''An Esoteric Cosmology'' Eighteen Lectures delivered in Paris May 25 to June 14, 1906, Bn 94.1, GA 94, France. St. George Publications, Spring Valley, New York, 1978, IX. The Astral World, https://wn.rsarchive.org/Lectures/GA094/English/SGP1978/19060602p01.html Quote: "Another result of this inverse unraveling of things in the astral world is that it teaches man to know himself. Feelings and passions are expressed by plant and animal forms. When man begins to behold his passions in the astral world he sees them as animal forms. These forms proceed from himself, but he sees them as if they were assailing him. This is because his own being is objectivised—otherwise he could not behold himself. Thus it is only in the astral world that man learns true self knowledge in contemplating the images of his passions in the animal forms which hurl, themselves upon him. A feeling of hatred entertained against another being appears as an attacking demon."</ref> | |||
==Parapsychology== | |||
{| | |||
! Year | |||
! Belief | |||
|- | |||
| 1990 | |||
| 26% | |||
|- | |||
| 2000 | |||
| 32% | |||
|- | |||
| 2005 | |||
| 26% | |||
|} <ref name=skepdic1/> | |||
===Early research=== | |||
The concept of clairvoyance gained some support from the US and Russian government during and after the ], and both governments made several attempts to harness it as an intelligence gathering tool. <ref name=Time>Waller, Douglas (1995-12-11), "The Vision Thing", '']'', p.45</ref> | |||
The earliest record of ] clairvoyance is credited to the ], a follower of ], who in 1784 was treating a local dull-witted peasant named Victor Race. During treatment, Race reportedly went into a trance and underwent a personality change, becoming fluent and articulate, and giving diagnosis and prescription for his own disease as well as those of others.<ref>]. (1999). ''Fits, Trances, and Visions: Experiencing Religion and Explaining Experience from Wesley to James''. Princeton University Press. p. 126. {{ISBN|0-691-01024-2}}</ref> Clairvoyance was a reported ability of some mediums during the ] period of the late 19th and early 20th centuries, and psychics of many descriptions have claimed clairvoyant ability up to the present day.<ref>]. (1985). ''A Critical Historical Overview of Parapsychology''. In ]. ''A Skeptic's Handbook of Parapsychology''. Prometheus Books. pp. 3–96. {{ISBN|0-87975-300-5}}</ref> | |||
] | |||
Scientific opinion appears divided regarding phenomena such as clairvoyance. As a general rule, while trained scientists may not be as likely to believe in parapsychological phenomena as the general public, they are far from monolithic in their disbelief. Surveys of this group are rare, but in their 1994 paper in the ''Psychological Bulletin'' entitled "Does psi exist? Replicable evidence for an anomalous process of information transfer", Daryl J. Bem and Charles Honorton quote a 1979 survey: | |||
Early researchers of clairvoyance included ], Gustav Pagenstecher, and ].<ref>Roeckelein, Jon. (2006). ''Elsevier's Dictionary of Psychological Theories''. Elsevier Science. p. 450. {{ISBN|0-444-51750-2}}</ref> Clairvoyance experiments were reported in 1884 by ]. Playing cards were enclosed in envelopes and a subject under hypnosis attempted to identify them. The subject was reported to have been successful in a series of 133 trials but the results dropped to chance level when performed before a group of scientists in Cambridge. J. M. Peirce and ] reported a similar experiment in which they tested 36 subjects over 23,384 trials. They did not find above chance scores.<ref>]. ''The Search for a Demonstration of ESP''. In ]. (1985). ''A Skeptic's Handbook of Parapsychology''. Prometheus Books. pp. 97–127. {{ISBN|0-87975-300-5}}</ref> | |||
<blockquote>A survey of more than 1,100 college professors in the United States found that 55% of natural scientists, 66% of social scientists (excluding psychologists), and 77% of academics in the arts, humanities, and education believed that ESP is either an established fact or a likely possibility. The comparable figure for psychologists was only 34%. Moreover, an equal number of psychologists declared ESP to be an impossibility, a view expressed by only 2% of all other respondents (Wagner; Monnet, 1979).</blockquote> | |||
] (1911) and ] (1920) analyzed early cases of clairvoyance and concluded they were best explained by coincidence or fraud.<ref>]. (1920). ''Is Spiritualism Based On Fraud? The Evidence Given By Sir A. C. Doyle and Others Drastically Examined''. Chapter ''The Subtle Art of Clairvoyance''. London: Watts & Co. pp. 93–108</ref><ref>]. (1911). ''The Evidence for the Supernatural: A Critical Study Made with "Uncommon Sense"''. Chapter ''Telepathy and Clairvoyance''. K. Paul, Trench, Trübner. pp. 107–142</ref> In 1919, the magician ] staged a séance at his flat in ]. The spiritualist ] attended and declared the clairvoyance manifestations genuine.<ref>Baker, Robert A. (1996). ''Hidden Memories: Voices and Visions From Within''. Prometheus Books. p. 234. {{ISBN|978-1-57392-094-0}}</ref><ref>Christopher, Milbourne. (1996). ''The Illustrated History of Magic''. Greenwood Publishing Group. p. 264. {{ISBN|978-0-435-07016-8}}</ref> | |||
According to skeptics, clairvoyance is the result of fraud or self-]. <ref name=skepdic1/> | |||
A significant development in clairvoyance research came when ], a parapsychologist at ], introduced a standard methodology, with a standard statistical approach to analyzing data, as part of his research into ]. A number of psychological departments attempted and failed to repeat Rhine's experiments. At ], W. S. Cox (1936) produced 25,064 trials with 132 subjects in a playing card ESP experiment. Cox concluded: "There is no evidence of extrasensory perception either in the 'average man' or of the group investigated or in any particular individual of that group. The discrepancy between these results and those obtained by Rhine is due either to uncontrollable factors in experimental procedure or to the difference in the subjects."<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Cox | first1 = W. S. | year = 1936 | title = An experiment in ESP | doi = 10.1037/h0054630 | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 19 | issue = 4| page = 437 }}</ref> Four other psychological departments failed to replicate Rhine's results.<ref>]. (1938). ''ESP, House of Cards''. The American Scholar. Vol. 8, No. 1. pp. 13–22. "Rhine's results fail to be confirmed. At Colgate University (40, 000 tests, 7 subjects), at Chicago (extensive series on 315 students), at Southern Methodist College (75, 000 tests), at Glasgow, Scotland (6, 650 tests), at London University (105, 000 tests), not a single individual was found who under rigidly conducted experiments could score above chance. At Stanford University it has been convincingly shown that the conditions favorable to the intrusion of subtle errors produce above-chance records which come down to chance when sources of error are eliminated."</ref><ref>]. ''The Search for a Demonstration of ESP''. In ]. (1985). ''A Skeptic's Handbook of Parapsychology''. Prometheus Books. pp. 105–127. {{ISBN|0-87975-300-5}} | |||
==Clairvoyance and related phenomena through history== | |||
*{{cite journal | last1 = Adam | first1 = E. T. | year = 1938 | title = A summary of some negative experiments | journal = Journal of Parapsychology | volume = 2 | pages = 232–236 }} | |||
*Crumbaugh, J. C. (1938). ''An experimental study of extra-sensory perception''. Masters thesis. Southern Methodist University. | |||
*{{cite journal | last1 = Heinlein | first1 = C. P | last2 = Heinlein | first2 = J. H. | year = 1938 | title = Critique of the premises of statistical methodology of parapsychology | journal = Journal of Parapsychology | volume = 5 | pages = 135–148 | doi=10.1080/00223980.1938.9917558}} | |||
*Willoughby, R. R. (1938). ''Further card-guessing experiments''. ''Journal of Psychology'' 18: 3–13.</ref> It was revealed that Rhine's experiments contained methodological flaws and procedural errors.<ref>]. (1938). ''Extra-Sensory Perception: What Is It?''. ''American Journal of Sociology''. Vol. 43, No. 4. pp. 623–634. "Investigating Rhine's methods, we find that his mathematical methods are wrong and that the effect of this error would in some cases be negligible and in others very marked. We find that many of his experiments were set up in a manner which would tend to increase, instead of to diminish, the possibility of systematic clerical errors; and lastly, that the ESP cards can be read from the back."</ref><ref>Wynn, Charles; Wiggins, Arthur. (2001). ''Quantum Leaps in the Wrong Direction: Where Real Science Ends...and Pseudoscience Begins''. Joseph Henry Press. p. 156. {{ISBN|978-0-309-07309-7}} "In 1940, Rhine coauthored a book, ''Extrasensory Perception After Sixty Years'' in which he suggested that something more than mere guess work was involved in his experiments. He was right! It is now known that the experiments conducted in his laboratory contained serious methodological flaws. Tests often took place with minimal or no screening between the subject and the person administering the test. Subjects could see the backs of cards that were later discovered to be so cheaply printed that a faint outline of the symbol could be seen. Furthermore, in face-to-face tests, subjects could see card faces reflected in the tester's eyeglasses or cornea. They were even able to (consciously or unconsciously) pick up clues from the tester's facial expression and voice inflection. In addition, an observant subject could identify the cards by certain irregularities like warped edges, spots on the backs, or design imperfections."</ref><ref>]. (2003). ''Pseudoscience and the Paranormal''. Prometheus Books. p. 122. {{ISBN|978-1573929790}} "The procedural errors in the Rhine experiments have been extremely damaging to his claims to have demonstrated the existence of ESP. Equally damaging has been the fact that the results have not replicated when the experiments have been conducted in other laboratories."</ref> | |||
] was tested by Rhine at Duke University in 1933 with ]. Certain symbols were placed on the cards and sealed in an envelope, and she was asked to guess their contents. She performed poorly and later criticized the tests by claiming the cards lacked a ] called "energy stimulus" and that she could not perform clairvoyance on command.<ref>Hazelgrove, Jenny. (2000). ''Spiritualism and British Society Between the Wars''. Manchester University Press. p. 204. {{ISBN|978-0719055591}}</ref> The parapsychologist ] and his colleagues tested Garrett in May 1937. Most of the experiments were carried out in the Psychological Laboratory at the ]. A total of over 12,000 guesses were recorded but Garrett failed to produce above chance level.<ref>Russell, A. S; Benn, John Andrews. (1938). ''Discovery the Popular Journal of Knowledge''. Cambridge University Press. pp. 305–306</ref> Soal wrote: "In the case of Mrs. Eileen Garrett we fail to find the slightest confirmation of Dr. J. B. Rhine's remarkable claims relating to her alleged powers of extra-sensory perception. Not only did she fail when I took charge of the experiments, but she failed equally when four other carefully trained experimenters took my place."<ref>]. ''A Repetition of Dr. Rhine's work with Mrs. Eileen Garrett''. Proc. S.P.R. Vol. XLII. pp. 84–85. Also quoted in ]. (1955). ''A New Approach To Psychical Research''. Watts & Co. pp. 90–92.</ref> | |||
There have been anecdotal reports of clairvoyance and 'clear' abilities throughout history in most cultures.{{Fact|date=May 2007}} These episodes are often reported as being experienced through early adulthood.{{Fact|date=February 2007}} Often clairvoyance has been associated with religious or ]ic figures, offices and practices. For example, ancient Hindu religious texts list clairvoyance amongst other forms of 'clear' experiencing, as ]s, or 'perfections', skills that are yielded through appropriate meditation and personal discipline. But a large number of anecdotal accounts of clairvoyance are of the spontaneous variety among the general populace. For example, many people report seeing a loved one who has recently died before they have learned by other means that their loved one is deceased. While anecdotal accounts do not provide scientific proof of clairvoyance, such common experiences continue to motivate research into such phenomena. | |||
===Remote viewing=== | |||
Clairvoyance was one of the phenomena reportedly observed in the behavior of ], people who were ] and in a ] state (nowadays equated with ] by most people) in the time of ].{{Fact|date=May 2007}} The earliest record of somnambulistic clairvoyance is credited to the ], a follower of Mesmer, who in 1784 was treating a local dull-witted peasant named Victor Race. During treatment, Race reportedly would go into trance and undergo a personality change, becoming fluent and articulate, and giving diagnosis and prescription for his own disease as well as those of others. When he came out of the trance state he would be unaware of anything he had said or done. This behavior is somewhat reminiscent of the reported behaviors of the 20th century medical clairvoyant and psychic ]. It is reported that although Puységur used the term 'clairvoyance', he did not think of these phenomena as "]", since he accepted mesmerism as one of the natural sciences. | |||
], also known as remote sensing, remote perception, telesthesia and travelling clairvoyance, is the alleged paranormal ability to perceive a remote or hidden target without support of the senses.<ref>{{cite book |last=Blom |first=Jan |title=A dictionary of hallucinations |publisher=Springer |year=2009 |isbn=978-1-4419-1222-0 |publication-place=New York |page=451 |oclc=618047801}}</ref> | |||
Clairvoyance was a reported ability of some mediums during the ] period of the late 19th and early 20th centuries and was one of the phenomena studied by members of the ] (SPR). Psychics of many descriptions have claimed clairvoyant ability up to the present day. | |||
A well-known recent study of remote viewing is the US government-funded project at the ] from the 1970s through the mid-1990s. In 1972, ] and ] initiated a series of human subject studies to determine whether participants (the ''viewers'' or ''percipients'') could reliably identify and accurately describe salient features of remote locations (''targets''). In the early studies, a human ''sender'' was typically present at the remote location as part of the experiment protocol. A three-step process was used. First, target conditions to be experienced by the senders were randomly selected. Second, in the viewing step, participants were asked to verbally express or sketch their impressions of the remote scene. Third, in these descriptions were matched by separate judges, as closely as possible, with the intended targets. The term ] was coined to describe this overall process. The first paper by Puthoff and Targ on remote viewing was published in '']'' in March 1974; in it, the team reported some degree of remote viewing success.<ref name="nat251">{{Cite journal |last1=Targ |first1=Russel |last2=Puthoff |first2=Harold |year=1974 |title=Information transmission under conditions of sensory shielding |journal=Nature |volume=251 |issue=5476 |pages=602–607 |bibcode=1974Natur.251..602T |doi=10.1038/251602a0 |pmid=4423858 |s2cid=4152651}}</ref> After the publication of these findings, other attempts to replicate the experiments were carried out <ref name="hast1">{{Cite journal |author=Hastings, A.C. |last2=Hurt |first2=D.B. |date=October 1976 |title=A confirmatory remote viewing experiment in a group setting |journal=Proceedings of the IEEE |volume=64 |issue=10 |pages=1544–1545 |doi=10.1109/PROC.1976.10369 |s2cid=36582119}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |author=Whitson, T.W. |last2=Bogart |first2=D.N. |last3=Palmer |first3=J. |last4=Tart |first4=C.T. |date=October 1976 |title=Preliminary experiments in group 'Remote viewing' |journal=Proceedings of the IEEE |volume=64 |issue=10 |pages=1550–1551 |doi=10.1109/PROC.1976.10371 |s2cid=27302086}}</ref> with remotely linked groups using computer conferencing.<ref>{{Cite journal |author=Vallee, J. |last2=Hastings |first2=A.C. |last3=Askevold |first3=G. |date=October 1976 |title=Remote viewing experiments through computer conferencing |journal=Proceedings of the IEEE |volume=64 |issue=10 |pages=1551–1552 |doi=10.1109/PROC.1976.10372 |s2cid=24096224}}</ref> | |||
While experimental research into clairvoyance began with SPR researchers, experimental studies became more systematic with the efforts of ] and his associates at ], and such research efforts continue to the present day. Perhaps the best-known study of clairvoyance in recent times was the US government-funded ] project at ]/] during the 1970s through the mid-1990s. | |||
The psychologists ] and Richard Kammann attempted to replicate Targ and Puthoff's remote viewing experiments at the Stanford Research Institute. In a series of 35 studies, they could not do so, so they investigated the original experiments' procedure. Marks and Kammann discovered that the notes given to the judges in Targ and Puthoff's experiments contained clues as to which order they were carried out, such as referring to yesterday's two targets, or the date of the session at the top of the page. They concluded that these clues explained the experiment's high hit rates.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Marks |first1=David |author-link=David Marks (psychologist) |last2=Kammann |first2=Richard |year=1978 |title=Information transmission in remote viewing experiments |journal=Nature |volume=274 |issue=5672 |pages=680–681 |bibcode=1978Natur.274..680M |doi=10.1038/274680a0 |s2cid=4249968}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Marks |first1=David |author-link=David Marks (psychologist) |year=1981 |title=Sensory cues invalidate remote viewing experiments |journal=Nature |volume=292 |issue=5819 |page=177 |bibcode=1981Natur.292..177M |doi=10.1038/292177a0 |pmid=7242682 |s2cid=4326382|doi-access=free }}</ref> Marks achieved 100% accuracy without visiting any of the sites but by using cues.<ref>{{cite book |last=Bridgstock |first=Martin |title=Beyond belief: skepticism, science and the paranormal |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2009 |isbn=978-0-521-75893-2 |publication-place=Cambridge Port Melbourne, Vic |page=106 |oclc=652432050 |quote=The explanation used by Marks and Kammann clearly involves the use of ]. Marks and Kammann argued that the 'cues'—clues to the order in which sites had been visited—provided sufficient information for the results, without any recourse to extrasensory perception. Indeed Marks himself was able to achieve 100 percent accuracy in allocating some transcripts to sites without visiting any of the sites himself, purely on the ground basis of the cues. From Occam's razor, it follows that if a straightforward natural explanation exists, there is no need for the spectacular paranormal explanation: Targ and Puthoff's claims are not justified.}}</ref> ] has written that controlled tests by several other researchers, eliminating several sources of cuing and extraneous evidence present in the original tests, produced negative results. Students were also able to solve Puthoff and Targ's locations from the clues inadvertently included in the transcripts.<ref>{{cite Encyclopedia of Claims|title=Remote Viewing|first-letter=R|access-date=26 January 2022|archive-url=|archive-date=}}</ref> | |||
Some parapsychologists have proposed that our different functional labels (clairvoyance, ], ], etc.) all refer to one basic underlying mechanism, although there is not yet any satisfactory theory for what that mechanism may be. | |||
In 1980, ] claimed that a rejudging of the transcripts from one of Targ and Puthoff's experiments revealed an above-chance result.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Tart |first1=Charles |author-link=Charles Tart |last2=Puthoff |first2=Harold |author-link2=Harold E. Puthoff |last3=Targ |first3=Russell |author-link3=Russell Targ |year=1980 |title=Information Transmission in Remote Viewing Experiments |journal=Nature |volume=284 |issue=5752 |page=191 |bibcode=1980Natur.284..191T |doi=10.1038/284191a0 |pmid=7360248 |doi-access=free |s2cid=4326363}}</ref> Targ and Puthoff again refused to provide copies of the transcripts, and they were not made available for study until July 1985, when it was discovered they still contained ]s.<ref>{{cite book |last=Hines |first=Terence |title=Pseudoscience and the paranormal |publisher=Prometheus Books |year=2003 |isbn=978-1-57392-979-0 |publication-place=Amherst, NY |page=136 |oclc=50124260 |author-link=Terence Hines}}</ref> Marks and Christopher Scott (1986) wrote: "considering the importance for the remote viewing hypothesis of adequate cue removal, Tart's failure to perform this basic task seems beyond comprehension. As previously concluded, remote viewing has not been demonstrated in the experiments conducted by Puthoff and Targ, only the repeated failure of the investigators to remove sensory cues."<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Marks |first1=David |author-link=David Marks (psychologist) |last2=Scott |first2=Christopher |year=1986 |title=Remote Viewing Exposed |journal=Nature |volume=319 |issue=6053 |page=444 |bibcode=1986Natur.319..444M |doi=10.1038/319444a0 |pmid=3945330 |doi-access=free |s2cid=13642580}}</ref> | |||
==Parapsychological research== | |||
In 1982, ], then Dean of the School of Engineering at Princeton University, wrote a comprehensive review of psychic phenomena from an engineering perspective. His paper included numerous references to remote viewing studies at the time.<ref>{{Cite journal |author=Jahn, R.G. |date=February 1982 |title=The persistent paradox of psychic phenomena: An engineering perspective |url=http://www.princeton.edu/~pear/pdfs/1982-persistant-paradox-psychic-phenomena.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110514011527/http://www.princeton.edu/~pear/pdfs/1982-persistant-paradox-psychic-phenomena.pdf |archive-date=2011-05-14 |url-status=live |journal=Proceedings of the IEEE |volume=70 |issue=2 |pages=136–170 |citeseerx=10.1.1.15.8760 |doi=10.1109/PROC.1982.12260 |s2cid=31434794}}</ref> Statistical flaws in his work have been proposed by others in the parapsychological community and the general scientific community.<ref name="Jeffers2006">{{cite journal |author=Stanley Jeffers |date=May–June 2006 |title=The PEAR proposition: Fact or fallacy? |url=http://www.csicop.org/si/show/pear_proposition_fact_or_fallacy/ |journal=] |volume=30 |issue=3 |access-date=2014-01-24 |archive-date=February 1, 2014 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20140201122738/http://www.csicop.org/si/show/pear_proposition_fact_or_fallacy/ |url-status=dead }}</ref><ref>{{cite web |author=George P. Hansen |title=Princeton Remote-Viewing Experiments (PEAR) – A Critique |url=http://www.tricksterbook.com/ArticlesOnline/PEARCritique.htm |access-date=2014-04-06 |publisher=Tricksterbook.com}}</ref> | |||
Parapsychological research studies of ] and clairvoyance have produced favorable results significantly above chance, and meta-analysis of these studies increases the significance to astronomical proportions. For instance, at the Stanford Research Institute, remote viewing experiments undertaken between 1973 and 1988 were analyzed by Edwin May and his colleagues in 1988, and the odds against the results being due to chance were more than a billion billion to one. The SRI results were replicated at the Princeton Engineering Anomalies Research Laboratory.<ref name="ConsciousUniverse"> ''The Conscious Universe: The Scientific Truth of Psychic Phenomena'' by Dean I. Radin Harper Edge, ISBN 0-06-251502-0</ref> (Radin 1997:91-109) | |||
==Scientific reception== | |||
== Skepticism == | |||
According to scientific research, clairvoyance is generally explained as the result of ], ], fraud, ], self-], ], ], ] or failures to appreciate the base rate of chance occurrences and not as a paranormal power.<ref name=skepdic1/><ref>Rawcliffe, Donovan. (1988). ''Occult and Supernatural Phenomena''. Dover Publications. pp. 367–463. {{ISBN|0-486-20503-7}}</ref><ref>]. (1988). ''The Psychology of Anomalous Experience: A Cognitive Approach''. Prometheus Books. {{ISBN|0-87975-435-4}}</ref><ref>Zusne, Leonard; Jones, Warren. (1989). ''Anomalistic Psychology: A Study of Magical Thinking''. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 152–168. {{ISBN|0-8058-0508-7}}</ref> Parapsychology is generally regarded by the scientific community as a ].<ref>Friedlander, Michael W. (1998). ''At the Fringes of Science''. Westview Press. p. 119. {{ISBN|0-8133-2200-6}} "Parapsychology has failed to gain general scientific acceptance even for its improved methods and claimed successes, and it is still treated with a lopsided ambivalence among the scientific community. Most scientists write it off as pseudoscience unworthy of their time."</ref><ref>]; ]. (2013). ''Philosophy of Pseudoscience: Reconsidering the Demarcation Problem''. University of Chicago Press p. 158. {{ISBN|978-0-226-05196-3}} "Many observers refer to the field as a "pseudoscience". When mainstream scientists say that the field of parapsychology is not scientific, they mean that no satisfying naturalistic cause-and-effect explanation for these supposed effects has yet been proposed and that the field's experiments cannot be consistently replicated."</ref> In 1988, the ] concluded "The committee finds no scientific justification from research conducted over a period of 130 years, for the existence of parapsychological phenomena."<ref>]. (1993). ''How We Know What Isn't So: The Fallibility of Human Reason in Everyday Life''. Free Press. p. 160. {{ISBN|978-0-02-911706-4}}</ref> | |||
Parapsychological research is regarded by critics as a ]<ref>Marks, D.F. (2000). ''The Psychology of the Psychic'' (2nd Ed.) New York: Prometheus Books. ISBN 1573927988</ref> In 1988, the US ] concluded that it "...finds no scientific justification from research conducted over a period of 130 years, for the existence of parapsychological phenomena."<ref> | |||
James Randi</ref> | |||
] say that if clairvoyance were a reality it would have become abundantly clear. |
] say that if clairvoyance were a reality, it would have become abundantly clear. They also contend that those who believe in ] phenomena do so for merely psychological reasons.<ref>]; Wilson, Krissy. (2007). ''Cognitive Factors Underlying Paranormal Beliefs and Experiences''. In Sala, Sergio. ''Tall Tales About the Mind and Brain: Separating Fact From Fiction''. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 3–22. {{ISBN|978-0198568773}}</ref> According to ] (''Psychology,'' 8th ed.): | ||
<blockquote>The search for a valid and reliable test of clairvoyance has resulted in thousands of experiments. One controlled procedure has invited 'senders' to telepathically transmit one of four visual images to 'receivers' deprived of sensation in a nearby chamber (Bem & Honorton, 1994). The result? A reported 32 percent accurate response rate, surpassing the chance rate of 25 percent. But follow-up studies have (depending on who was summarizing the results) failed to replicate the phenomenon or produced mixed results (Bem & others, 2001; Milton & Wiseman, 2002; Storm, 2000, 2003).<br /><br />One skeptic, magician ], had a longstanding offer of ]—"to anyone who proves a genuine psychic power under proper observing conditions" (Randi, 1999). French, Australian, and Indian groups have parallel offers of up to 200,000 euros to anyone with demonstrable paranormal abilities (CFI, 2003). Large as these sums are, the scientific seal of approval would be worth far more to anyone whose claims could be authenticated. To refute those who say there is no ESP, one need only produce a single person who can demonstrate a single, reproducible ESP phenomenon. So far, no such person has emerged. Randi's offer has been publicized for three decades and dozens of people have been tested, sometimes under the scrutiny of an independent panel of judges. Still, nothing. "People's desire to believe in the paranormal is stronger than all the evidence that it does not exist." ], "Blackmore's first law", 2004.<ref>]. (2006). ''Psychology''. Worth Publishers; 8th edition. {{ISBN|978-0716764281}}</ref></blockquote> | |||
<blockquote> | |||
The search for a valid and reliable test of clairvoyance has resulted in thousands of experiments. One controlled procedure has invited 'senders' to telepathically transmit one of four visual images to 'receivers' deprived of sensation in a nearby chamber (Bem & Honorton, 1994). The result? A reported 32 percent accurate response rate, surpassing the chance rate of 25 percent. But follow-up studies have (depending on who was summarizing the results) failed to replicate the phenomenon or produced mixed results (Bem & others, 2001; Milton & Wiseman, 2002; Storm, 2000, 2003). | |||
</blockquote> | |||
<blockquote> | |||
One skeptic, magician ], has a longstanding offer—now U.S. $1 million—“to anyone who proves a genuine psychic power under proper observing conditions” (Randi, 1999). French, Australian, and Indian groups have parallel offers of up to 200,000 euros to anyone with demonstrable paranormal abilities (CFI, 2003). Large as these sums are, the scientific seal of approval would be worth far more to anyone whose claims could be authenticated. To refute those who say there is no ESP, one need only produce a single person who can demonstrate a single, reproducible ESP phenomenon. So far, no such person has emerged. Randi’s offer has been publicized for three decades and dozens of people have been tested, sometimes under the scrutiny of an independent panel of judges. Still, nothing. "People's desire to believe in the paranormal is stronger than all the evidence that it does not exist." ], "Blackmore's first law", 2004. | |||
</blockquote> | |||
Clairvoyance is considered a ] by mainstream ].<ref>{{cite book|last1=Blom|first1=Jan Dirk|title=A Dictionary of Hallucinations|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=qbF44AEMGdcC&q=clairvoyance+is+hallucination&pg=PA99|access-date=2012-01-11|year=2010|publisher=Springer Science+Business Media, LLC|location=New York, Dordrecht, Heidelberg, London|isbn=978-1-4419-1222-0|doi=10.1007/978-1-4419-1223-7|page=99|quote='''Clairvoyance'''<br><br>Also known as lucidity, telesthesia, and cryptestesia. ''Clairvoyance'' is French for seeing clearly. The term is used in the parapsychological literature to denote a * visual or * compound hallucination attributable to a metaphysical source. It is therefore interpreted as * telepathic, * veridical or at least * coincidental hallucination.<br><br>'''Reference'''<br>Guily, R.E. (1991) ''Harper's encyclopedia of mystical and paranormal experience.'' New York: Castle Books.}}</ref> | |||
==Other related terms== | |||
The words "clairvoyance" and "psychic" are often used to refer to many different kinds of paranormal sensory experiences, but there are more specific names: | |||
===Clairsentience (feeling/touching) === | |||
In the field of parapsychology, '''clairsentience''' is a form of ] wherein a person acquires ] knowledge primarily by means of feeling.<ref>, retrieved ], ]</ref> | |||
In addition to parapsychology, the term also plays a role in some religions. For example: clairsentience is one of the six human special functions mentioned or recorded in Buddhism. It is an ability that can be obtained at advanced meditation level. Generally the term refers to a person who can feel the vibration of other people. There are many different degrees of clairsentience ranging from the perception of diseases of other people to the thoughts or emotions of other people. The ability differs from third eye in that this kind of ability can not have a vivid picture in the mind. Instead, a very vivid feeling can form. | |||
] is related to clairsentience. The word stems from ''psyche'' and ''metric'', which means "to measure with the mind". | |||
===Clairaudience (hearing/listening)=== | |||
In the field of parapsychology, '''clairaudience''' is a form of ] wherein a person acquires information by ] auditory means. It is often considered to be a form of clairvoyance.<ref>, Retrieved ], ]</ref> Clairaudience is essentially the ability to hear in a ] manner, as opposed to paranormal seeing (clairvoyance) and feeling (]). Clairaudient people have ]-mediated hearing. Clairaudience may refer not to actual perception of sound, but may instead indicate impressions of the "inner mental ear" similar to the way many people think words without having auditory impressions. But it may also refer to actual perception of sounds such as voices, tones, or noises which are not apparent to other humans or to recording equipment. For instance, a clairaudient person might claim to hear the voices or thoughts of the spirits of persons who are deceased. Clairaudience may be positively distinguished from the voices heard by the mentally ill when it reveals information unavailable to the clairaudient person by normal means (including ] or other magic tricks), and thus may be termed "]" or ]. | |||
===Clairalience (smelling)=== | |||
In the field of parapsychology, '''clairalience''' is a form of ] wherein a person acquires psychic knowledge primarily by means of smelling.<ref>http://www.ghostvillage.com/resources/2003/resources_10132003.shtml</ref> | |||
===Claircognizance (knowing)=== | |||
In the field of parapsychology, '''claircognizance''' is a form of ] wherein a person acquires psychic knowledge primarily by means of intrinsic knowledge. It is the ability to know something without knowing how or why you know it. | |||
===Clairgustance (tasting)=== | |||
In the field of parapsychology, '''clairgustance''' is defined as a form of ] that allegedly allows one to taste a substance without putting anything in one's mouth. It is claimed that those who possess this ability are able to perceive the essence of a substance from the spiritual or ethereal realms through taste.{{Fact|date=February 2007}} | |||
== Developing clairvoyant abilities == | |||
Current thinking among proponents of clairvoyance posits that most people are born with clairvoyant abilities but then start to subliminate them as their childhood training compels them to adhere to acceptable social norms. Numerous institutes offer training courses that attempt to revive the clairvoyant abilities present in those early years.{{Fact|date=September 2007}} | |||
Another school of thought says that our "sixth sense" grows when we do spiritual practice. With regular spiritual practice done according to basic spiritual principles we increase our "]" and are able to perceive and experience the "subtle world" to greater degrees.<ref>{{Cite web | |||
|url=http://www.spiritualresearchfoundation.org/spiritualresearch/spiritualscience/sixthsense/#4 | |||
|title= Spiritual Research Foundation: How does one develop the sixth sense? | |||
|accessdate=2007-07-15 |format= |work= }}</ref> | |||
Clairvoyance is one of the abilities that may be gained by such discipline. | |||
According to many ]- and ]-related practices, abilities such as clairvoyance and many other 'supernormal' abilities are by-products of spiritual awakening and the realisation of divine consciousness. Integral to spiritual and mind expansion is ] and ]. By expanding lung capacity and learning to use the lungs as a 'bellows' to direct ] ({{lang-zh|氣}} qì, meaning "air") around the body and open the subtle energy channels we also naturally expand the mind and refine consciousness. This is how these seemingly miraculous powers develop, though they are not truly miraculous. They are considered to be latent abilities that everyone possesses but need 'waking up.'{{Fact|date=September 2007}} | |||
Such abilities in some schools of thought are considered distractions from the true path of ] and can lead to the practitioner falling off the true path. The re-discovery of these energetic abilities relies on the activation of the ']' ({{lang-zh|丹田}} dān tián, meaning "energy gate") that is the central energy reservoir just below the navel. When the practitioner learns to 'turn' it and move it as if it were a fifth limb then qi can begin to be pushed around the body. The Dan Tien is strong as a baby but quickly slows to a crawl as one ages. A major part of Taoist and Chinese Buddhist practice is learning to activate the Dan Tien once again. This may also explain why such abilities are a bit stronger as a child and quickly disappear as one ages but can be awakened again at any time by the proper practice of arts such as ] and ] to expand the ] and spirit. There are many abilities that can be developed in this way — telepathy, prediction, ], ], telekinesis, ] and energetic healing. | |||
==See also== | ==See also== | ||
{{Div col|colwidth=30em}} | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] (retroactive clairvoyance) | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* Retroactive clairvoyance ''aka'' ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
{{div col end}} | |||
==References== | ==References== | ||
{{Reflist|30em}} | |||
<references/> | |||
== |
== Bibliography == | ||
*{{cite book|author=S. A. Jain|title=Reality|date=1992|publisher=Jwalamalini Trust|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=uRIaAAAAMAAJ|quote=Not in Copyright.}} | |||
* by By BelindaGrace. Rockpool Publishing (2007) | |||
* by Spiritual Science Research Foundation | |||
==Further reading== | |||
* by David G. Myers | |||
{{refbegin|40em}} | |||
* by Daniel Finkelstein, '']'', ], ]. | |||
* ] (1981). ''Parapsychology: Science or Magic? A Psychological Perspective''. Pergamon Press. {{ISBN|0-08-025772-0}}. | |||
*'']'' by ], 1929. Preface by ]. | |||
* Willis Dutcher (1922). . Berlin, WI: Heaney Magic. | |||
* explains how time and the 4th dimension work in basics, from a psionic ability user's view, to experience future-sight through Clairvoyance. | |||
* ] (1993). ''How We Know What Isn't So: Fallibility of Human Reason in Everyday Life''. Free Press. {{ISBN|978-0-02-911706-4}}. | |||
* Social Experiment - Case Study | |||
* ] (1988). ''Extrasensory Deception: ESP, Psychics, Shirley MacLaine, Ghosts, UFOs''. Macmillan of Canada. {{ISBN|0-7715-9539-5}}. | |||
*] (1980). . In ''Essays on Mind''. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. {{ISBN|978-0-898-59017-3}}. | |||
* ] (1989). ''The Search for Psychic Power: ESP and Parapsychology Revisited''. Prometheus Books. {{ISBN|0-87975-516-4}}. | |||
* ] (2003). ''Pseudoscience and the Paranormal''. Prometheus Books. {{ISBN|1-57392-979-4}}. | |||
* ]. (2000). '']'' (2nd Edition). Prometheus Books. {{ISBN|1-57392-798-8}}. | |||
* ] (1920). . Chapter "The Subtle Art of Clairvoyance". London: Watts & Co. pp. 93–108. | |||
{{refend}} | |||
==External links== | |||
{{Wikiquote}} | |||
{{Wiktionary|clairvoyance|clairvoyant}} | |||
{{Wikisource1911Enc|clairvoyance}} | |||
* – ] | |||
* – '']'' | |||
* – '']'' | |||
{{Parapsychology}} | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | |||
] | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | |||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
<br> | |||
{{Parapsychology}} |
Latest revision as of 01:27, 18 November 2024
Claimed form of extrasensory perception This article is about the alleged extrasensory ability. For the album, see Clairvoyance (album). For the book, see Clairvoyance (book). "Clairvoyant" redirects here. For other uses, see Clairvoyant (disambiguation).
Clairvoyance (/klɛərˈvɔɪ.əns/; from French clair 'clear' and voyance 'vision') is the claimed ability to acquire information that would be considered impossible to get through scientifically proven sensations, thus classified as extrasensory perception, or "sixth sense". Any person who is claimed to have such ability is said to be a clairvoyant (/klɛərˈvɔɪ.ənt/) ('one who sees clearly').
Claims for the existence of paranormal and psychic abilities such as clairvoyance have not been supported by scientific evidence. Parapsychology explores this possibility, but the existence of the paranormal is not accepted by the scientific community. The scientific community widely considers parapsychology, including the study of clairvoyance, a pseudoscience.
Usage
Pertaining to the ability of clear-sightedness, clairvoyance refers to the paranormal ability to see persons and events that are distant in time or space. It can be divided into roughly three classes: precognition, the ability to perceive or predict future events, retrocognition, the ability to see past events, and remote viewing, the perception of contemporary events happening outside the range of normal perception.
In history and religion
Throughout history, there have been numerous places and times in which people have claimed themselves, or others, to be clairvoyant.
In several religions, stories of certain individuals being able to see things far removed from their immediate sensory perception are commonplace, especially within pagan religions where oracles were used. Prophecy often involved some degree of clairvoyance, especially when future events were predicted. This ability has sometimes been attributed to a higher power rather than to the person performing it.
Christianity
A number of Christian saints were said to be able to see or know things that were far removed from their immediate sensory perception as a kind of gift from God, including Charbel Makhlouf, Padre Pio and Anne Catherine Emmerich in Catholicism and Gabriel Urgebadze, Paisios Eznepidis and John Maximovitch in Orthodoxy. Jesus Christ in the Gospels is also recorded as able to know things far removed from his immediate human perception. Some Christians today also share the same claim.
Jainism
Main article: Jain epistemologyIn Jainism, clairvoyance is regarded as one of the five kinds of knowledge. The beings of hell and heaven (devas) are said to possess clairvoyance by birth. According to Jain text Sarvārthasiddhi, "this kind of knowledge has been called avadhi as it ascertains matter in downward range or knows objects within limits".
Anthroposophy
Rudolf Steiner, famous as a clairvoyant himself, claimed that it is easy for a clairvoyant to confuse their own emotional and spiritual being with the objective spiritual world.
Parapsychology
Early research
The earliest record of somnambulist clairvoyance is credited to the Marquis de Puységur, a follower of Franz Mesmer, who in 1784 was treating a local dull-witted peasant named Victor Race. During treatment, Race reportedly went into a trance and underwent a personality change, becoming fluent and articulate, and giving diagnosis and prescription for his own disease as well as those of others. Clairvoyance was a reported ability of some mediums during the spiritualist period of the late 19th and early 20th centuries, and psychics of many descriptions have claimed clairvoyant ability up to the present day.
Early researchers of clairvoyance included William Gregory, Gustav Pagenstecher, and Rudolf Tischner. Clairvoyance experiments were reported in 1884 by Charles Richet. Playing cards were enclosed in envelopes and a subject under hypnosis attempted to identify them. The subject was reported to have been successful in a series of 133 trials but the results dropped to chance level when performed before a group of scientists in Cambridge. J. M. Peirce and E. C. Pickering reported a similar experiment in which they tested 36 subjects over 23,384 trials. They did not find above chance scores.
Ivor Lloyd Tuckett (1911) and Joseph McCabe (1920) analyzed early cases of clairvoyance and concluded they were best explained by coincidence or fraud. In 1919, the magician P. T. Selbit staged a séance at his flat in Bloomsbury. The spiritualist Arthur Conan Doyle attended and declared the clairvoyance manifestations genuine.
A significant development in clairvoyance research came when J. B. Rhine, a parapsychologist at Duke University, introduced a standard methodology, with a standard statistical approach to analyzing data, as part of his research into extrasensory perception. A number of psychological departments attempted and failed to repeat Rhine's experiments. At Princeton University, W. S. Cox (1936) produced 25,064 trials with 132 subjects in a playing card ESP experiment. Cox concluded: "There is no evidence of extrasensory perception either in the 'average man' or of the group investigated or in any particular individual of that group. The discrepancy between these results and those obtained by Rhine is due either to uncontrollable factors in experimental procedure or to the difference in the subjects." Four other psychological departments failed to replicate Rhine's results. It was revealed that Rhine's experiments contained methodological flaws and procedural errors.
Eileen Garrett was tested by Rhine at Duke University in 1933 with Zener cards. Certain symbols were placed on the cards and sealed in an envelope, and she was asked to guess their contents. She performed poorly and later criticized the tests by claiming the cards lacked a psychic energy called "energy stimulus" and that she could not perform clairvoyance on command. The parapsychologist Samuel Soal and his colleagues tested Garrett in May 1937. Most of the experiments were carried out in the Psychological Laboratory at the University College London. A total of over 12,000 guesses were recorded but Garrett failed to produce above chance level. Soal wrote: "In the case of Mrs. Eileen Garrett we fail to find the slightest confirmation of Dr. J. B. Rhine's remarkable claims relating to her alleged powers of extra-sensory perception. Not only did she fail when I took charge of the experiments, but she failed equally when four other carefully trained experimenters took my place."
Remote viewing
Remote viewing, also known as remote sensing, remote perception, telesthesia and travelling clairvoyance, is the alleged paranormal ability to perceive a remote or hidden target without support of the senses.
A well-known recent study of remote viewing is the US government-funded project at the Stanford Research Institute from the 1970s through the mid-1990s. In 1972, Harold E. Puthoff and Russell Targ initiated a series of human subject studies to determine whether participants (the viewers or percipients) could reliably identify and accurately describe salient features of remote locations (targets). In the early studies, a human sender was typically present at the remote location as part of the experiment protocol. A three-step process was used. First, target conditions to be experienced by the senders were randomly selected. Second, in the viewing step, participants were asked to verbally express or sketch their impressions of the remote scene. Third, in these descriptions were matched by separate judges, as closely as possible, with the intended targets. The term remote viewing was coined to describe this overall process. The first paper by Puthoff and Targ on remote viewing was published in Nature in March 1974; in it, the team reported some degree of remote viewing success. After the publication of these findings, other attempts to replicate the experiments were carried out with remotely linked groups using computer conferencing.
The psychologists David Marks and Richard Kammann attempted to replicate Targ and Puthoff's remote viewing experiments at the Stanford Research Institute. In a series of 35 studies, they could not do so, so they investigated the original experiments' procedure. Marks and Kammann discovered that the notes given to the judges in Targ and Puthoff's experiments contained clues as to which order they were carried out, such as referring to yesterday's two targets, or the date of the session at the top of the page. They concluded that these clues explained the experiment's high hit rates. Marks achieved 100% accuracy without visiting any of the sites but by using cues. James Randi has written that controlled tests by several other researchers, eliminating several sources of cuing and extraneous evidence present in the original tests, produced negative results. Students were also able to solve Puthoff and Targ's locations from the clues inadvertently included in the transcripts.
In 1980, Charles Tart claimed that a rejudging of the transcripts from one of Targ and Puthoff's experiments revealed an above-chance result. Targ and Puthoff again refused to provide copies of the transcripts, and they were not made available for study until July 1985, when it was discovered they still contained sensory cues. Marks and Christopher Scott (1986) wrote: "considering the importance for the remote viewing hypothesis of adequate cue removal, Tart's failure to perform this basic task seems beyond comprehension. As previously concluded, remote viewing has not been demonstrated in the experiments conducted by Puthoff and Targ, only the repeated failure of the investigators to remove sensory cues."
In 1982, Robert G. Jahn, then Dean of the School of Engineering at Princeton University, wrote a comprehensive review of psychic phenomena from an engineering perspective. His paper included numerous references to remote viewing studies at the time. Statistical flaws in his work have been proposed by others in the parapsychological community and the general scientific community.
Scientific reception
According to scientific research, clairvoyance is generally explained as the result of confirmation bias, expectancy bias, fraud, hallucination, self-delusion, sensory leakage, subjective validation, wishful thinking or failures to appreciate the base rate of chance occurrences and not as a paranormal power. Parapsychology is generally regarded by the scientific community as a pseudoscience. In 1988, the US National Research Council concluded "The committee finds no scientific justification from research conducted over a period of 130 years, for the existence of parapsychological phenomena."
Skeptics say that if clairvoyance were a reality, it would have become abundantly clear. They also contend that those who believe in paranormal phenomena do so for merely psychological reasons. According to David G. Myers (Psychology, 8th ed.):
The search for a valid and reliable test of clairvoyance has resulted in thousands of experiments. One controlled procedure has invited 'senders' to telepathically transmit one of four visual images to 'receivers' deprived of sensation in a nearby chamber (Bem & Honorton, 1994). The result? A reported 32 percent accurate response rate, surpassing the chance rate of 25 percent. But follow-up studies have (depending on who was summarizing the results) failed to replicate the phenomenon or produced mixed results (Bem & others, 2001; Milton & Wiseman, 2002; Storm, 2000, 2003).
One skeptic, magician James Randi, had a longstanding offer of U.S. $1 million—"to anyone who proves a genuine psychic power under proper observing conditions" (Randi, 1999). French, Australian, and Indian groups have parallel offers of up to 200,000 euros to anyone with demonstrable paranormal abilities (CFI, 2003). Large as these sums are, the scientific seal of approval would be worth far more to anyone whose claims could be authenticated. To refute those who say there is no ESP, one need only produce a single person who can demonstrate a single, reproducible ESP phenomenon. So far, no such person has emerged. Randi's offer has been publicized for three decades and dozens of people have been tested, sometimes under the scrutiny of an independent panel of judges. Still, nothing. "People's desire to believe in the paranormal is stronger than all the evidence that it does not exist." Susan Blackmore, "Blackmore's first law", 2004.
Clairvoyance is considered a hallucination by mainstream psychiatry.
See also
- Astral projection
- Astral religion
- Aura
- Body of light
- Clairvoyance (book)
- Inner eye
- List of topics characterized as pseudoscience
- Mediumship
- Out-of-body experience
- Photoacoustic effect
- Postcognition
- Postdiction (retroactive clairvoyance)
- Precognition
- Remote viewing
- Scientific skepticism
- Second sight
- Synchronicity
- Third eye
- Thought-Forms (book)
References
- Paul Sédir (1907). Les Miroirs Magiques (PDF). Librairie Générale des Sciences Occultes (3rd ed.). Paris. p. 22. Archived (PDF) from the original on April 3, 2019.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) - "clairvoyance". Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary. Merriam-Webster. Retrieved February 22, 2022. "Clairvoyance - Definition and More from the Free Merriam-Webster Dictionary". Archived from the original on February 27, 2012. Retrieved October 6, 2007.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link) - "clairvoyance". Encyclopedia Britannica. Archived from the original on February 18, 2008. Retrieved October 7, 2007. The ESP entry includes clairvoyance.
{{cite encyclopedia}}
: CS1 maint: postscript (link) - "clairvoyant". Oxford Learners Dictionaries. Retrieved April 8, 2017.
- ^ Carroll, Robert Todd. (2003). "Clairvoyance". Retrieved 2014-04-30.
-
- Bunge, Mario. (1983). Treatise on Basic Philosophy: Volume 6: Epistemology & Methodology II: Understanding the World. Springer. p. 226. ISBN 90-277-1635-8 "Despite being several thousand years old, and having attracted a large number of researchers over the past hundred years, we owe no single firm finding to parapsychology: no hard data on telepathy, clairvoyance, precognition, or psychokinesis."
- Stenger, Victor. (1990). Physics and Psychics: The Search for a World Beyond the Senses. Prometheus Books. p. 166. ISBN 0-87975-575-X "The bottom line is simple: science is based on consensus, and at present a scientific consensus that psychic phenomena exist is still not established."
- Zechmeister, Eugene; Johnson, James. (1992). Critical Thinking: A Functional Approach. Brooks/Cole Pub. Co. p. 115. ISBN 0534165966 "There exists no good scientific evidence for the existence of paranormal phenomena such as ESP. To be acceptable to the scientific community, evidence must be both valid and reliable."
- Hines, Terence. (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. p. 144. ISBN 1-57392-979-4 "It is important to realize that, in one hundred years of parapsychological investigations, there has never been a single adequate demonstration of the reality of any psi phenomenon."
- "Dictionary.com "Pseudoscience"". Dictionary.reference.com. Retrieved September 22, 2012.
- "Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy "Science and Pseudo-Science"". Plato.stanford.edu. September 3, 2008. Retrieved September 22, 2012.
- "Science Needs to Combat Pseudoscience: A Statement by 32 Russian Scientists and Philosophers". Quackwatch.com. July 17, 1998. Retrieved September 22, 2012.
- "International Cultic Studies Association "Science Fiction in Pseudoscience"". Csj.org. Archived from the original on August 12, 2018. Retrieved September 22, 2012.
-
- Gross, Paul R; Levitt, Norman; Lewis, Martin W (1996), The Flight from Science and Reason, New York Academy of Sciences, p. 565, ISBN 978-0801856761,
The overwhelming majority of scientists consider parapsychology, by whatever name, to be pseudoscience.
- Friedlander, Michael W (1998), At the Fringes of Science, Westview Press, p. 119, ISBN 978-0-8133-2200-1,
Parapsychology has failed to gain general scientific acceptance even for its improved methods and claimed successes, and it is still treated with a lopsided ambivalence among the scientific community. Most scientists write it off as pseudoscience unworthy of their time.
- Pigliucci, Massimo; Boudry, Maarten (2013), Philosophy of Pseudoscience: Reconsidering the Demarcation Problem, University Of Chicago Press, p. 158, hdl:1854/LU-3161824, ISBN 978-0-226-05196-3,
Many observers refer to the field as a 'pseudoscience'. When mainstream scientists say that the field of parapsychology is not scientific, they mean that no satisfying naturalistic cause-and-effect explanation for these supposed effects has yet been proposed and that the field's experiments cannot be consistently replicated.
- Gross, Paul R; Levitt, Norman; Lewis, Martin W (1996), The Flight from Science and Reason, New York Academy of Sciences, p. 565, ISBN 978-0801856761,
- Cordón, Luis A. (2005). Popular Psychology: An Encyclopedia. Westport, Conn: Greenwood Press. p. 182. ISBN 978-0-313-32457-4.
The essential problem is that a large portion of the scientific community, including most research psychologists, regards parapsychology as a pseudoscience, due largely to its failure to move beyond null results in the way science usually does. Ordinarily, when experimental evidence fails repeatedly to support a hypothesis, that hypothesis is abandoned. Within parapsychology, however, more than a century of experimentation has failed even to conclusively demonstrate the mere existence of paranormal phenomenon, yet parapsychologists continue to pursue that elusive goal.
- Melton, John. (2001). The Encyclopedia of Occultism & Parapsychology. p. 297. Gale Group, Detroit. ISBN 978-0810385702.
- S. A. Jain 1992, p. 16.
- Steiner, Correspondence and Documents 1901–1925, 1988, p. 9. ISBN 0880102071
- Ruse, Michael (2018). The Problem of War: Darwinism, Christianity, and Their Battle to Understand Human Conflict. Oxford University Press. p. 97. ISBN 978-0-19-086757-7.
- Rudolf Steiner, Errors in Spiritual Investigation: Meeting the Guardian of the Threshold, A Lecture Berlin, March 6, 1913, Bn 62; GA 62; CW 62, Mercury Press, Spring Valley, New York, 1983, https://wn.rsarchive.org/Lectures/19130306p01.html Quote: "He therefore must learn above all else to know himself, so that when he is able to confront a spiritual outer world in the same way as he confronts an objective being he can distinguish himself from what is truth. If he does not learn to delimit himself in this way, he will always confuse that which is only within him, that which is only his subjective experience, with the spiritual world picture; he can never arrive at a real grasp of spiritual reality."
- Rudolf Steiner An Esoteric Cosmology Eighteen Lectures delivered in Paris May 25 to June 14, 1906, Bn 94.1, GA 94, France. St. George Publications, Spring Valley, New York, 1978, IX. The Astral World, https://wn.rsarchive.org/Lectures/GA094/English/SGP1978/19060602p01.html Quote: "Another result of this inverse unraveling of things in the astral world is that it teaches man to know himself. Feelings and passions are expressed by plant and animal forms. When man begins to behold his passions in the astral world he sees them as animal forms. These forms proceed from himself, but he sees them as if they were assailing him. This is because his own being is objectivised—otherwise he could not behold himself. Thus it is only in the astral world that man learns true self knowledge in contemplating the images of his passions in the animal forms which hurl, themselves upon him. A feeling of hatred entertained against another being appears as an attacking demon."
- Taves, Ann. (1999). Fits, Trances, and Visions: Experiencing Religion and Explaining Experience from Wesley to James. Princeton University Press. p. 126. ISBN 0-691-01024-2
- Hyman, Ray. (1985). A Critical Historical Overview of Parapsychology. In Kurtz, Paul. A Skeptic's Handbook of Parapsychology. Prometheus Books. pp. 3–96. ISBN 0-87975-300-5
- Roeckelein, Jon. (2006). Elsevier's Dictionary of Psychological Theories. Elsevier Science. p. 450. ISBN 0-444-51750-2
- Hansel, C. E. M. The Search for a Demonstration of ESP. In Paul Kurtz. (1985). A Skeptic's Handbook of Parapsychology. Prometheus Books. pp. 97–127. ISBN 0-87975-300-5
- McCabe, Joseph. (1920). Is Spiritualism Based On Fraud? The Evidence Given By Sir A. C. Doyle and Others Drastically Examined. Chapter The Subtle Art of Clairvoyance. London: Watts & Co. pp. 93–108
- Tuckett, Ivor Lloyd. (1911). The Evidence for the Supernatural: A Critical Study Made with "Uncommon Sense". Chapter Telepathy and Clairvoyance. K. Paul, Trench, Trübner. pp. 107–142
- Baker, Robert A. (1996). Hidden Memories: Voices and Visions From Within. Prometheus Books. p. 234. ISBN 978-1-57392-094-0
- Christopher, Milbourne. (1996). The Illustrated History of Magic. Greenwood Publishing Group. p. 264. ISBN 978-0-435-07016-8
- Cox, W. S. (1936). "An experiment in ESP". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 19 (4): 437. doi:10.1037/h0054630.
- Jastrow, Joseph. (1938). ESP, House of Cards. The American Scholar. Vol. 8, No. 1. pp. 13–22. "Rhine's results fail to be confirmed. At Colgate University (40, 000 tests, 7 subjects), at Chicago (extensive series on 315 students), at Southern Methodist College (75, 000 tests), at Glasgow, Scotland (6, 650 tests), at London University (105, 000 tests), not a single individual was found who under rigidly conducted experiments could score above chance. At Stanford University it has been convincingly shown that the conditions favorable to the intrusion of subtle errors produce above-chance records which come down to chance when sources of error are eliminated."
- Hansel, C. E. M. The Search for a Demonstration of ESP. In Paul Kurtz. (1985). A Skeptic's Handbook of Parapsychology. Prometheus Books. pp. 105–127. ISBN 0-87975-300-5
- Adam, E. T. (1938). "A summary of some negative experiments". Journal of Parapsychology. 2: 232–236.
- Crumbaugh, J. C. (1938). An experimental study of extra-sensory perception. Masters thesis. Southern Methodist University.
- Heinlein, C. P; Heinlein, J. H. (1938). "Critique of the premises of statistical methodology of parapsychology". Journal of Parapsychology. 5: 135–148. doi:10.1080/00223980.1938.9917558.
- Willoughby, R. R. (1938). Further card-guessing experiments. Journal of Psychology 18: 3–13.
- Gulliksen, Harold. (1938). Extra-Sensory Perception: What Is It?. American Journal of Sociology. Vol. 43, No. 4. pp. 623–634. "Investigating Rhine's methods, we find that his mathematical methods are wrong and that the effect of this error would in some cases be negligible and in others very marked. We find that many of his experiments were set up in a manner which would tend to increase, instead of to diminish, the possibility of systematic clerical errors; and lastly, that the ESP cards can be read from the back."
- Wynn, Charles; Wiggins, Arthur. (2001). Quantum Leaps in the Wrong Direction: Where Real Science Ends...and Pseudoscience Begins. Joseph Henry Press. p. 156. ISBN 978-0-309-07309-7 "In 1940, Rhine coauthored a book, Extrasensory Perception After Sixty Years in which he suggested that something more than mere guess work was involved in his experiments. He was right! It is now known that the experiments conducted in his laboratory contained serious methodological flaws. Tests often took place with minimal or no screening between the subject and the person administering the test. Subjects could see the backs of cards that were later discovered to be so cheaply printed that a faint outline of the symbol could be seen. Furthermore, in face-to-face tests, subjects could see card faces reflected in the tester's eyeglasses or cornea. They were even able to (consciously or unconsciously) pick up clues from the tester's facial expression and voice inflection. In addition, an observant subject could identify the cards by certain irregularities like warped edges, spots on the backs, or design imperfections."
- Hines, Terence. (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. p. 122. ISBN 978-1573929790 "The procedural errors in the Rhine experiments have been extremely damaging to his claims to have demonstrated the existence of ESP. Equally damaging has been the fact that the results have not replicated when the experiments have been conducted in other laboratories."
- Hazelgrove, Jenny. (2000). Spiritualism and British Society Between the Wars. Manchester University Press. p. 204. ISBN 978-0719055591
- Russell, A. S; Benn, John Andrews. (1938). Discovery the Popular Journal of Knowledge. Cambridge University Press. pp. 305–306
- Soal, Samuel. A Repetition of Dr. Rhine's work with Mrs. Eileen Garrett. Proc. S.P.R. Vol. XLII. pp. 84–85. Also quoted in Antony Flew. (1955). A New Approach To Psychical Research. Watts & Co. pp. 90–92.
- Blom, Jan (2009). A dictionary of hallucinations. New York: Springer. p. 451. ISBN 978-1-4419-1222-0. OCLC 618047801.
- Targ, Russel; Puthoff, Harold (1974). "Information transmission under conditions of sensory shielding". Nature. 251 (5476): 602–607. Bibcode:1974Natur.251..602T. doi:10.1038/251602a0. PMID 4423858. S2CID 4152651.
- Hastings, A.C.; Hurt, D.B. (October 1976). "A confirmatory remote viewing experiment in a group setting". Proceedings of the IEEE. 64 (10): 1544–1545. doi:10.1109/PROC.1976.10369. S2CID 36582119.
- Whitson, T.W.; Bogart, D.N.; Palmer, J.; Tart, C.T. (October 1976). "Preliminary experiments in group 'Remote viewing'". Proceedings of the IEEE. 64 (10): 1550–1551. doi:10.1109/PROC.1976.10371. S2CID 27302086.
- Vallee, J.; Hastings, A.C.; Askevold, G. (October 1976). "Remote viewing experiments through computer conferencing". Proceedings of the IEEE. 64 (10): 1551–1552. doi:10.1109/PROC.1976.10372. S2CID 24096224.
- Marks, David; Kammann, Richard (1978). "Information transmission in remote viewing experiments". Nature. 274 (5672): 680–681. Bibcode:1978Natur.274..680M. doi:10.1038/274680a0. S2CID 4249968.
- Marks, David (1981). "Sensory cues invalidate remote viewing experiments". Nature. 292 (5819): 177. Bibcode:1981Natur.292..177M. doi:10.1038/292177a0. PMID 7242682. S2CID 4326382.
- Bridgstock, Martin (2009). Beyond belief: skepticism, science and the paranormal. Cambridge Port Melbourne, Vic: Cambridge University Press. p. 106. ISBN 978-0-521-75893-2. OCLC 652432050.
The explanation used by Marks and Kammann clearly involves the use of Occam's razor. Marks and Kammann argued that the 'cues'—clues to the order in which sites had been visited—provided sufficient information for the results, without any recourse to extrasensory perception. Indeed Marks himself was able to achieve 100 percent accuracy in allocating some transcripts to sites without visiting any of the sites himself, purely on the ground basis of the cues. From Occam's razor, it follows that if a straightforward natural explanation exists, there is no need for the spectacular paranormal explanation: Targ and Puthoff's claims are not justified.
- Randi, James (n.d.) . "Remote Viewing". An Encyclopedia of Claims, Frauds, and Hoaxes of the Occult and Supernatural. Digital adaptation by Gilles-Maurice de Schryver. (Online ed.). James Randi Educational Foundation . Retrieved January 26, 2022.
- Tart, Charles; Puthoff, Harold; Targ, Russell (1980). "Information Transmission in Remote Viewing Experiments". Nature. 284 (5752): 191. Bibcode:1980Natur.284..191T. doi:10.1038/284191a0. PMID 7360248. S2CID 4326363.
- Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the paranormal. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. p. 136. ISBN 978-1-57392-979-0. OCLC 50124260.
- Marks, David; Scott, Christopher (1986). "Remote Viewing Exposed". Nature. 319 (6053): 444. Bibcode:1986Natur.319..444M. doi:10.1038/319444a0. PMID 3945330. S2CID 13642580.
- Jahn, R.G. (February 1982). "The persistent paradox of psychic phenomena: An engineering perspective" (PDF). Proceedings of the IEEE. 70 (2): 136–170. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.15.8760. doi:10.1109/PROC.1982.12260. S2CID 31434794. Archived (PDF) from the original on May 14, 2011.
- Stanley Jeffers (May–June 2006). "The PEAR proposition: Fact or fallacy?". Skeptical Inquirer. 30 (3). Archived from the original on February 1, 2014. Retrieved January 24, 2014.
- George P. Hansen. "Princeton Remote-Viewing Experiments (PEAR) – A Critique". Tricksterbook.com. Retrieved April 6, 2014.
- Rawcliffe, Donovan. (1988). Occult and Supernatural Phenomena. Dover Publications. pp. 367–463. ISBN 0-486-20503-7
- Reed, Graham. (1988). The Psychology of Anomalous Experience: A Cognitive Approach. Prometheus Books. ISBN 0-87975-435-4
- Zusne, Leonard; Jones, Warren. (1989). Anomalistic Psychology: A Study of Magical Thinking. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 152–168. ISBN 0-8058-0508-7
- Friedlander, Michael W. (1998). At the Fringes of Science. Westview Press. p. 119. ISBN 0-8133-2200-6 "Parapsychology has failed to gain general scientific acceptance even for its improved methods and claimed successes, and it is still treated with a lopsided ambivalence among the scientific community. Most scientists write it off as pseudoscience unworthy of their time."
- Pigliucci, Massimo; Boudry, Maarten. (2013). Philosophy of Pseudoscience: Reconsidering the Demarcation Problem. University of Chicago Press p. 158. ISBN 978-0-226-05196-3 "Many observers refer to the field as a "pseudoscience". When mainstream scientists say that the field of parapsychology is not scientific, they mean that no satisfying naturalistic cause-and-effect explanation for these supposed effects has yet been proposed and that the field's experiments cannot be consistently replicated."
- Gilovich, Thomas. (1993). How We Know What Isn't So: The Fallibility of Human Reason in Everyday Life. Free Press. p. 160. ISBN 978-0-02-911706-4
- French, Chis; Wilson, Krissy. (2007). Cognitive Factors Underlying Paranormal Beliefs and Experiences. In Sala, Sergio. Tall Tales About the Mind and Brain: Separating Fact From Fiction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 3–22. ISBN 978-0198568773
- Myers, David. (2006). Psychology. Worth Publishers; 8th edition. ISBN 978-0716764281
- Blom, Jan Dirk (2010). A Dictionary of Hallucinations. New York, Dordrecht, Heidelberg, London: Springer Science+Business Media, LLC. p. 99. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-1223-7. ISBN 978-1-4419-1222-0. Retrieved January 11, 2012.
Clairvoyance
Also known as lucidity, telesthesia, and cryptestesia. Clairvoyance is French for seeing clearly. The term is used in the parapsychological literature to denote a * visual or * compound hallucination attributable to a metaphysical source. It is therefore interpreted as * telepathic, * veridical or at least * coincidental hallucination.
Reference
Guily, R.E. (1991) Harper's encyclopedia of mystical and paranormal experience. New York: Castle Books.
Bibliography
Further reading
- James Alcock (1981). Parapsychology: Science or Magic? A Psychological Perspective. Pergamon Press. ISBN 0-08-025772-0.
- Willis Dutcher (1922). On the Other Side of the Footlights: An Expose of Routines, Apparatus and Deceptions Resorted to by Mediums, Clairvoyants, Fortune Tellers and Crystal Gazers in Deluding the Public. Berlin, WI: Heaney Magic.
- Thomas Gilovich (1993). How We Know What Isn't So: Fallibility of Human Reason in Everyday Life. Free Press. ISBN 978-0-02-911706-4.
- Henry Gordon (1988). Extrasensory Deception: ESP, Psychics, Shirley MacLaine, Ghosts, UFOs. Macmillan of Canada. ISBN 0-7715-9539-5.
- Donald Hebb (1980). Extrasensory Perception: A Problem. In Essays on Mind. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 978-0-898-59017-3.
- C. E. M. Hansel (1989). The Search for Psychic Power: ESP and Parapsychology Revisited. Prometheus Books. ISBN 0-87975-516-4.
- Terence Hines (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. ISBN 1-57392-979-4.
- David Marks. (2000). The Psychology of the Psychic (2nd Edition). Prometheus Books. ISBN 1-57392-798-8.
- Joseph McCabe (1920). Is Spiritualism Based On Fraud? The Evidence Given By Sir A. C. Doyle and Others Drastically Examined. Chapter "The Subtle Art of Clairvoyance". London: Watts & Co. pp. 93–108.
External links
- Springer Psychic: "A Study in 'Clairvoyance'" – Joe Nickell
- "Debunking the Sixth Sense" – Science Daily
- "Clairvoyance" – The Skeptic's Dictionary