Misplaced Pages

Mixed language: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 20:32, 21 July 2008 edit89.242.104.114 (talk) Undid revision 227064382 by Angr (talk)See talk.← Previous edit Latest revision as of 22:37, 15 December 2024 edit undoDl2000 (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Extended confirmed users, New page reviewers, Pending changes reviewers, Rollbackers820,488 editsm ordinals not normally used in dates (WP:DATESNO); fix quot; tidy 
(527 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Language that arises amongst a bilingual group}}
A '''mixed language''' is a ] that arises through the fusion of two source languages, normally in situations of thorough ]. Although the concept is frequently encountered in historical linguistics from the early twentieth century, attested cases of language mixture, as opposed to ], ], or simple borrowing, are quite rare. A genuine mixed language may mark the appearance of a new ethnic or cultural group, such as the ''].'' The fusion of more than two languages is not attested.
{{Multiple issues|
{{more citations needed|date=December 2018}}
{{POV|date=January 2018}}
}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=December 2024}}
A '''mixed language''', also referred to as a '''hybrid language''', '''contact language''', or '''fusion language''', is a language that arises among a bilingual group combining aspects of two or more languages but not clearly deriving primarily from any single language.<ref name="Matras 2008">{{cite book | title=The Mixed Language Debate: Theoretical and Empirical Advances | year=2008 | editor=Matras, Yaron |editor2=Bakker, Peter | url=https://books.google.com/books?id=H_8rdQ8yLzIC | page=191| publisher=Walter de Gruyter | isbn=9783110197242 }}</ref> It differs from a ] or ] in that, whereas creoles/pidgins arise where speakers of many languages acquire a common language, a mixed language typically arises in a population that is fluent in both of the source languages.

Because all languages show some degree of mixing<ref>Zuckermann (2009) p. 48, citing Hjelmslev (1938) and Schuchardt (1884).</ref> by virtue of containing ], it is a matter of controversy whether the concept of a mixed language can meaningfully be distinguished from the type of contact and borrowing seen in all languages.<ref name="arends-1994">Arends et al. 1994</ref><ref>{{cite journal | url=http://languagecontact.humanities.manchester.ac.uk/YM/downloads/Matras,%20Y.%20(2000)%20Mixed%20languages%20a%20functional-communicative%20approach.pdf | title=Mixed languages: a functional–communicative approach | author=Yaron Matras | journal=Bilingualism: Language and Cognition | year=2000 | volume=3 | issue=2 | pages=79–99 | doi=10.1017/S1366728900000213 | access-date=2017-05-15 | archive-date=2021-02-20 | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210220013822/http://languagecontact.humanities.manchester.ac.uk/YM/downloads/Matras,%20Y.%20%282000%29%20Mixed%20languages%20a%20functional-communicative%20approach.pdf | url-status=dead }}</ref> Scholars debate to what extent language mixture can be distinguished from other mechanisms such as ], ], or lexical borrowing.<ref>According to Google n-gram, the German term {{lang|de|Mischsprache}} is first attested in 1832, and attested in English since 1909.</ref>


==Definitions== ==Definitions==
Other terms used in ] for the concept of a mixed language include ''hybrid language'', ''contact language'', and ''fusion language''; in older usage, 'jargon' was sometimes used in this sense.<ref>"jargon, n.1." OED Online. Oxford University Press, March 2015. Web. 4 May 2015, sense 5.</ref> In some linguists' usage, ] and ]s are types of mixed languages, whereas in others' usage, creoles and pidgins are merely among the kinds of language that might become full-fledged mixed languages.
A '''mixed language''' differs from a ] in that the speakers developing the language are fluent, even native, speakers of both languages, whereas a pidgin develops when groups of people with little knowledge of each other's languages come into contact and have need of a basic communication system, as for trade, but do not have enough contact to learn each other's language.


Thomason (1995) classifies mixed languages into two categories: Category 1 languages exhibit "heavy influence from the dominant group's language in all aspects of structure and grammar as well as lexicon" (Winford 171). Category 2 languages show a "categorial specificity of the structural borrowing" or a uniform borrowing of specific categories (Winford).{{citation needed|date=November 2013}}
In a mixed language both source languages are clearly identifiable. This differs from a ], which generally has one identifiable parent in addition to diverse input which can not be traced to any particular language. While creoles tend to have drastically simplified ], mixed languages often retain the inflectional complexities of both parent languages.


Mixed language and intertwined language are seemingly interchangeable terms for some researchers. Some use the term "intertwining" instead of "mixing" because the former implies "mixture of two systems which are not necessarily the same order" nor does it suggest "replacement of the either the lexicon or of the grammatical system", unlike ], massive grammatical replacement, and re-]. The grammar of a mixed language typically comes from a language well known to first-generation speakers, which Arends claims is the language spoken by the mother. This is because of the close relationship between mother and child and the likelihood that the language is spoken by the community at large.{{citation needed|date=November 2013}}
==Code-switching==
Finally, a mixed language differs from ], such as ], in that speakers do not need to know the source languages. The fusion of the source languages is fixed in the grammar and vocabulary, not left to the speaker. However, it is believed that mixed languages evolve from persistent code-switching, with younger generations picking up the code-switching, but not necessarily the source languages that generated it.


Arends et al. classify an intertwined language as a language that "has lexical morphemes from one language and grammatical morphemes from another". This definition does not include ], which combines French lexical items in specific contexts, but still utilizes Cree lexical and grammatical items.<ref name="arends-1994"/>
Most ] language names, such as ] and ], are not mixed languages, or even code-switching, but ] of a language (here ] and ]) characterized by large numbers of ]s from a second language (here ] and ]). English developed from such a situation, ] with a large number of French borrowings, but is not considered a mixed language.


Yaron Matras distinguishes between three types of models for mixed language: "language maintenance and ], unique and predetermined processes ("intertwining"), and "conventionalisation of language mixing patterns". The first model involves the use of one language for heavy substitutions of entire grammatical paradigms or morphology of another language. This is because a speech community will not adopt a newer dominant language, and so adapt their language with grammatical material from the dominant language. Bakker (1997) argues that mixed languages result from mixed populations. Languages "intertwine", in that the morphosyntax (provided by female native speakers) mixes with the lexicon of another language (spoken by men, often in a colonialist context). This appears to have been the case with Michif, where European men and Cree, Nakota, and Ojibwe women had offspring who learned a mixture of French and Cree. The third model "assumes a gradual loss of the conversational function of language alternation as a means of expressing contrast". In other words, language no longer becomes a means of differentiation between two speech communities as a result of language mixing.<ref name="journals.cambridge.org">Matras, Yaron, , "Bilingualism: Language and Cognition / Volume 3 / Issue 2 / August 2000 / p. 79 - 99</ref>
==Mixed languages==
Genuine mixed languages include:
*] is considered by many linguists to be a mixture of ] and ] syntax, grammar, and vocabulary, with influences from ], ], ], ], ], and ]. <ref></ref><ref></ref><ref>Journal of Semitic Studies 1958 3(1):58-79; doi:10.1093/jss/3.1.58</ref><ref></ref><ref>The Structure of Maltese by Joseph Aquilina
Journal of the American Oriental Society, Vol. 80, No. 3 (Jul. - Sep., 1960), pp. 267-268</ref><ref></ref><ref></ref><ref></ref><ref></ref><ref></ref>
*], a mixture of ] and ], where the nouns and adjectives tend to be French (including agreement), and the ] verbs are entirely Cree. There are two simultaneous gender systems, French masculine/feminine as well as Cree animate/inanimate, and the Cree ] (fourth ]).
*], a mixture of ] and ], which retains Aleut verbs but has replaced most of the inflectional endings with their Russian equivalents.
*], comprising mostly Greek root words, but with many ] grammatical endings and Turkish ], and no gender.
*] or Ma’a, an inherited ] vocabulary with a borrowed ] morphology.
*], an inherited ] grammar and phonology with a borrowed Spanish lexicon.


Lexical reorientation, according to Matras, is defined as "the conscious shifting of the linguistic field that is responsible for encoding meaning or conceptual representations away from the language in which linguistic interaction is normally managed, organised, and processed: speakers adopt in a sense one linguistic system to express lexical meaning (or symbols, in the Buhlerian sense of the term) and another to organize the relations among lexical symbols, as well as within sentences, utterances, and interaction. The result is a split, by source language, between lexicon and grammar."<ref name="journals.cambridge.org"/>
The histories of these languages differ. Michif and Mednyj Aleut appear to have risen through the mixture and intermarriage of two bilingual peoples, French with Cree and Russian with Aleut. Cappadocian Greek and Media Lengua, on the other hand, appear to have arisen as minority languages (Greek and Quechua) shifted under the influence of the surrounding majority language (Turkish and Spanish). While the Greek and Quechua were bilingual in Turkish and Spanish, the reverse was not true. The history of Mbugu is not known.


==Differentiation with other language mixtures==
''Possible'' examples include:

*], a mixture of ] and ]
A mixed language differs from ], ] and ] in very fundamental ways. In most cases, mixed language speakers are fluent, even native, speakers of both languages; however, speakers of Michif (a verb-noun or V-N mixed language) are unique in that many are not fluent in both of the source languages.<ref>{{Cite book|title=Pidgins, Creoles and Mixed Languages|volume = 48|last=Velupillai|first=Viveka|date=2015-03-24|publisher=John Benjamins Publishing Company|isbn=9789027252715|series=Creole Language Library|location=Amsterdam|doi = 10.1075/cll.48}}</ref> Pidgins, on the other hand, develop in a situation, usually in the context of trade, where speakers of two (or more) different languages come into contact and need to find some way to communicate with each other. Creoles develop when a pidgin language becomes a first language for young speakers. While creoles tend to have drastically simplified ], mixed languages often retain the inflectional complexities of one, or both, of the parent languages. For instance, Michif retains the complexities of its Cree verb-phrases and its French noun-phrases.<ref>{{Cite book|title=Pidgins, Creoles and Mixed Languages|last=Viveka.|first=Velupillai|date=2015|publisher=John Benjamins Publishing Company|oclc=942975077}}</ref>
*] (a mixture of ] and ]).

*] (a mixture of ], various local German and French dialects, some ], and a few ] words.)
It also differs from a language that has undergone heavy borrowing, such as Korean, Japanese, and Vietnamese from Chinese (see ]), English from French, or ] from Sicilian/Italian. In these cases, despite the heavy borrowing, the grammar and basic words of the borrowing language remain relatively unchanged, with the borrowed words confined mainly to more abstract or foreign concepts, and any complex morphology remains that of the host language rather than being borrowed along with the borrowed word. In the case of Maltese, for example, if verbs borrowed from Italian were inflected using Italian inflectional rules rather than Arabic-derived ones, then Maltese would be a candidate for being a mixed language.
*], mixture of ] and ], Spanish verbs are changed to match Guaraní ] and conjugated following Guaraní patterns.

*] (Portuguese/Spanish).
Finally, a mixed language differs from ], such as ] or ], in that, once it has developed, the fusion of the source languages is fixed in the grammar and vocabulary, and speakers do not need to know the source languages in order to speak it. But, linguists believe that mixed languages evolve from persistent code-switching, with younger generations picking up the code-switching, but not necessarily the source languages that generated it.{{citation needed|date=November 2013}}
**] (Portuguese/Spanish).

*] (a mixture of ] and ]).
Languages such as ] and ] are not mixed languages, or even examples of code-switching, but ] of a language (here ] and ]), characterized by large numbers of ]s from a second language (here ] and ]).{{Citation needed|date=December 2022|reason=Other sources on the subject say that Angloromani is a language, not sure where this came from.}} ] (the immediate fore-runner of Modern English) developed from such a situation, incorporating many ] borrowings into ], but it is not considered a mixed language.<ref>{{cite web|title=Middle English Chapter 5. |url=https://www.csun.edu/~sk36711/WWW/engl400/me1.pdf}}</ref>
*] (a mixture of ] and ]).

*] (a mixture of ] and ]).
==Proposed examples==

===Michif===
{{See also|Michif}}

Michif derives nouns, numerals, definite/indefinite articles, possessive pronouns, some adverbs and adjectives from ], while it derives demonstratives (in/animate), question words, verbs (in/animacy agreement with the subject/object), and some adverbs/verb-like adjectives from ].<ref name="arends-1994"/> The Cree components of Michif generally remain grammatically intact, while the French lexicon and grammar is restricted to noun phrases where nouns occur with a French possessive element or article (i.e. in/definite, masculine/feminine, singular/plural).<ref name="silva-corvalan-1997">Silva-Corvalán 1997</ref><ref name="thomason-1997">Thomason 1997</ref> Further, many speakers of Michif are able to identify the French and Cree components of a given sentence, likely from the phonological and morphological features of words. Although the phonological systems of both French and Cree are generally independent in Michif, there is convergence in 1) mid-vowel raising, 2) sibilant harmony, 3) vowel length (e.g. French vowel pairs / and / differ in length as in Cree), and 4) instances where the three nasal vowels /æ̃/, /ũ/, and /ĩ/ occur in the Cree components, although this last point of convergence may be due to Ojibwe influence.<ref name="thomason-1997"/> Scholars propose that, in the ] multilingual community, Michif emerged as a need to symbolize a new social identity.<ref name="silva-corvalan-1997"/> The first unambiguous mention of Michif dates to the 1930s.<ref name="thomason-1997"/>

The Métis of St. Laurent, a tribe of indigenous people in Canada, were made to feel their language was a sign of inferiority by nuns, priests, and other missionaries who insisted that the Metis switch to Standard Canadian French. Because missionaries stigmatized Michif French as an inferior, "bastardized" form of Canadian French, the Métis began to develop a sense of inferiority and shame which they associated with speaking Michif. Although Michif may have arisen as a way for Métis people to identify themselves, it became taboo to speak Michif inter-ethnically.<ref name=GL >Lavalle, Guy http://iportal.usask.ca/docs/Native_studies_review/v7/issue1/pp81-93.pdf</ref>

In an attempt to make students unlearn Michif French, some nuns used a "token-system" in which each student was given ten tokens each week, and for every use of Michif French, a student would have to surrender a token. Students with the most tokens were rewarded with a prize. Overall, this system did not work.<ref name=GL />

===Mednyj Aleut===
{{See also|Mednyj Aleut}}

Mednyj Aleut is identified as a mixed language composed of mostly intact systematic components from two typologically and genetically unrelated languages: ] and ]. This mixed language's grammar and lexicon are both largely Aleut in origin, while the finite verb morphology, a whole grammatical subsystem, is primarily of Russian origin. Nonetheless, there are some syntactic patterns with Russian influence and some Aleut features in the finite verb complex such as, 1) a topic-number agreement pattern, 2) Aleut pronouns with unaccusatives, 3) the Aleut agglutinative tense + number + person/number pattern in one of two alternative past-tense forms. Scholars hypothesize that due to the elaborate Russian and Aleut components of Mednyj Aleut, the Aleut/Russian creoles in which the mixed language arose must have been fluent bilinguals of Aleut and Russian and, therefore, not a pidgin language—that is, "imperfect learning" is usually a feature in the emergence of a pidgin. Furthermore, some code-switching and deliberate decisions likely served as mechanisms for the development of Mednyj Aleut and it is possible that these were motivated by a need for a language that reflected the community's new group identity.<ref name="thomason-1997"/>

===Ma'a===
{{See also|Mbugu}}
Ma’a has a ] basic vocabulary and a primarily ] grammatical structure. The language also shares some phonological units with languages in the Cushitic phylum (e.g. the voiceless lateral fricative, the voiceless glottal stop, and the voiceless velar fricative that do not occur in Bantu), as well as syntactic structures, derivational processes, and a feature of inflectional morphology. However, few productive non-lexical structures in Ma’a appear derived from Cushitic. ] therefore argues for a classification of Ma’a as a mixed language since it does not have enough Cushitic grammar to be genetically related to the Cushitic language. By contrast, Ma’a has a productive set of inflectional structures derived from Bantu. Ma’a also demonstrates phonological structures derived from Bantu—for instance, the prenasalized voiced stops /ᵐb ⁿd ᶮɟ ᵑg/, phonemic tones, the absence of pharyngeal fricatives, labialized dorsal stops, ejective and retroflex stops, and final consonants— as well as noun classification, number category, and verb morphology patterns of Bantu. Syntactic and derivational patterns in Ma’a vary between Cushitic and Bantu origins—some Ma’a constructions used, such as genitive and copula constructions, are both from Cushitic and Bantu. These observations, in view of additional ] cases like Cappadocian Greek, Anglo-Romani, and Mednyj Aleut, suggest that Ma'a arose as a product of massive interference from a Bantu language via intense cultural pressure on a Cushitic-speaking community.<ref>Thomason, Sarah Grey. GENETIC RELATIONSHIP AND THE CASE OF MA'A (MBUGU), Studies in African Linguistics. 14.2. University of Pittsburgh, 1983. Web. http://elanguage.net/journals/sal/article/view/1138/1154.</ref>

===Media Lengua===
{{Main|Media Lengua}}

Media Lengua (roughly translated to "half language" or "in-between language"), also known as ''Chaupi-shimi'', ''Chaupi-lengua'', ''Chaupi-Quichua'', ''Quichuañol'', ''Chapu-shimi'' or ''llanga-shimi'',<ref group=nb>''Llanga-shimi'' is typically a derogatory term used by speakers of Quichua to describe their language. However, it also appears to describe Media Lengua in the Imbabura Communities. It is believed that the term was introduced by ] school teachers to further discredit the indigenous populations</ref><ref name="Pallares">Pallares, A. (2002). From peasant struggles to Indian resistance: the Ecuadorian Andes in the late twentieth century. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press.</ref> is a mixed language that consists of Spanish ] and ]ian ] grammar, most conspicuously in its ]. In terms of vocabulary, almost all ]s (89%<ref name="Stewart">Stewart, Jesse (2011). . (thesis)</ref><ref name="Muysken1997">Muysken, Pieter (1997). "Media Lengua", in Thomason, Sarah G. ''Contact languages: a wider perspective'' Amsterdam: John Benjamins (pp. 365-426)</ref>), including ], are of Spanish origin and appear to conform to Quichua ]. Media Lengua is one of the few widely acknowledged examples of a "bilingual mixed language" in both the conventional and narrow ] sense because of its split between roots and suffixes.<ref name="Backus">Backus, Ad. 2003. Can a mixed language be conventionalised alternational codeswitching? in Matras & Bakker (eds) The Mixed Language Debate: theoretical and empirical advances Mouton de Gruyter Berlin: 237-/270.</ref><ref name="McConvell and Meakins">McConvell, Patrick, and Felicity Meakins. 2005. Gurindji Kriol: A Mixed Language Emerges from Code-switching. Quatro Fonologias Quechuas, 25(1), 9-30.</ref> Such extreme and systematic borrowing is only rarely attested, and Media Lengua is not typically described as a variety of either Quichua or Spanish. Arends et al. list two languages subsumed under the name ''Media Lengua'': Salcedo Media Lengua and Media Lengua of Saraguro.<ref name="arends-1994"/> The northern variety of Media Lengua, found in the province of ], is commonly referred to as Imbabura Media Lengua<ref name="Gómez-Rendón2008">Gómez-Rendón, J. A. (2008). Mestizaje lingüístico en los Andes: génesis y estructura de una lengua mixta (1era. ed.). Quito, Ecuador: Abya-Yala.</ref><ref name="Gómez-Rendón">Gómez-Rendón, J. (2005). La Media Lengua de Imbabura. Encuentros conflictos bilingüismo contacto de lenguas en el mundo andino (pp. 39-58). Madrid: Iberoamericana.</ref> and more specifically, the dialect varieties within the province are known as Pijal Media Lengua and Anglas Media Lengua.<ref name="Stewart"/>

Scholars indicate that Media Lengua arose largely via relexification mechanisms.<ref name="matras-bakker-2003">Matras & Bakker 2003</ref> Pieter Muysken suggests that the social context in which the language emerged as an intralanguage involved a presence of "acculturated Indians" that neither identified with traditional, rural Quechua nor with urban Spanish cultures. This is an instance of a language developing from a need for "ethnic self-identification".<ref name="thomason-1997"/>

===Light Warlpiri===
{{See also|Light Warlpiri}}

Light Warlpiri, seen as a form of Warlpiri by speakers, derives verbs and verbal morphology largely from Australian ], while nouns are largely from ] and English and nominal morphology from Warlpiri. Light Warlpiri likely developed as an intralanguage via ] between Warlpiri and either Kriol or English. This code-mixing conventionalized into Light Warlpiri, which is now learned by Lajamanu children as a first language, along with Warlpiri, although Light Warlpiri is often produced first and used in daily interactions with younger speakers and adults within the ] community. Light Warlpiri is considered a new language for several reasons: 1) Light Warlpiri speakers use an auxiliary verb-system that older Warlpiri speakers do not while code-mixing, 2) elements are distributed differently in Light Warlpiri than in code-mixing varieties of older Warlpiri speakers, 3) Light Warlpiri is a native language, which indicates stability of the language, and 4) grammatical structures and lexical items from each source language occur consistently in Light Warlpiri.<ref>O'Shannessy 2005</ref>

===Gurindji Kriol===
{{Main|Gurindji Kriol language}}

Gurindji Kriol exhibits a structural split between the noun phrase and verb phrase, with Gurindji contributing the noun structure including case-marking, and the verb structure including TAM (]) auxiliaries coming from Kriol. In this respect, Gurindji Kriol is classified as a verb-noun (V-N) mixed language. Other examples of V-N mixed languages include ] and ]. The maintenance of Gurindji within the mixed language can be seen as the perpetuation of Aboriginal identity under massive and continuing cultural incursion.

===Cappadocian Greek and Cypriot Arabic===
{{See also|Cappadocian Greek|Languages of Cyprus}}

Both ] and ] are cases of extreme borrowing—the former from ] and the latter from ]. The remaining Greek dialects of ] display borrowing of vocabulary, function words, derivational morphology, and some borrowed nominal and verbal inflectional morphology from Turkish. Cypriot Arabic largely shows borrowing of vocabulary, and consequently Greek morphosyntax.<ref name="matras-bakker-2003"/> Both Cappadocian Greek and Cypriot Arabic (as well as Ma'a) differ socially from Michif and Mednyj Aleut because they have evolved out of intense language contact, extensive bilingualism, and a strong pressure for speakers to shift to the dominant language. Nonetheless, neither language has an entire grammar and lexicon that is derived from a single historical source and in each case the linguistic group achieves fluent bilingualism. The social context in which they arose largely distinguishes them from pidgins and creoles and, for some scholars, identifies them closely with mixed languages.<ref name="thomason-kaufman-1988"/>

===Kaqchikel-K'iche' Mayan language===
{{main|Cauque Mayan language}}
The Kaqchikel-K'iche' Mixed Language, also known as the Cauqué Mixed Language or Cauqué Mayan, is spoken in the aldea of Santa María Cauqué, Santiago Sacatepéquez, Department of Sacatepéquez in Guatemala. A 1998 study by the Summer Institute of Linguistics (SIL) estimated speaker population at 2,000.<ref name="ethnologue.com">"Kaqchikel-K'iche' Mixed Language." Lewis, M. Paul, Gary F. Simons, and Charles D. Fennig (eds.). 2016. Ethnologue: Languages of the World, Nineteenth edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL International. Web. https://www.ethnologue.com/language/ckz.</ref> While the language's grammatical base is from K'iche', its lexicon is supplied by Kaqchikel.

===Other possible mixed languages===
*], a freeform mixture of ] and ] spoken in rural areas of ].
*], a mixture of ] and ] spoken in the ] of ], ].
*], a mix of ] and ].<ref>{{cite book|last=Long|first=Daniel|title=English on the Bonin (Ogasawara) Islands|year=2007|publisher=Duke University Press|isbn=978-0-8223-6671-3}}</ref>
*], or ''Tagdal'', a ] base with a majority-] vocabulary, sometimes considered a mixed language.<ref>{{cite book |last=Benítez-Torres |first=Carlos M. |year=2009 |chapter=Inflectional vs. Derivational Morphology in Tagdal: A Mixed Language |editor1=Masangu Matondo |editor2=Fiona Mc Laughlin |editor3=Eric Potsdam |title=Selected Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference on African Linguistics |location=Somerville |pages=69–83 |publisher=Cascadilla Proceedings Project |chapter-url=http://www.lingref.com/cpp/acal/38/paper2136.pdf}}</ref>
*], a mixture of ] and ] that involves incorporating elements of Spanish grammar and vocabulary into Guaraní.
*] and ], important historical languages spoken in ], composed mainly of Amerindian (predominantly ]) lexicon and ]. {{Citation needed|date=December 2018}}
*], a combination of ] and ].
*], mixing ] and ] elements.<ref>Wurm, Mühlhäusler, & Tryon, ''Atlas of languages of intercultural communication in the Pacific, Asia and the Americas'', 1996:682.</ref>
*], ] grammar, pronunciation, pragmatics, loanwords and substrate and German vocabulary. {{Citation needed|date=December 2018}}
*] such as ], which derives most of its lexicon from ] but uses ] grammar and syntax.{{citation needed|date=November 2013}}
*], Danish grammar and semantics with German vocabulary. {{Citation needed|date=December 2018}}
*], a mixture of ] and Old Rapa.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Walworth |first=Mary E. |date=2017a |title=Reo Rapa: A Polynesian Contact Language |journal=Journal of Language Contact |volume=10 |issue=1 |pages=98–141 |doi=10.1163/19552629-01001006 |doi-access=free }}</ref>
*] as spoken in ] may possibly be classed as a mixed language, as it is largely mixed ] and ] lexicon with ] grammar.<ref>https://www.academia.edu/37913089/Arb%C3%ABresh_Language_mixing_translanguaging_and_possible_solutions_to_issue_of_maintenance Di Maggio, M.H. (2018) Arbëresh: Language mixing, translanguaging and possible solutions to issue of maintenance]</ref>
* ], theorised by ] to have used elements of ], ], ], versions of ] including Talmudic and post-Talmudic, and various ], including ], ], ], ], and ].<ref>Weinreich, M. (1959). History of the Yiddish Language: The Problems and Their Implications in Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 103(4), 563–570. http://www.jstor.org/stable/985559. Accessed 29 October 2024.</ref><ref>Weinreich, M. (1956). The Jewish Languages of Romance Stock and Their Relation to Earliest Yiddish in Romance Philology, 9(4), 403–428. Brepols: University of California Press. http://www.jstor.org/stable/44938790. Accessed 29 October 2024.</ref>

=== Possible mixed languages with a Chinese element ===

*], a mix of ] and ]<ref>{{cite book|last1=Lee-Smith|first1=Mei|last2=Wurm|first2=Stephen|editor=Stephen Wurm |editor2=Peter Mühlhäusler |editor3=Darrell T. Tyron|title=Atlas of Languages of Intercultural Communication in the Pacific, Asia, and the Americas|year=1996|publisher=Walter de Gruyter|isbn=978-3-11-013417-9|pages=883–897|chapter=The Wutun Language}}</ref>
*], Chinese–Tibetan {{Citation needed|date=December 2018}}
*], a mix of one of the ] and ]{{citation needed|date=November 2013}}
*] and ], Mandarin–Miao {{Citation needed|date=December 2018}}
*], Mandarin–Santa {{Citation needed|date=December 2018}}
*], Hunanese–Miao {{Citation needed|date=December 2018}}
*], Uyghur–Mandarin {{Citation needed|date=December 2018}}
*], a mix of English with a Chinese language, especially when spoken by second-generation Chinese<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Jiang |first1=Yajun |date=17 October 2008 |title=Chinglish and China English |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/english-today/article/abs/chinglish-and-china-english/B8168721C6BF7BE4EA8FB123A47447DD |journal=English Today |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=51–56 |doi=10.1017/S0266078400008105 |s2cid=146487147 |access-date=16 June 2021}}
</ref>
*], a mix of ], ] and ].
*], a mix of ], ], and ].

==Controversy==
In 1861, ] denied "the possibility of a mixed language".<ref>''Lectures on the Science of Language'', 1st series, 6th edition, p. 86</ref> In 1881, William D. Whitney wrote the following, expressing skepticism regarding the chances of a language being proven a mixed language.

<blockquote>
Such a thing as the adoption on the part of one tongue, by a direct process, of any part or parts of the formal structure of another tongue has, so far as is known, not come under the notice of linguistic students during the recorded periods of language-history. So far as these are concerned, it appears to be everywhere the case that when the speakers of two languages, A and B, are brought together into one community, there takes place no amalgamation of their speech, into AB; but for a time the two maintain their own several identity, only as modified each by the admission of material from the other in accordance with the ordinary laws of mixture; we may call them A<sup>b</sup> and B<sup>a</sup>, and not AB. e shall doubtless meet now and then with the claim that such and such a case presents peculiar conditions which separate it from the general class, and that some remote and difficult problem in language-history is to be solved by admitting promiscuous mixture. Any one advancing such a claim, however, does it at his peril; the burden of proof is upon him to show what the peculiar conditions might have been, and how they should have acted to produce the exceptional result; he will be challenged to bring forward some historically authenticated case of analogous results; and his solution, if not rejected altogether, will be looked upon with doubt and misgiving until he shall have complied with these reasonable requirements.<ref>''On mixture in language'' (Transactions of the American Philological Association, 1881, reprinted in Whitney on Language. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press, pp. 179-180)</ref></blockquote>

] was an important proponent of the idea of mixed languages in the very late 19th and early 20th century.<ref>van Driem, George, 2001, ''Languages of the Himalayas.''Vol I, p. 163.</ref> In the judgement of ], Schmidt produced "not a scrap of evidence" for his theory.<ref>Seboek, Thomas, 1942, "An examination of the Austroasiatic language family", ''Language'' 18.3, p. 215</ref> ] provides a summary of the various objections to Schmidt's theory of ''Sprachmischung'',<ref>Schlauch, Margaret. 1936. "The Social Basis of Linguistics". ''Science and Society: A Marxian quarterly'' 1.1: 8-44, esp. pp. 36-37.</ref> by prominent linguists such as ], ] and A. Kholodovich.

Despite the old and broad consensus that rejected the idea of a "mixed language", Thomason and Kaufman in 1988 proposed to revive the idea that some languages had shared genealogy.<ref name="thomason-kaufman-1988">Thomason & Kaufman 1988</ref> ],<ref name="meakins-2013">Meakins 2013</ref> who finds Thomason and Kaufman's account credible, suggests that a mixed language results from the fusion of usually two source languages, normally in situations of thorough ], so that it is not possible to classify the resulting language as belonging to either of the ] that were its sources.

Despite these recent efforts to rehabilitate mixed languages as an idea, many linguists remained unconvinced. For example, van Driem rejects one by one each of Thomason and Kaufman's examples as well as those more recently proposed.<ref>van Driem, George, 2001, ''Languages of the Himalayas.''Vol I, p. 163-175.</ref> Most recently, Versteegh rejects the notion of a mixed language, writing that at "no point is it necessary to posit a category of mixed languages."<ref>Versteegh, Kees (2017). The myth of the mixed languages. Saade, Benjamin and Tosco, Mauro, eds. Advances in Maltese Linguistics, pp. 245–266. Berlin, De Gruyter. DOI (Chapter): https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110565744-011
</ref>

In the opinion of linguist Maarten Mous the notion of mixed languages has been rejected because "ixed languages pose a challenge to ] because these languages defy classification. One attitude towards mixed languages has been that they simply do not exist, and that the claims for mixed languages are instances of a naive use of the term. The inhibition to accept the existence of mixed languages is linked to the fact that it was inconceivable how they could emerge, and moreover their mere existence posited a threat to the validity of the ] and to genetic linguistics."<ref>MATRAS, Yaron and BAKKER, Peter eds.: The Mixed Language Debate: Theoretical and Empirical Advances 2003. p. 209.</ref>


==See also== ==See also==
*]
*] *]
*]
*]
*] *]
*] *]
*] *]
*] *]
*] (the vocabulary of a sign language with the grammar of an oral language, but without an established language community)
*]
*{{anli|Macaronic language}}
*]
*]
*]
*]

== Notes ==
{{Reflist|group=nb}}

== References ==

=== Citations ===
{{Reflist|30em}}


==References== === Sources ===
* {{cite book |author=Arends, Jacques |author2=Pieter Muysken |author3=Norvel Smith |url = https://books.google.com/books?id=TDA2K3Y0CAYC |title = Pidgins and Creoles: An Introduction |publisher = John Benjamins Publishing |year=1994 |page = 46 |isbn=978-9027299505 }}
{{reflist}}
*{{cite book | author=Bakker, Peter | title=A Language of Our Own: The Genesis of Michif, the Mixed Cree-French Language of the Canadian Metis | location=] | publisher=] | year=1997 | id=ISBN 0-19-509712-2}} * {{cite book |author=Bakker, Peter |title = A Language of Our Own: The Genesis of Michif, the Mixed Cree-French Language of the Canadian Metis |location=] | publisher=] |year=1997 |isbn = 978-0-19-509712-2}}
*{{cite book | author=Bakker, P., and M. Mous, eds. | title=Mixed languages: 15 case studies in language intertwining | location=] | publisher=IFOTT | year=1994 }} * {{cite book |editor = Bakker, P. |editor2=M. Mous |title = Mixed languages: 15 case studies in language intertwining |location=] | publisher=IFOTT | year=1994 }}
*{{cite book | author=Matras, Yaron and Peter Bakker, eds. | title=The Mixed Language Debate: Theoretical and Empirical Advances | location=] | publisher=Walter de Gruyter | year=2003 | id=ISBN 3-11-017776-5}} * {{cite book |editor = Matras, Yaron |editor2=Peter Bakker |title = The Mixed Language Debate: Theoretical and Empirical Advances |location=] |publisher = Walter de Gruyter | year=2003 |isbn = 978-3-11-017776-3 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=qZMRV8y6T8AC}}
* Mous, Maarten. 2003. ''The making of a mixed language: The case of Ma'a/Mbugu''. Creole language library (No. 26). Amsterdam: J. Benjamins Pub. Co. * Meakins, Felicity. (2011). ''Case-marking in Contact: The Development and Function of Case-Marking in Gurindji Kriol''. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
* Meakins, Felicity. (2013). ''''. In Bakker, Peter and Yaron Matras (eds)''Contact Languages: A Comprehensive Guide'' Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp.&nbsp;159–228.
*{{cite book | author=Sebba, Mark | title=Contact Languages: Pidgins and Creoles |
* Mous, Maarten. (2003). ''The making of a mixed language: The case of Ma'a/Mbugu''. Creole language library (No. 26). Amsterdam: J. Benjamins Pub. Co.
publisher=MacMillan| year=1997|id=ISBN 0-333-63024-6}}
*{{cite book | author=Thomason, Sarah and Terrence Kaufman | title=Language Contact, Creolization, and Genetic Linguistics | publisher=] | year=1988 | id=ISBN 0-520-07893-4}} *{{cite book | author=Sebba, Mark | title=Contact Languages: Pidgins and Creoles |
publisher=MacMillan| year=1997|isbn=978-0-333-63024-2}}
* O'Shannessy, Carmel. (2005). ''Special Issue: Language Shift, Code-mixing and Variation, Light Warlpiri: A New Language''. Australian Journal of Linguistics. (25.1).
*{{cite book | author=Silva-Corvalán, Carmen | title=Spanish in Four Continents: Studies in Language Contact and Bilingualism | url=https://books.google.com/books?id=XL5Z5E6O0Q4C | publisher=Georgetown University Press | year= 1997| isbn=978-1589014152 }}
*{{cite book | author=Thomason, Sarah Grey | title=Contact Languages: A Wider Perspective, Creole Language Library | url=https://books.google.com/books?id=to6P22ieV5kC | publisher=John Benjamins Publishing | year=1997 | pages=303–466| isbn=978-9027252395 }}
*{{cite book |author1=Thomason, Sarah |author2=Terrence Kaufman |name-list-style=amp | title=Language Contact, Creolization, and Genetic Linguistics | publisher=] | year=1988 | isbn=978-0-520-07893-2}}
*Velupillai, Viveka. (2015). Pidgins, Creoles, & Mixed Languages: An Introduction. Chapter 3: Mixed Languages. John Benjamins Publishing. pp.&nbsp;69–97.


{{Authority control}}
]
]


]
]
] ]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 22:37, 15 December 2024

Language that arises amongst a bilingual group
This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these messages)
This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.
Find sources: "Mixed language" – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (December 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this message)
The neutrality of this article is disputed. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please do not remove this message until conditions to do so are met. (January 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this message)
(Learn how and when to remove this message)

A mixed language, also referred to as a hybrid language, contact language, or fusion language, is a language that arises among a bilingual group combining aspects of two or more languages but not clearly deriving primarily from any single language. It differs from a creole or pidgin language in that, whereas creoles/pidgins arise where speakers of many languages acquire a common language, a mixed language typically arises in a population that is fluent in both of the source languages.

Because all languages show some degree of mixing by virtue of containing loanwords, it is a matter of controversy whether the concept of a mixed language can meaningfully be distinguished from the type of contact and borrowing seen in all languages. Scholars debate to what extent language mixture can be distinguished from other mechanisms such as code-switching, substrata, or lexical borrowing.

Definitions

Other terms used in linguistics for the concept of a mixed language include hybrid language, contact language, and fusion language; in older usage, 'jargon' was sometimes used in this sense. In some linguists' usage, creoles and pidgins are types of mixed languages, whereas in others' usage, creoles and pidgins are merely among the kinds of language that might become full-fledged mixed languages.

Thomason (1995) classifies mixed languages into two categories: Category 1 languages exhibit "heavy influence from the dominant group's language in all aspects of structure and grammar as well as lexicon" (Winford 171). Category 2 languages show a "categorial specificity of the structural borrowing" or a uniform borrowing of specific categories (Winford).

Mixed language and intertwined language are seemingly interchangeable terms for some researchers. Some use the term "intertwining" instead of "mixing" because the former implies "mixture of two systems which are not necessarily the same order" nor does it suggest "replacement of the either the lexicon or of the grammatical system", unlike relexification, massive grammatical replacement, and re-grammaticalization. The grammar of a mixed language typically comes from a language well known to first-generation speakers, which Arends claims is the language spoken by the mother. This is because of the close relationship between mother and child and the likelihood that the language is spoken by the community at large.

Arends et al. classify an intertwined language as a language that "has lexical morphemes from one language and grammatical morphemes from another". This definition does not include Michif, which combines French lexical items in specific contexts, but still utilizes Cree lexical and grammatical items.

Yaron Matras distinguishes between three types of models for mixed language: "language maintenance and language shift, unique and predetermined processes ("intertwining"), and "conventionalisation of language mixing patterns". The first model involves the use of one language for heavy substitutions of entire grammatical paradigms or morphology of another language. This is because a speech community will not adopt a newer dominant language, and so adapt their language with grammatical material from the dominant language. Bakker (1997) argues that mixed languages result from mixed populations. Languages "intertwine", in that the morphosyntax (provided by female native speakers) mixes with the lexicon of another language (spoken by men, often in a colonialist context). This appears to have been the case with Michif, where European men and Cree, Nakota, and Ojibwe women had offspring who learned a mixture of French and Cree. The third model "assumes a gradual loss of the conversational function of language alternation as a means of expressing contrast". In other words, language no longer becomes a means of differentiation between two speech communities as a result of language mixing.

Lexical reorientation, according to Matras, is defined as "the conscious shifting of the linguistic field that is responsible for encoding meaning or conceptual representations away from the language in which linguistic interaction is normally managed, organised, and processed: speakers adopt in a sense one linguistic system to express lexical meaning (or symbols, in the Buhlerian sense of the term) and another to organize the relations among lexical symbols, as well as within sentences, utterances, and interaction. The result is a split, by source language, between lexicon and grammar."

Differentiation with other language mixtures

A mixed language differs from pidgins, creoles and code-switching in very fundamental ways. In most cases, mixed language speakers are fluent, even native, speakers of both languages; however, speakers of Michif (a verb-noun or V-N mixed language) are unique in that many are not fluent in both of the source languages. Pidgins, on the other hand, develop in a situation, usually in the context of trade, where speakers of two (or more) different languages come into contact and need to find some way to communicate with each other. Creoles develop when a pidgin language becomes a first language for young speakers. While creoles tend to have drastically simplified morphologies, mixed languages often retain the inflectional complexities of one, or both, of the parent languages. For instance, Michif retains the complexities of its Cree verb-phrases and its French noun-phrases.

It also differs from a language that has undergone heavy borrowing, such as Korean, Japanese, and Vietnamese from Chinese (see Sino-Xenic), English from French, or Maltese from Sicilian/Italian. In these cases, despite the heavy borrowing, the grammar and basic words of the borrowing language remain relatively unchanged, with the borrowed words confined mainly to more abstract or foreign concepts, and any complex morphology remains that of the host language rather than being borrowed along with the borrowed word. In the case of Maltese, for example, if verbs borrowed from Italian were inflected using Italian inflectional rules rather than Arabic-derived ones, then Maltese would be a candidate for being a mixed language.

Finally, a mixed language differs from code-switching, such as Spanglish or Portuñol, in that, once it has developed, the fusion of the source languages is fixed in the grammar and vocabulary, and speakers do not need to know the source languages in order to speak it. But, linguists believe that mixed languages evolve from persistent code-switching, with younger generations picking up the code-switching, but not necessarily the source languages that generated it.

Languages such as Franglais and Anglo-Romani are not mixed languages, or even examples of code-switching, but registers of a language (here French and English), characterized by large numbers of loanwords from a second language (here English and Romani). Middle English (the immediate fore-runner of Modern English) developed from such a situation, incorporating many Norman borrowings into Old English, but it is not considered a mixed language.

Proposed examples

Michif

See also: Michif

Michif derives nouns, numerals, definite/indefinite articles, possessive pronouns, some adverbs and adjectives from French, while it derives demonstratives (in/animate), question words, verbs (in/animacy agreement with the subject/object), and some adverbs/verb-like adjectives from Cree. The Cree components of Michif generally remain grammatically intact, while the French lexicon and grammar is restricted to noun phrases where nouns occur with a French possessive element or article (i.e. in/definite, masculine/feminine, singular/plural). Further, many speakers of Michif are able to identify the French and Cree components of a given sentence, likely from the phonological and morphological features of words. Although the phonological systems of both French and Cree are generally independent in Michif, there is convergence in 1) mid-vowel raising, 2) sibilant harmony, 3) vowel length (e.g. French vowel pairs / and / differ in length as in Cree), and 4) instances where the three nasal vowels /æ̃/, /ũ/, and /ĩ/ occur in the Cree components, although this last point of convergence may be due to Ojibwe influence. Scholars propose that, in the Métis multilingual community, Michif emerged as a need to symbolize a new social identity. The first unambiguous mention of Michif dates to the 1930s.

The Métis of St. Laurent, a tribe of indigenous people in Canada, were made to feel their language was a sign of inferiority by nuns, priests, and other missionaries who insisted that the Metis switch to Standard Canadian French. Because missionaries stigmatized Michif French as an inferior, "bastardized" form of Canadian French, the Métis began to develop a sense of inferiority and shame which they associated with speaking Michif. Although Michif may have arisen as a way for Métis people to identify themselves, it became taboo to speak Michif inter-ethnically.

In an attempt to make students unlearn Michif French, some nuns used a "token-system" in which each student was given ten tokens each week, and for every use of Michif French, a student would have to surrender a token. Students with the most tokens were rewarded with a prize. Overall, this system did not work.

Mednyj Aleut

See also: Mednyj Aleut

Mednyj Aleut is identified as a mixed language composed of mostly intact systematic components from two typologically and genetically unrelated languages: Aleut and Russian. This mixed language's grammar and lexicon are both largely Aleut in origin, while the finite verb morphology, a whole grammatical subsystem, is primarily of Russian origin. Nonetheless, there are some syntactic patterns with Russian influence and some Aleut features in the finite verb complex such as, 1) a topic-number agreement pattern, 2) Aleut pronouns with unaccusatives, 3) the Aleut agglutinative tense + number + person/number pattern in one of two alternative past-tense forms. Scholars hypothesize that due to the elaborate Russian and Aleut components of Mednyj Aleut, the Aleut/Russian creoles in which the mixed language arose must have been fluent bilinguals of Aleut and Russian and, therefore, not a pidgin language—that is, "imperfect learning" is usually a feature in the emergence of a pidgin. Furthermore, some code-switching and deliberate decisions likely served as mechanisms for the development of Mednyj Aleut and it is possible that these were motivated by a need for a language that reflected the community's new group identity.

Ma'a

See also: Mbugu

Ma’a has a Cushitic basic vocabulary and a primarily Bantu grammatical structure. The language also shares some phonological units with languages in the Cushitic phylum (e.g. the voiceless lateral fricative, the voiceless glottal stop, and the voiceless velar fricative that do not occur in Bantu), as well as syntactic structures, derivational processes, and a feature of inflectional morphology. However, few productive non-lexical structures in Ma’a appear derived from Cushitic. Sarah G. Thomason therefore argues for a classification of Ma’a as a mixed language since it does not have enough Cushitic grammar to be genetically related to the Cushitic language. By contrast, Ma’a has a productive set of inflectional structures derived from Bantu. Ma’a also demonstrates phonological structures derived from Bantu—for instance, the prenasalized voiced stops /ᵐb ⁿd ᶮɟ ᵑg/, phonemic tones, the absence of pharyngeal fricatives, labialized dorsal stops, ejective and retroflex stops, and final consonants— as well as noun classification, number category, and verb morphology patterns of Bantu. Syntactic and derivational patterns in Ma’a vary between Cushitic and Bantu origins—some Ma’a constructions used, such as genitive and copula constructions, are both from Cushitic and Bantu. These observations, in view of additional language contact cases like Cappadocian Greek, Anglo-Romani, and Mednyj Aleut, suggest that Ma'a arose as a product of massive interference from a Bantu language via intense cultural pressure on a Cushitic-speaking community.

Media Lengua

Main article: Media Lengua

Media Lengua (roughly translated to "half language" or "in-between language"), also known as Chaupi-shimi, Chaupi-lengua, Chaupi-Quichua, Quichuañol, Chapu-shimi or llanga-shimi, is a mixed language that consists of Spanish vocabulary and Ecuadorian Quichua grammar, most conspicuously in its morphology. In terms of vocabulary, almost all lexemes (89%), including core vocabulary, are of Spanish origin and appear to conform to Quichua phonotactics. Media Lengua is one of the few widely acknowledged examples of a "bilingual mixed language" in both the conventional and narrow linguistic sense because of its split between roots and suffixes. Such extreme and systematic borrowing is only rarely attested, and Media Lengua is not typically described as a variety of either Quichua or Spanish. Arends et al. list two languages subsumed under the name Media Lengua: Salcedo Media Lengua and Media Lengua of Saraguro. The northern variety of Media Lengua, found in the province of Imbabura, is commonly referred to as Imbabura Media Lengua and more specifically, the dialect varieties within the province are known as Pijal Media Lengua and Anglas Media Lengua.

Scholars indicate that Media Lengua arose largely via relexification mechanisms. Pieter Muysken suggests that the social context in which the language emerged as an intralanguage involved a presence of "acculturated Indians" that neither identified with traditional, rural Quechua nor with urban Spanish cultures. This is an instance of a language developing from a need for "ethnic self-identification".

Light Warlpiri

See also: Light Warlpiri

Light Warlpiri, seen as a form of Warlpiri by speakers, derives verbs and verbal morphology largely from Australian Kriol, while nouns are largely from Warlpiri and English and nominal morphology from Warlpiri. Light Warlpiri likely developed as an intralanguage via code-mixing between Warlpiri and either Kriol or English. This code-mixing conventionalized into Light Warlpiri, which is now learned by Lajamanu children as a first language, along with Warlpiri, although Light Warlpiri is often produced first and used in daily interactions with younger speakers and adults within the Lajamanu community. Light Warlpiri is considered a new language for several reasons: 1) Light Warlpiri speakers use an auxiliary verb-system that older Warlpiri speakers do not while code-mixing, 2) elements are distributed differently in Light Warlpiri than in code-mixing varieties of older Warlpiri speakers, 3) Light Warlpiri is a native language, which indicates stability of the language, and 4) grammatical structures and lexical items from each source language occur consistently in Light Warlpiri.

Gurindji Kriol

Main article: Gurindji Kriol language

Gurindji Kriol exhibits a structural split between the noun phrase and verb phrase, with Gurindji contributing the noun structure including case-marking, and the verb structure including TAM (tense-aspect-mood) auxiliaries coming from Kriol. In this respect, Gurindji Kriol is classified as a verb-noun (V-N) mixed language. Other examples of V-N mixed languages include Michif and Light Warlpiri. The maintenance of Gurindji within the mixed language can be seen as the perpetuation of Aboriginal identity under massive and continuing cultural incursion.

Cappadocian Greek and Cypriot Arabic

See also: Cappadocian Greek and Languages of Cyprus

Both Cappadocian Greek and Cypriot Maronite-Arabic are cases of extreme borrowing—the former from Turkish and the latter from Greek. The remaining Greek dialects of Asia Minor display borrowing of vocabulary, function words, derivational morphology, and some borrowed nominal and verbal inflectional morphology from Turkish. Cypriot Arabic largely shows borrowing of vocabulary, and consequently Greek morphosyntax. Both Cappadocian Greek and Cypriot Arabic (as well as Ma'a) differ socially from Michif and Mednyj Aleut because they have evolved out of intense language contact, extensive bilingualism, and a strong pressure for speakers to shift to the dominant language. Nonetheless, neither language has an entire grammar and lexicon that is derived from a single historical source and in each case the linguistic group achieves fluent bilingualism. The social context in which they arose largely distinguishes them from pidgins and creoles and, for some scholars, identifies them closely with mixed languages.

Kaqchikel-K'iche' Mayan language

Main article: Cauque Mayan language

The Kaqchikel-K'iche' Mixed Language, also known as the Cauqué Mixed Language or Cauqué Mayan, is spoken in the aldea of Santa María Cauqué, Santiago Sacatepéquez, Department of Sacatepéquez in Guatemala. A 1998 study by the Summer Institute of Linguistics (SIL) estimated speaker population at 2,000. While the language's grammatical base is from K'iche', its lexicon is supplied by Kaqchikel.

Other possible mixed languages

Possible mixed languages with a Chinese element

Controversy

In 1861, Max Müller denied "the possibility of a mixed language". In 1881, William D. Whitney wrote the following, expressing skepticism regarding the chances of a language being proven a mixed language.

Such a thing as the adoption on the part of one tongue, by a direct process, of any part or parts of the formal structure of another tongue has, so far as is known, not come under the notice of linguistic students during the recorded periods of language-history. So far as these are concerned, it appears to be everywhere the case that when the speakers of two languages, A and B, are brought together into one community, there takes place no amalgamation of their speech, into AB; but for a time the two maintain their own several identity, only as modified each by the admission of material from the other in accordance with the ordinary laws of mixture; we may call them A and B, and not AB. e shall doubtless meet now and then with the claim that such and such a case presents peculiar conditions which separate it from the general class, and that some remote and difficult problem in language-history is to be solved by admitting promiscuous mixture. Any one advancing such a claim, however, does it at his peril; the burden of proof is upon him to show what the peculiar conditions might have been, and how they should have acted to produce the exceptional result; he will be challenged to bring forward some historically authenticated case of analogous results; and his solution, if not rejected altogether, will be looked upon with doubt and misgiving until he shall have complied with these reasonable requirements.

Wilhelm Schmidt was an important proponent of the idea of mixed languages in the very late 19th and early 20th century. In the judgement of Thomas Sebeok, Schmidt produced "not a scrap of evidence" for his theory. Margaret Schlauch provides a summary of the various objections to Schmidt's theory of Sprachmischung, by prominent linguists such as Alfredo Trombetti, Antoine Meillet and A. Kholodovich.

Despite the old and broad consensus that rejected the idea of a "mixed language", Thomason and Kaufman in 1988 proposed to revive the idea that some languages had shared genealogy. Meakins, who finds Thomason and Kaufman's account credible, suggests that a mixed language results from the fusion of usually two source languages, normally in situations of thorough bilingualism, so that it is not possible to classify the resulting language as belonging to either of the language families that were its sources.

Despite these recent efforts to rehabilitate mixed languages as an idea, many linguists remained unconvinced. For example, van Driem rejects one by one each of Thomason and Kaufman's examples as well as those more recently proposed. Most recently, Versteegh rejects the notion of a mixed language, writing that at "no point is it necessary to posit a category of mixed languages."

In the opinion of linguist Maarten Mous the notion of mixed languages has been rejected because "ixed languages pose a challenge to historical linguistics because these languages defy classification. One attitude towards mixed languages has been that they simply do not exist, and that the claims for mixed languages are instances of a naive use of the term. The inhibition to accept the existence of mixed languages is linked to the fact that it was inconceivable how they could emerge, and moreover their mere existence posited a threat to the validity of the comparative method and to genetic linguistics."

See also

Notes

  1. Llanga-shimi is typically a derogatory term used by speakers of Quichua to describe their language. However, it also appears to describe Media Lengua in the Imbabura Communities. It is believed that the term was introduced by Mestizo school teachers to further discredit the indigenous populations

References

Citations

  1. Matras, Yaron; Bakker, Peter, eds. (2008). The Mixed Language Debate: Theoretical and Empirical Advances. Walter de Gruyter. p. 191. ISBN 9783110197242.
  2. Zuckermann (2009) p. 48, citing Hjelmslev (1938) and Schuchardt (1884).
  3. ^ Arends et al. 1994
  4. Yaron Matras (2000). "Mixed languages: a functional–communicative approach" (PDF). Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 3 (2): 79–99. doi:10.1017/S1366728900000213. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 February 2021. Retrieved 15 May 2017.
  5. According to Google n-gram, the German term Mischsprache is first attested in 1832, and attested in English since 1909.
  6. "jargon, n.1." OED Online. Oxford University Press, March 2015. Web. 4 May 2015, sense 5.
  7. ^ Matras, Yaron, "Mixed Languages: a functional-communicative approach", "Bilingualism: Language and Cognition / Volume 3 / Issue 2 / August 2000 / p. 79 - 99
  8. Velupillai, Viveka (24 March 2015). Pidgins, Creoles and Mixed Languages. Creole Language Library. Vol. 48. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. doi:10.1075/cll.48. ISBN 9789027252715.
  9. Viveka., Velupillai (2015). Pidgins, Creoles and Mixed Languages. John Benjamins Publishing Company. OCLC 942975077.
  10. "Middle English Chapter 5" (PDF).
  11. ^ Silva-Corvalán 1997
  12. ^ Thomason 1997
  13. ^ Lavalle, Guy http://iportal.usask.ca/docs/Native_studies_review/v7/issue1/pp81-93.pdf
  14. Thomason, Sarah Grey. GENETIC RELATIONSHIP AND THE CASE OF MA'A (MBUGU), Studies in African Linguistics. 14.2. University of Pittsburgh, 1983. Web. http://elanguage.net/journals/sal/article/view/1138/1154.
  15. Pallares, A. (2002). From peasant struggles to Indian resistance: the Ecuadorian Andes in the late twentieth century. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press.
  16. ^ Stewart, Jesse (2011). A Brief Descriptive Grammar of Pijal Media Lengua and an Acoustic Vowel Space Analysis of Pijal Media Lengua and Imbabura Quichua.. (thesis)
  17. Muysken, Pieter (1997). "Media Lengua", in Thomason, Sarah G. Contact languages: a wider perspective Amsterdam: John Benjamins (pp. 365-426)
  18. Backus, Ad. 2003. Can a mixed language be conventionalised alternational codeswitching? in Matras & Bakker (eds) The Mixed Language Debate: theoretical and empirical advances Mouton de Gruyter Berlin: 237-/270.
  19. McConvell, Patrick, and Felicity Meakins. 2005. Gurindji Kriol: A Mixed Language Emerges from Code-switching. Quatro Fonologias Quechuas, 25(1), 9-30.
  20. Gómez-Rendón, J. A. (2008). Mestizaje lingüístico en los Andes: génesis y estructura de una lengua mixta (1era. ed.). Quito, Ecuador: Abya-Yala.
  21. Gómez-Rendón, J. (2005). La Media Lengua de Imbabura. Encuentros conflictos bilingüismo contacto de lenguas en el mundo andino (pp. 39-58). Madrid: Iberoamericana.
  22. ^ Matras & Bakker 2003
  23. O'Shannessy 2005
  24. ^ Thomason & Kaufman 1988
  25. "Kaqchikel-K'iche' Mixed Language." Lewis, M. Paul, Gary F. Simons, and Charles D. Fennig (eds.). 2016. Ethnologue: Languages of the World, Nineteenth edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL International. Web. https://www.ethnologue.com/language/ckz.
  26. Long, Daniel (2007). English on the Bonin (Ogasawara) Islands. Duke University Press. ISBN 978-0-8223-6671-3.
  27. Benítez-Torres, Carlos M. (2009). "Inflectional vs. Derivational Morphology in Tagdal: A Mixed Language" (PDF). In Masangu Matondo; Fiona Mc Laughlin; Eric Potsdam (eds.). Selected Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference on African Linguistics. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. pp. 69–83.
  28. Wurm, Mühlhäusler, & Tryon, Atlas of languages of intercultural communication in the Pacific, Asia and the Americas, 1996:682.
  29. Walworth, Mary E. (2017a). "Reo Rapa: A Polynesian Contact Language". Journal of Language Contact. 10 (1): 98–141. doi:10.1163/19552629-01001006.
  30. https://www.academia.edu/37913089/Arb%C3%ABresh_Language_mixing_translanguaging_and_possible_solutions_to_issue_of_maintenance Di Maggio, M.H. (2018) Arbëresh: Language mixing, translanguaging and possible solutions to issue of maintenance]
  31. Weinreich, M. (1959). History of the Yiddish Language: The Problems and Their Implications in Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 103(4), 563–570. http://www.jstor.org/stable/985559. Accessed 29 October 2024.
  32. Weinreich, M. (1956). The Jewish Languages of Romance Stock and Their Relation to Earliest Yiddish in Romance Philology, 9(4), 403–428. Brepols: University of California Press. http://www.jstor.org/stable/44938790. Accessed 29 October 2024.
  33. Lee-Smith, Mei; Wurm, Stephen (1996). "The Wutun Language". In Stephen Wurm; Peter Mühlhäusler; Darrell T. Tyron (eds.). Atlas of Languages of Intercultural Communication in the Pacific, Asia, and the Americas. Walter de Gruyter. pp. 883–897. ISBN 978-3-11-013417-9.
  34. Jiang, Yajun (17 October 2008). "Chinglish and China English". English Today. 11 (1): 51–56. doi:10.1017/S0266078400008105. S2CID 146487147. Retrieved 16 June 2021.
  35. Lectures on the Science of Language, 1st series, 6th edition, p. 86
  36. On mixture in language (Transactions of the American Philological Association, 1881, reprinted in Whitney on Language. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press, pp. 179-180)
  37. van Driem, George, 2001, Languages of the Himalayas.Vol I, p. 163.
  38. Seboek, Thomas, 1942, "An examination of the Austroasiatic language family", Language 18.3, p. 215
  39. Schlauch, Margaret. 1936. "The Social Basis of Linguistics". Science and Society: A Marxian quarterly 1.1: 8-44, esp. pp. 36-37.
  40. Meakins 2013
  41. van Driem, George, 2001, Languages of the Himalayas.Vol I, p. 163-175.
  42. Versteegh, Kees (2017). The myth of the mixed languages. Saade, Benjamin and Tosco, Mauro, eds. Advances in Maltese Linguistics, pp. 245–266. Berlin, De Gruyter. DOI (Chapter): https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110565744-011
  43. MATRAS, Yaron and BAKKER, Peter eds.: The Mixed Language Debate: Theoretical and Empirical Advances 2003. p. 209.

Sources

Categories: