Misplaced Pages

Stratum (linguistics): Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 21:33, 21 July 2008 edit89.242.104.114 (talk) Undid revision 227078225 by JdeJ (talk) RV vandalism← Previous edit Latest revision as of 10:31, 27 December 2024 edit undoMHO16 (talk | contribs)4 editsm Not Very accurate need sourceTag: Visual edit 
(657 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Language that influences, or is influenced by another through contact}}
:''see ] for the philosophical term.''
{{About|the term in linguistics||Stratum (disambiguation)}}
In ], a '''substratum''' (] ''sub'': ''under'' + ''stratum'': ''layer'' → ''lower layer'') is a ] that influences another one while that second, intrusive, language supplants it. Similarly, a '''superstratum''' is an intrusive language that exerts influence on another language. An '''adstratum''' refers to a language that is in contact with another language in a neighbour population without either identifiably having higher or lower prestige.
{{More footnotes|date=August 2009}}
In ], a '''stratum''' (] for 'layer') or '''strate''' is a historical layer of ] that influences or is influenced by another language through ]. The notion of "strata" was first developed by the Italian linguist ] , and became known in the English-speaking world through the work of two different authors in 1932.<ref>"Why Don't the English Speak Welsh?" Hildegard Tristram, chapter 15 in ''The Britons in Anglo-Saxon England'', N. J. Higham (ed.), The Boydell Press 2007 {{ISBN|1843833123}}, pp. 192–214. </ref>


Thus, both terms refer to a situation where an intrusive language establishes itself in the territory of another, typically as the result of ]. Whether the superstratum (the local language persists and the intrusive language disappears) or the substratum (the local language disappears and the intrusive language persists) case applies will normally only be evident after several generations , during which the intrusive language exists within a ] culture. In order for the intrusive language to persist (''substratum'' case), the immigrant population will either need to take the position of a political ] or immigrate in significant numbers relative to the local population. (i.e. the intrusion qualifies as an ] or ], an example would be the ] giving raise to ] outside of Italy, displacing ]) Both concepts apply to a situation where an intrusive language establishes itself in the territory of another, typically as the result of ]. Whether the superstratum case (the local language persists and the intrusive language disappears) or the substratum one (the local language disappears and the intrusive language persists) applies will normally only be evident after several generations, during which the intrusive language exists within a ] culture.


In order for the intrusive language to persist, the ''substratum'' case, the immigrant population will either need to take the position of a political ] or immigrate in significant numbers relative to the local population, i.e., the intrusion qualifies as an ] or ]. An example would be the ] giving rise to ] outside Italy, displacing ] and many other ].
The ''superstratum'' case refers to elite populations which eventually adopt the local language (an example would be the ] and ] in France, who eventually abandoned their Germanic dialects in favour of Romance). The boundary case where neither language quite succeeds in displacing the other results in a ].


The ''superstratum'' case refers to elite invading populations that eventually adopt the language of the native lower classes. An example would be the ] and ] in France, who eventually abandoned their Germanic dialects in favor of other Indo-European languages of the Romance branch, profoundly influencing the local speech in the process.
==Substratum==
The term is also used of substrate interference, i.e. the influence exerted by the substratum language on the supplanting language. According to some classifications, this is one of three main types of ]: substratum interference differs from both ], which involves mutual borrowing between languages of roughly equal prestige and no language replacement, and ], which refers to the influence a socially dominating language has on another, receding language which might be eventually relegated to the status of a substratum language.


==Substratum {{anchor|Substratum}}==
In a typical case of substrate interference, a language A occupies a given territory, and another language B arrives in the same territory (brought, for example, with migrations of population). Then language B begins to supplant language A: the speakers of language A abandon their own language in favour of B, generally because they believe that it is in their best (e.g. economic, political, cultural, social) interests to do so. During the language shift, however, the receding language A still influences language B (for example, through the transfer of ]s, ], or grammatical patterns from A to B).
A substratum (plural: substrata) or substrate is a language that an intrusive language influences, which may or may not ultimately change it to become a new language. The term is also used of substrate interference, i.e. the influence the substratum language exerts on the replacing language. According to some classifications, this is one of three main types of ]: substratum interference differs from both ], which involves no language replacement but rather mutual borrowing between languages of equal "value", and ], which refers to the influence a socially dominating language has on another, receding language that might eventually be relegated to the status of a substratum language.


In a typical case of substrate interference, a Language A occupies a given territory and another Language B arrives in the same territory, brought, for example, with migrations of population. Language B then begins to supplant language A: the speakers of Language A abandon their own language in favor of the other language, generally because they believe that it will help them achieve certain goals within government, the workplace, and in social settings. During the language shift, the receding language A still influences language B, for example, through the transfer of ]s, ]s, or grammatical patterns from A to B.
For example, ] is a substratum of ]. A ], the ], lived in the current French-speaking territory before the arrival of the ]. Given the cultural, economic and political prestige which ] enjoyed, the Gauls eventually abandoned their language in favour of Latin, which evolved in this region until eventually it took the form of Modern French. The Gaulish speech disappeared, but it remains detectable in some French words (approximately ninety) as well as place-names of Gaulish origin.


In most cases, the ability to identify substrate influence in a language requires knowledge of the structure of the substrate language. This can be acquired in numerous ways:<ref>{{cite thesis|first=Janne|last=Saarikivi|title=Substrata Uralica: Studies on Finno-Ugrian substrate influence in Northern Russian dialects|year=2006|type=Ph.D.|publisher=University of Helsinki|pages=12–14}}</ref>
Another example is the influence of the ] ], extinct since the 18th century, on the ] dialects of the ] and ].
* The substrate language, or some later descendant of it, still survives in a part of its former range;
* Written records of the substrate language may exist to various degrees;
* The substrate language itself may be unknown entirely, but it may have surviving close relatives that can be used as a base of comparison.


One of the first-identified cases of substrate influence is an example of a substrate language of the second type: ], from the ancient Celtic people the Gauls. The ] lived in the modern French-speaking territory before the arrival of the ], namely the invasion of Julius Caesar's army. Given the cultural, economic and political advantages that came with being a Latin speaker, the Gauls eventually abandoned their language in favor of the language brought to them by the Romans, which evolved in this region, until eventually it took the form of the French language that is known today. The Gaulish speech disappeared in the late Roman era, but remnants of its vocabulary survive in some French words, approximately 200, as well as place-names of Gaulish origin.<ref>Giovanni Battista Pellegrini, "Substrata", in ''Romance Comparative and Historical Linguistics'', ed. Rebecca Posner et al. (The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter, 1980), 65.</ref>
Linguistic substrata may be difficult to detect, especially if the substratum language and its nearest relatives are extinct. For example, the earliest form of the ] may have been influenced by a non-Indo-European language, purportedly the source of about one quarter of the most ancient Germanic word-stock; see '']''.


It is posited that some structural changes in French were shaped at least in part by Gaulish influence<ref>Giovanni Battista Pellegrini, "Substrata", in ''Romance Comparative and Historical Linguistics'', ed. Rebecca Posner et al. (The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter, 1980), 65.</ref> including diachronic sound changes and ] phenomena due to the retention of Gaulish phonetic patterns after the adoption of Latin,<ref>Henri Guiter, "Sur le substrat gaulois dans la Romania", in ''Munus amicitae. Studia linguistica in honorem Witoldi Manczak septuagenarii'', eds., Anna Bochnakowa & Stanislan Widlak, Krakow, 1995.</ref><ref>Eugeen Roegiest, ''Vers les sources des langues romanes: Un itinéraire linguistique à travers la Romania'' (Leuven, Belgium: Acco, 2006), 83.</ref><ref>], ''La Langue gauloise'' (Paris: Errance, 1994), 46-7. {{ISBN|978-2-87772-224-7}}</ref> ]s such as ''aveugle'' ("blind", literally without eyes, from Latin ''ab oculis'', which was a calque on the Gaulish word {{lang|xtg|exsops}} with the same semantic construction as modern French)<ref>], ''La Langue gauloise'' (Paris: Errance, 1994), 158. {{ISBN|978-2-87772-224-7}}</ref> with other Celtic calques possibly including "oui", the word for yes,<ref name="Matasovic">Matasović, Ranko. 2007. “Insular Celtic as a Language Area”. In Tristam, Hildegard L.C. 2007, ''The Celtic Languages in Contact''. Bonn: Papers from the Workshop within the Framework of the XIII International Congress of Celtic Studies. Page 106.</ref> while syntactic and morphological effects are also posited.<ref name="Matasovic"/><ref>Savignac, Jean-Paul. 2004. ''Dictionnaire Français-Gaulois''. Paris: La Différence. Pages 26, 294-5.</ref><ref>Filppula, Markku, Klemola, Juhani and Paulasto, Heli. 2008. ''English and Celtic in Contact''. New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group. Pages 77-82</ref>
]s typically have multiple substrata, rarely homogeneous ones.


Other examples of substrate languages are the influence of the now extinct ] ] on the ] dialects of the ] and ] islands. In the Arab ] and ], colloquial ] dialects, most especially ], ], and ] dialects, often exhibit significant substrata from other regional Semitic (especially ]), Iranian, and Berber languages. ] has ], ] and ] substrata.
The term was coined by Walter von Wartburg.


Typically, ]s have multiple substrata, with the actual influence of such languages being indeterminate.
===Indo-European===

Substrata in ]:
===Unattested substrata===
*]
In the absence of all three lines of evidence mentioned above, linguistic substrata may be difficult to detect. Substantial indirect evidence is needed to infer the former existence of a substrate. The nonexistence of a substrate is ],<ref name="Matasovic14">{{cite journal|first=Ranko|last=Matasović|author-link=Ranko Matasović|title=Substratum words in Balto-Slavic|journal=Filologija|issue=60|year=2014|pages=75–102}}</ref> and to avoid digressing into speculation, ] must lie on the side of the scholar claiming the influence of a substrate. The principle of ]<ref>{{cite web|first=Don|last=Ringe|title=The Linguistic Diversity of Aboriginal Europe|website=]|date=2009-01-06|url=http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=980|access-date=2017-09-30}}</ref> and results from the study of ] suggest that many languages have formerly existed that have since then been replaced under expansive language families, such as Indo-European, Afro-Asiatic, Uralic or Bantu. However, it is not a given that such expansive languages would have acquired substratum influence from the languages they have replaced.
*]

*]
Several examples of this type of substratum have still been claimed. For example, the earliest form of the ] may have ], purportedly the source of about one quarter of the most ancient Germanic vocabulary. There are similar arguments for a ], a ], and a substrate underlying the ]. Relatively clear examples are the ] of the ] and the "]" (], ], and ]): while unattested, their existence has been noted in medieval chronicles, and one or more of them have left substantial influence in the ].

By contrast, more contentious cases are the ] and ], which hypothesize large families of substrate languages across western Europe. Some smaller-scale unattested substrates that remain under debate involve alleged extinct branches of the Indo-European family, such as "]" substrate in the Germanic languages, and a "Temematic" substrate in ], proposed by ].<ref name="Matasovic14" /> The name ''Temematic'' is an abbreviation of "tenuis, media, media aspirata, tenuis", referencing a sound shift presumed common to the group.

When a substrate language or its close relatives cannot be directly studied, their investigation is rooted in the study of ] and ]. The study of unattested substrata often begins from the study of ''substrate words'', which lack a clear etymology.<ref name="Leschber16">{{cite book|first=Corinna|last=Leschber|contribution=On the stratification of substratum languages|title=Etymology and the European Lexicon: Proceedings of the 14th Fachtagung der Indogermanischen Gesellschaft, 17–22 September 2012, Copenhagen|year=2016|location=Wiesbaden|publisher=Reichert Verlag}}</ref> Such words can in principle still be native inheritance, lost everywhere else in the language family, but they might in principle also originate from a substrate.<ref name="Schrijver97">{{cite book|first=Peter|last=Schrijver|contribution=Animal, vegetable and mineral: some Western European substratum words|year=1997|title=Sound Law and Analogy|pages=293–316|editor-first=A.|editor-last=Lubotsky|location=Amsterdam/Atlanta}}</ref> The sound structure of words of unknown origin — their ] and ] — can often suggest hints in either direction.<ref name="Matasovic14"/><ref>{{cite journal|first=Michael|last=Witzel|title=Early Sources for South Asian Substrate Languages|journal=Mother Tongue|year=1999|url=http://www.people.fas.harvard.edu/%7Ewitzel/MT-Substrates.pdf}}</ref>

So can their meaning: words referring to the natural landscape, in particular indigenous fauna and flora, have often been found especially likely to derive from substrate languages.<ref name="Matasovic14"/><ref name="Leschber16"/><ref name="Schrijver97"/> None of these conditions, is sufficient by itself to claim any one word as originating from an unknown substratum.<ref name="Matasovic14"/> Occasionally words that have been proposed to be of substrate origin will be found out to have cognates in more distantly related languages after all, and therefore likely native: an example is Proto-Indo-European ''*mori'' 'sea', found widely in the northern and western Indo-European languages, but in more eastern Indo-European languages only in ].<ref name="Schrijver97"/>

===Concept history===
Although the influence of the prior language when a community speaks, and adopts, a new one may have been informally acknowledged beforehand, the concept was formalized and popularized initially in the late 19th century. As historical phonology emerged as a discipline, the initial dominant viewpoint was that influences from ] on phonology and grammar should be assumed to be marginal, and an internal explanation should always be favored if possible. As articulated by Max Mueller in 1870, {{Lang|de|Es gibt keine Mischsprache}} ("there are no ]").<ref>{{cite book|last1=Thomason |first1=Sarah Grey |last2=Kaufmann |first2=Terrence |title=Language Contact, Creolization, and Genetic Linguistics|date=12 February 1992|pages=1–3|publisher=University of California Press |isbn=9780520912793|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=b_6OMfZ1QpUC&q=Language+contact+substratum+theory+origin&pg=IA3}}</ref> In the 1880s, dissent began to crystallize against this viewpoint. Within Romance language linguistics, the 1881 ''Lettere glottologiche'' of ] argued that the early phonological development of ] and other ] was shaped by the retention by Celts of their "oral dispositions" even after they had switched to Latin.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Hoyt |first1=David L. |last2=Ostlund |first2=Karen|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=N8H7YPY4oswC&q=Ascoli+lettere+glottologiche+substrate+theory&pg=PA103|page=103|title=The Study of Language and the Politics of Community in Global Context|year = 2006|publisher=Lexington Books |isbn = 9780739109557}}</ref>

In 1884, ]'s related but distinct concept of ] was used to counter Mueller's view. In modern historical linguistics, debate persists on the details of how language contact may induce structural changes. The respective extremes of "all change is contact" and "there are no structural changes ever" have largely been abandoned in favor of a set of conventions on how to demonstrate contact induced structural changes. These include adequate knowledge of the two languages in question, a historical explanation, and evidence that the contact-induced phenomenon did not exist in the recipient language before contact, among other guidelines.


==Superstratum== ==Superstratum==
In ], a '''superstratum''' or '''superstrate''' is the counterpart to a ]. When one ] succeeds another, the former is termed the superstratum and the latter the substratum. In the case of ], for example, ] is the superstrate and ] is the substrate. A superstratum (plural: superstrata) or superstrate offers the counterpart to a substratum. When a different language influences a base language to result in a new language, linguists label the influencing language a superstratum and the influenced language a substratum.


It is also used to describe an imposed linguistic element, akin to what ] underwent after 1066 with ]. The ] and ] coinages adopted by European languages (and now, languages worldwide) to describe scientific topics (anatomy, medicine, botany, zoology, all the '-]' words, etc.) can also be termed a superstratum, although for this last, ] would be a better choice. A superstrate may also represent an imposed linguistic element akin to what occurred with ] and ] after the Norman Conquest of 1066 when use of the English language carried low prestige. The ] coinages from Greek and Latin roots adopted by European languages (and subsequently by other languages) to describe scientific topics (sociology, zoology, philosophy, botany, medicine, all "]" words, etc.) can also be termed a superstratum,{{cn|date=April 2021}} although for this last case, "]" might be a better designation (despite the prestige of science and of its language). In the case of ], for example, ] is the superstrate and ] the substrate.


Some linguists contend that ] (and ] in general) consists of an ] superstratum projected onto an ] substratum.<ref>Benedict (1990), Lewin (1976), Matsumoto (1975), Miller (1967), Murayama (1976), Shibatani (1990).</ref> Some scholars also argue for the existence of Altaic superstrate influences on ] spoken in ].<ref>{{cite book|title=Language Interrupted: Signs of Non-Native Acquisition in Standard Language Grammars|first=John|last=McWhorter|chapter=Mandarin Chinese: "Altaicization" or Simplification?|year=2007|publisher=Oxford University Press}}</ref> In this case, however, the superstratum refers to influence, not language succession. Other views detect ''sub''strate effects.<ref>Hashimoto (1986), Janhunen (1996), McWhorter (2007).</ref>
A modern example is the ], which was historically created by the superstrate ] superimposed over the substrate ]. The result then became a substrate over which ] (particularly ]) superstrated itself. In recent times, the resulting language mix has become a substrate again, this time for ].


==Adstratum== ==Adstratum==
{{Unreferenced section|date=July 2019}}
The term '''adstratum''' refers to a ] which is equal in ] to another. Generally the term is used only when speaking about languages in a particular country or geopolitical region. For example, early in ]'s history, ] and ] had an adstratal relationship.
An adstratum (plural: adstrata) or adstrate is a language that influences another language by virtue of geographic proximity, not by virtue of its relative prestige. For example, early in ]'s history, ] served as an adstrate, contributing to the lexical structure of ].<ref>For example, ''take'' replaced earlier ''niman'' in the lexical slot of a transitive verb for "to take", though archaic forms of ''to nim'' survived in England.</ref>


The phenomenon is less common today in standardized linguistic varieties and more common in colloquial forms of speech. Modern nations tend to favour a single linguistic variety, often corresponding to the ] of the ] and other important regions, over others.
The phenomenon is relatively rare today, since modern nations generally have only one dominant language (often corresponding to the ] of the ]). In ], where dozens of languages are widespread, many could be said to share an adstratal relationship, although ] is certainly dominant in North India. A more accurate example would be the situation in ], where the ] and ] languages have roughly the same status, and could justifiably be called adstrates.


In ], where dozens of languages are widespread, many languages could be said to share an adstratal relationship, but ] is certainly a dominant adstrate in ].
The term '''adstratum''' is also used to identify systematic influences or a layer of borrowings in a given language from another language where the two languages coexist as separate entities. Many modern languages have an appreciable adstratum from English. The ] and ] coinages adopted by European languages (and now, languages worldwide) to describe scientific topics (anatomy, medicine, botany, zoology, all the '-]' words, etc.) can also justifiably be called adstrata.


A different example would be the sociolinguistic situation in ], where the ] and ] languages have roughly the same status. They could justifiably be called adstrates to each other as each has provided a large set of lexical specifications to the other.
==Notable examples==
*Current result language – Substratum (Superstratum)


The term adstratum is also used to identify systematic influences or a layer of borrowings in a given language from another language, independently of whether the two languages continue coexisting as separate entities. Many modern languages have an appreciable adstratum from English, due both to the cultural influence and economic preponderance of the United States on international markets, and the earlier colonization by the ] that made English a global ]. The Greek and Latin coinages adopted by European languages, including English and now languages worldwide, to describe scientific topics, sociology, medicine, anatomy, biology, all the '-]' words, etc., are also justifiably called adstrata.
**French – Gaulish (Latin){{Fact|date=May 2008}}

**] – various Indian languages (substrate), especially Hindi (English)
Another example is found in ] and ], which contain a heavy Semitic, particularly Arabic, adstratum. ] is a linguistic variety of ] with adstrata from ] and ], mostly in the sphere of religion. ] were linked geographically to Yiddish-speaking villages in Eastern Europe for centuries up until the ].
**] – ] (English)

**] – African languages (])
==Notable examples of possible substrate or superstrate influence==
**] – ], African languages (French)
===Substrate influence on superstrate===
**] – pre-existing ] languages (]) (arguably adstrata)
{|class="wikitable"
!Area
!Resultant language
!Substrate
!Superstrate
!Superstrate introduced by
|-
|] (]), Northern ]
|] (Viet), ], ], ]
|Various ]s
|]
|Sinicisation (], ], and ]), between the first millennium BC and the first millennium AD
|-
||]||]
|]
|rowspan="4" valign="center"|]
| rowspan="4" valign="center"| ] during the ]
|-
||]||]||]
|-
||]||]||]
|-
||] (North Africa)||]||], ], ], and ]
|-
|]||]
|]
|rowspan="2" valign="center"|]
| rowspan="2" valign="center"|] ] expansion
|-
|]/]||], ] and ]||] and ]
|-
||]||]||] and ]||]|| rowspan="1" |]
|-
||]||]||]|| rowspan="4" valign="center" |]||] of ]
|-
||]||]||]||] during the ] in the 16th century
|-
||]||]||] and ]||the English during ] in the 16th century
|-
|]
|]
|] of transported enslaved Africans
|the English during ]
|-
||]||]||Southern Chinese varieties: ], ], ], ]||]|| ] during the ]
|-
||]||]||]|| rowspan="4" |]|| rowspan="4" |] who annexed it to the Roman Empire (1st century BC-7th century AD),
|-
||]|| rowspan="2" |]|| rowspan="2" |]
|-
||]
|-
|]
|]
|]
|-
||]||]||]||]||] during the incorporation of the Canary Islands into the ]
|-
||]||]
| ] and other ]
| rowspan="4" valign="center" |] of the 15th century
| rowspan="4" valign="center" |] during the ]<br> of the 15th century
|-
||]||]||], ]
|-
||]||]||]
|-
||]||]||], ], ], ], ], ], ], ], and ]
|-
||]||]|| ], ]<ref> {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20171010202740/http://150.164.100.248/profs/helianamello/data1/arquivos/THE_GENESIS_AND_DEVELOPMENT_OF_BRAZILIAN_VERNACULAR_PORTUGUESE.pdf |date=2017-10-10 }} Page 246, etc</ref>|| rowspan="2" |] of the 15th century||the Portuguese during the ]
|-
||]||]||], ], and ]||the Portuguese during the colonial rule in Africa
|-
||]||]||], ], ],<br/>] dialects, and other ] and languages spoken by Jews||Hebrew constructed from ] and ] Hebrew ||European ] in the late 19th and early 20th centuries who ] Hebrew as a vernacular
|-
|] and ]||]||]||]||Acquisition by ] in the 15th century
|-
||]||]||] ||]||Union with Danish crown, 1380–1814.
|-
|]/]
|]
|], various ]
|]
|Italian immigration to Uruguay and Argentina
|-
|]
|]
|]
|]
|Assimilation of East Balts by East Slavs in the Middle Ages
|-
|] (])
|]
| ]
|]
|] of the ]es and ]
|}

===Superstrate influence on substrate===
{|class="wikitable"
!Area
!Resultant language
!Substrate
!Superstrate
!Superstrate introduced by
|-
||]||]||]||]|| ]' dominance of ] around 500
|-
||]||]||]||]|| ] during the ]
|-
||]||]||]||]|| ] following the ] and during the subsequent ]
|-
||]||]||]||] (by way of ])||] during the ], and the Arabic and Mozarabic speakers in ] who were absorbed into ] and other Christian kingdoms during the ]
|-
|]
|]
|]
|], later ] and other ]<ref>{{Cite book|title=Lıngwa u lıngwıstıka|date=1998|publisher=Klabb Kotba Maltin|others=Borg, Karl.|isbn=99909-75-42-6|location=Valletta, Malta|oclc=82586980}}</ref>
|] and ] control, establishment of the ] on the islands in the 16th century<ref>{{Cite book|last=Brincat, Joseph M.|title=Il-Malti, elf sena ta' storja|date=2000|publisher=Pubblikazzjonijiet Indipendenza|isbn=99909-41-68-8|location=Malta|oclc=223378429}}</ref>
|-
|], ]
|]
|], ]
|] (first ], then ], and later individual Slavic languages such as ], ], ], ], and ])
|], rule by the ], ], and ]s
|-
|] (])
|]
|]
|]
|] during the ], and periods of ] and ]n rule
|}

==See also==
{{Wiktionary|stratum|substratum|substrate|superstratum|superstrate}}
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]


==References== ==References==
{{Reflist}}

==Further reading==
*Benedict, Paul K. (1990). ''Japanese/Austro-Tai''. Ann Arbor: Karoma.
*Cravens, Thomas D. (1994). "Substratum". ''The Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics'', ed. by R. E. Asher et al. Vol. 1, pp.&nbsp;4396–4398. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
* ] (1986). "The Altaicization of Northern Chinese". ''Contributions to Sino-Tibetan studies'', eds John McCoy & Timothy Light, 76–97. Leiden: Brill.
*] (1996). ''Manchuria: An Ethnic History''. Helsinki: Finno-Ugrian Society.
*Jungemann, Frédéric H. (1955). ''La teoría del substrato y los dialectos Hispano-romances y gascones''. Madrid.
*Lewin, Bruno (1976). . ''Journal of Japanese Studies'' 2:2.389–412
*Matsumoto, Katsumi (1975). "Kodai nihongoboin soshikikõ: naiteki saiken no kokoromi". ''Bulletin of the Faculty of Law and Letters'' (Kanazawa University) 22.83–152.
*McWhorter, John (2007). . USA: Oxford University Press.
*Miller, Roy Andrew (1967). ''The Japanese language''. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
*Murayama, Shichiro (1976). . ''Journal of Japanese Studies'' 2:2.413–436
*Shibatani, Masayoshi (1990). ''The languages of Japan''. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.
*Singler, John Victor (1983). . ''Current Approaches to African Linguistics (vol. 2)'', ed. by J. Kaye ''et al.'', 65–77. Dordrecht.
*Singler, John Victor (1988). "". ''Language'' 64.27–51.
*Vovin, Alexander (1994). "Long-distance relationships, reconstruction methodology and the origins of Japanese". ''Diachronica'' 11:1.95–114.
*{{cite book *{{cite book
|first = Walter von |first = Walter von
Line 62: Line 239:
|first = Uriel |first = Uriel
|last = Weinreich |last = Weinreich
|origyear = 1953 |orig-year = 1953
|year = 1979 |year = 1979
|title = Languages in contact: findings and problems |title = Languages in contact: findings and problems
|location = New York |location = New York
|publisher = Mouton Publishers |publisher = Mouton Publishers
|isbn = 9789027926890 |isbn = 978-90-279-2689-0
}} }}
*Fréderic H. Jungemann, 1955. ''La teoría del substrato y los dialectos hispano-romances y gascones''. Madrid.
*John Victor Singler, 1983. "The influence of African languages on pidgins and creoles." ''Current Approaches to African Linguistics (vol.2)'', ed. by J. Kaye ''et al.'', 65-77. Dordrecht.
*John Victor Singler, 1988. "The homogeneity of the substrate as a factor in pidgin/creole genesis." ''Language'' 64.27-51.


{{Authority control}}
==See also==
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]


{{DEFAULTSORT:Stratum (Linguistics)}}
]
]
]
] ]

]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 10:31, 27 December 2024

Language that influences, or is influenced by another through contact This article is about the term in linguistics. For other uses, see Stratum (disambiguation).
This article includes a list of general references, but it lacks sufficient corresponding inline citations. Please help to improve this article by introducing more precise citations. (August 2009) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

In linguistics, a stratum (Latin for 'layer') or strate is a historical layer of language that influences or is influenced by another language through contact. The notion of "strata" was first developed by the Italian linguist Graziadio Isaia Ascoli , and became known in the English-speaking world through the work of two different authors in 1932.

Both concepts apply to a situation where an intrusive language establishes itself in the territory of another, typically as the result of migration. Whether the superstratum case (the local language persists and the intrusive language disappears) or the substratum one (the local language disappears and the intrusive language persists) applies will normally only be evident after several generations, during which the intrusive language exists within a diaspora culture.

In order for the intrusive language to persist, the substratum case, the immigrant population will either need to take the position of a political elite or immigrate in significant numbers relative to the local population, i.e., the intrusion qualifies as an invasion or colonisation. An example would be the Roman Empire giving rise to Romance languages outside Italy, displacing Gaulish and many other Indo-European languages.

The superstratum case refers to elite invading populations that eventually adopt the language of the native lower classes. An example would be the Burgundians and Franks in France, who eventually abandoned their Germanic dialects in favor of other Indo-European languages of the Romance branch, profoundly influencing the local speech in the process.

Substratum

A substratum (plural: substrata) or substrate is a language that an intrusive language influences, which may or may not ultimately change it to become a new language. The term is also used of substrate interference, i.e. the influence the substratum language exerts on the replacing language. According to some classifications, this is one of three main types of linguistic interference: substratum interference differs from both adstratum, which involves no language replacement but rather mutual borrowing between languages of equal "value", and superstratum, which refers to the influence a socially dominating language has on another, receding language that might eventually be relegated to the status of a substratum language.

In a typical case of substrate interference, a Language A occupies a given territory and another Language B arrives in the same territory, brought, for example, with migrations of population. Language B then begins to supplant language A: the speakers of Language A abandon their own language in favor of the other language, generally because they believe that it will help them achieve certain goals within government, the workplace, and in social settings. During the language shift, the receding language A still influences language B, for example, through the transfer of loanwords, place names, or grammatical patterns from A to B.

In most cases, the ability to identify substrate influence in a language requires knowledge of the structure of the substrate language. This can be acquired in numerous ways:

  • The substrate language, or some later descendant of it, still survives in a part of its former range;
  • Written records of the substrate language may exist to various degrees;
  • The substrate language itself may be unknown entirely, but it may have surviving close relatives that can be used as a base of comparison.

One of the first-identified cases of substrate influence is an example of a substrate language of the second type: Gaulish, from the ancient Celtic people the Gauls. The Gauls lived in the modern French-speaking territory before the arrival of the Romans, namely the invasion of Julius Caesar's army. Given the cultural, economic and political advantages that came with being a Latin speaker, the Gauls eventually abandoned their language in favor of the language brought to them by the Romans, which evolved in this region, until eventually it took the form of the French language that is known today. The Gaulish speech disappeared in the late Roman era, but remnants of its vocabulary survive in some French words, approximately 200, as well as place-names of Gaulish origin.

It is posited that some structural changes in French were shaped at least in part by Gaulish influence including diachronic sound changes and sandhi phenomena due to the retention of Gaulish phonetic patterns after the adoption of Latin, calques such as aveugle ("blind", literally without eyes, from Latin ab oculis, which was a calque on the Gaulish word exsops with the same semantic construction as modern French) with other Celtic calques possibly including "oui", the word for yes, while syntactic and morphological effects are also posited.

Other examples of substrate languages are the influence of the now extinct North Germanic Norn language on the Scots dialects of the Shetland and Orkney islands. In the Arab Middle East and North Africa, colloquial Arabic dialects, most especially Levantine, Egyptian, and Maghreb dialects, often exhibit significant substrata from other regional Semitic (especially Aramaic), Iranian, and Berber languages. Yemeni Arabic has Modern South Arabian, Old South Arabian and Himyaritic substrata.

Typically, Creole languages have multiple substrata, with the actual influence of such languages being indeterminate.

Unattested substrata

In the absence of all three lines of evidence mentioned above, linguistic substrata may be difficult to detect. Substantial indirect evidence is needed to infer the former existence of a substrate. The nonexistence of a substrate is difficult to show, and to avoid digressing into speculation, burden of proof must lie on the side of the scholar claiming the influence of a substrate. The principle of uniformitarianism and results from the study of human genetics suggest that many languages have formerly existed that have since then been replaced under expansive language families, such as Indo-European, Afro-Asiatic, Uralic or Bantu. However, it is not a given that such expansive languages would have acquired substratum influence from the languages they have replaced.

Several examples of this type of substratum have still been claimed. For example, the earliest form of the Germanic languages may have been influenced by a non-Indo-European language, purportedly the source of about one quarter of the most ancient Germanic vocabulary. There are similar arguments for a Sanskrit substrate, a Greek one, and a substrate underlying the Sami languages. Relatively clear examples are the Finno-Ugric languages of the Chude and the "Volga Finns" (Merya, Muromian, and Meshcheran): while unattested, their existence has been noted in medieval chronicles, and one or more of them have left substantial influence in the Northern Russian dialects.

By contrast, more contentious cases are the Vasconic substratum theory and Old European hydronymy, which hypothesize large families of substrate languages across western Europe. Some smaller-scale unattested substrates that remain under debate involve alleged extinct branches of the Indo-European family, such as "Nordwestblock" substrate in the Germanic languages, and a "Temematic" substrate in Balto-Slavic, proposed by Georg Holzer. The name Temematic is an abbreviation of "tenuis, media, media aspirata, tenuis", referencing a sound shift presumed common to the group.

When a substrate language or its close relatives cannot be directly studied, their investigation is rooted in the study of etymology and linguistic typology. The study of unattested substrata often begins from the study of substrate words, which lack a clear etymology. Such words can in principle still be native inheritance, lost everywhere else in the language family, but they might in principle also originate from a substrate. The sound structure of words of unknown origin — their phonology and morphology — can often suggest hints in either direction.

So can their meaning: words referring to the natural landscape, in particular indigenous fauna and flora, have often been found especially likely to derive from substrate languages. None of these conditions, is sufficient by itself to claim any one word as originating from an unknown substratum. Occasionally words that have been proposed to be of substrate origin will be found out to have cognates in more distantly related languages after all, and therefore likely native: an example is Proto-Indo-European *mori 'sea', found widely in the northern and western Indo-European languages, but in more eastern Indo-European languages only in Ossetic.

Concept history

Although the influence of the prior language when a community speaks, and adopts, a new one may have been informally acknowledged beforehand, the concept was formalized and popularized initially in the late 19th century. As historical phonology emerged as a discipline, the initial dominant viewpoint was that influences from language contact on phonology and grammar should be assumed to be marginal, and an internal explanation should always be favored if possible. As articulated by Max Mueller in 1870, Es gibt keine Mischsprache ("there are no mixed languages"). In the 1880s, dissent began to crystallize against this viewpoint. Within Romance language linguistics, the 1881 Lettere glottologiche of Graziadio Isaia Ascoli argued that the early phonological development of French and other Gallo-Romance languages was shaped by the retention by Celts of their "oral dispositions" even after they had switched to Latin.

In 1884, Hugo Schuchardt's related but distinct concept of creole languages was used to counter Mueller's view. In modern historical linguistics, debate persists on the details of how language contact may induce structural changes. The respective extremes of "all change is contact" and "there are no structural changes ever" have largely been abandoned in favor of a set of conventions on how to demonstrate contact induced structural changes. These include adequate knowledge of the two languages in question, a historical explanation, and evidence that the contact-induced phenomenon did not exist in the recipient language before contact, among other guidelines.

Superstratum

A superstratum (plural: superstrata) or superstrate offers the counterpart to a substratum. When a different language influences a base language to result in a new language, linguists label the influencing language a superstratum and the influenced language a substratum.

A superstrate may also represent an imposed linguistic element akin to what occurred with English and Norman after the Norman Conquest of 1066 when use of the English language carried low prestige. The international scientific vocabulary coinages from Greek and Latin roots adopted by European languages (and subsequently by other languages) to describe scientific topics (sociology, zoology, philosophy, botany, medicine, all "-logy" words, etc.) can also be termed a superstratum, although for this last case, "adstratum" might be a better designation (despite the prestige of science and of its language). In the case of French, for example, Latin is the superstrate and Gaulish the substrate.

Some linguists contend that Japanese (and Japonic languages in general) consists of an Altaic superstratum projected onto an Austronesian substratum. Some scholars also argue for the existence of Altaic superstrate influences on varieties of Chinese spoken in Northern China. In this case, however, the superstratum refers to influence, not language succession. Other views detect substrate effects.

Adstratum

This section does not cite any sources. Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed. (July 2019) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

An adstratum (plural: adstrata) or adstrate is a language that influences another language by virtue of geographic proximity, not by virtue of its relative prestige. For example, early in England's history, Old Norse served as an adstrate, contributing to the lexical structure of Old English.

The phenomenon is less common today in standardized linguistic varieties and more common in colloquial forms of speech. Modern nations tend to favour a single linguistic variety, often corresponding to the dialect of the capital and other important regions, over others.

In India, where dozens of languages are widespread, many languages could be said to share an adstratal relationship, but Hindi is certainly a dominant adstrate in North India.

A different example would be the sociolinguistic situation in Belgium, where the French and Dutch languages have roughly the same status. They could justifiably be called adstrates to each other as each has provided a large set of lexical specifications to the other.

The term adstratum is also used to identify systematic influences or a layer of borrowings in a given language from another language, independently of whether the two languages continue coexisting as separate entities. Many modern languages have an appreciable adstratum from English, due both to the cultural influence and economic preponderance of the United States on international markets, and the earlier colonization by the British Empire that made English a global lingua franca. The Greek and Latin coinages adopted by European languages, including English and now languages worldwide, to describe scientific topics, sociology, medicine, anatomy, biology, all the '-logy' words, etc., are also justifiably called adstrata.

Another example is found in Spanish and Portuguese, which contain a heavy Semitic, particularly Arabic, adstratum. Yiddish is a linguistic variety of High German with adstrata from Hebrew and Aramaic, mostly in the sphere of religion. Slavic languages were linked geographically to Yiddish-speaking villages in Eastern Europe for centuries up until the Holocaust.

Notable examples of possible substrate or superstrate influence

Substrate influence on superstrate

Area Resultant language Substrate Superstrate Superstrate introduced by
China (Baiyue), Northern Vietnam Yue (Viet), Min, Au, Wu Various Old Yue languages Old Chinese Sinicisation (Qin's campaign against the Yue tribes, Han campaigns against Minyue, and Southward expansion of the Han dynasty), between the first millennium BC and the first millennium AD
Levant Levantine Arabic Western Aramaic Pre-classical Arabic Arabs during the Muslim conquests
Egypt Egyptian Arabic Coptic
Mesopotamia Mesopotamian Arabic Eastern Aramaic
Maghreb (North Africa) Maghrebi Arabic Berber languages, Punic, Vandalic, and Vulgar Latin
Ethiopia Amharic Central Cushitic languages South Semitic languages Bronze Age Semitic expansion
Eritrea/Ethiopia Tigrinya, Tigré and Ge'ez Central Cushitic and North Cushitic languages
England Old English Common Brittonic and British Latin Ingvaeonic languages Anglo-Saxon settlement of Britain
Cornwall Cornish English Cornish Early Modern English Anglicisation of Cornish people
Ireland Irish English Irish the English during the Plantations of Ireland in the 16th century
Scotland Scottish English Middle Scots and Scottish Gaelic the English during Scottish Reformation in the 16th century
Jamaica Jamaican Patois African languages of transported enslaved Africans the English during British colonial rule in Jamaica
Singapore Singaporean Mandarin Southern Chinese varieties: Min Nan, Teochew, Cantonese, Hainanese Standard Mandarin Singapore Government during the Speak Mandarin Campaign
France Gallo-Romance Gaulish Vulgar Latin Romans who annexed it to the Roman Empire (1st century BC-7th century AD),
Portugal Ibero-Romance Paleohispanic languages
Spain
Romania Common Romanian Daco-Thracian
Canary Islands Canarian Spanish Guanche Andalusian Spanish Andalusians during the incorporation of the Canary Islands into the Crown of Castile
Mexico Mexican Spanish Nahuatl and other indigenous languages of Mexico Spanish of the 15th century Spaniards during the Spanish Conquest
of the 15th century
Central Andes Andean Spanish Quechua, Aymaran languages
Paraguay Paraguayan Spanish Guaraní
Philippines Chavacano Tagalog, Ilokano, Hiligaynon, Cebuano, Bangingi, Sama, Tausug, Yakan, and Malay
Brazil Brazilian Portuguese Tupi, Bantu languages Portuguese of the 15th century the Portuguese during the colonial period
Angola Angolan Portuguese Umbundu, Kimbundu, and Kikongo the Portuguese during the colonial rule in Africa
Israel Modern Hebrew German, Russian, Yiddish,
Judeo-Arabic dialects, and other Jewish languages and languages spoken by Jews
Hebrew constructed from Biblical and mishnaic Hebrew European Jewish immigrants in the late 19th and early 20th centuries who modernized and reintroduced Hebrew as a vernacular
Shetland and Orkney Insular Scots Norn Scots Acquisition by Scotland in the 15th century
Norway Bokmål Old Norwegian Danish Union with Danish crown, 1380–1814.
Argentina/Uruguay Rioplatense Spanish Neapolitan, various Italian Languages Spanish Italian immigration to Uruguay and Argentina
Belarus Belarusian Baltic languages Old East Slavic Assimilation of East Balts by East Slavs in the Middle Ages
Russia (Russian North) North Russian Finno-Ugric languages Russian Russification of the Chudes and Volga Finns

Superstrate influence on substrate

Area Resultant language Substrate Superstrate Superstrate introduced by
France Old French Gallo-Romance Frankish Merovingians' dominance of Gaul around 500
England Middle English Old English Old Norman Normans during the Norman conquest
Greece Demotic Greek Medieval Greek Ottoman Turkish Ottoman Turks following the Fall of Constantinople and during the subsequent occupation of Greece
Spain Early Modern Spanish Old Spanish Arabic (by way of Mozarabic) Umayyads during the conquest of Hispania, and the Arabic and Mozarabic speakers in al-Andalus who were absorbed into Castille and other Christian kingdoms during the Reconquista
Malta Maltese Siculo-Arabic Sicilian, later Italian and other Romance languages Norman and Aragonese control, establishment of the Knights of St. John on the islands in the 16th century
Romania, Moldova Modern Romanian Common Romanian, Old Romanian Slavic languages (first Proto-Slavic, then Old Church Slavonic, and later individual Slavic languages such as Ukrainian, Polish, Russian, Serbian, and Bulgarian) Slavic migrations to the Balkans, rule by the Bulgarian, Polish-Lithuanian, and Russian Empires
Poland (Kashubia) Kashubian Pomeranian Low German German immigration to Pomerania during the Ostsiedlung, and periods of Teutonic and Prussian rule

See also

References

  1. "Why Don't the English Speak Welsh?" Hildegard Tristram, chapter 15 in The Britons in Anglo-Saxon England, N. J. Higham (ed.), The Boydell Press 2007 ISBN 1843833123, pp. 192–214.
  2. Saarikivi, Janne (2006). Substrata Uralica: Studies on Finno-Ugrian substrate influence in Northern Russian dialects (Ph.D.). University of Helsinki. pp. 12–14.
  3. Giovanni Battista Pellegrini, "Substrata", in Romance Comparative and Historical Linguistics, ed. Rebecca Posner et al. (The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter, 1980), 65.
  4. Giovanni Battista Pellegrini, "Substrata", in Romance Comparative and Historical Linguistics, ed. Rebecca Posner et al. (The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter, 1980), 65.
  5. Henri Guiter, "Sur le substrat gaulois dans la Romania", in Munus amicitae. Studia linguistica in honorem Witoldi Manczak septuagenarii, eds., Anna Bochnakowa & Stanislan Widlak, Krakow, 1995.
  6. Eugeen Roegiest, Vers les sources des langues romanes: Un itinéraire linguistique à travers la Romania (Leuven, Belgium: Acco, 2006), 83.
  7. Pierre-Yves Lambert, La Langue gauloise (Paris: Errance, 1994), 46-7. ISBN 978-2-87772-224-7
  8. Pierre-Yves Lambert, La Langue gauloise (Paris: Errance, 1994), 158. ISBN 978-2-87772-224-7
  9. ^ Matasović, Ranko. 2007. “Insular Celtic as a Language Area”. In Tristam, Hildegard L.C. 2007, The Celtic Languages in Contact. Bonn: Papers from the Workshop within the Framework of the XIII International Congress of Celtic Studies. Page 106.
  10. Savignac, Jean-Paul. 2004. Dictionnaire Français-Gaulois. Paris: La Différence. Pages 26, 294-5.
  11. Filppula, Markku, Klemola, Juhani and Paulasto, Heli. 2008. English and Celtic in Contact. New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group. Pages 77-82
  12. ^ Matasović, Ranko (2014). "Substratum words in Balto-Slavic". Filologija (60): 75–102.
  13. Ringe, Don (2009-01-06). "The Linguistic Diversity of Aboriginal Europe". Language Log. Retrieved 2017-09-30.
  14. ^ Leschber, Corinna (2016). "On the stratification of substratum languages". Etymology and the European Lexicon: Proceedings of the 14th Fachtagung der Indogermanischen Gesellschaft, 17–22 September 2012, Copenhagen. Wiesbaden: Reichert Verlag.
  15. ^ Schrijver, Peter (1997). "Animal, vegetable and mineral: some Western European substratum words". In Lubotsky, A. (ed.). Sound Law and Analogy. Amsterdam/Atlanta. pp. 293–316.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  16. Witzel, Michael (1999). "Early Sources for South Asian Substrate Languages" (PDF). Mother Tongue.
  17. Thomason, Sarah Grey; Kaufmann, Terrence (12 February 1992). Language Contact, Creolization, and Genetic Linguistics. University of California Press. pp. 1–3. ISBN 9780520912793.
  18. Hoyt, David L.; Ostlund, Karen (2006). The Study of Language and the Politics of Community in Global Context. Lexington Books. p. 103. ISBN 9780739109557.
  19. Benedict (1990), Lewin (1976), Matsumoto (1975), Miller (1967), Murayama (1976), Shibatani (1990).
  20. McWhorter, John (2007). "Mandarin Chinese: "Altaicization" or Simplification?". Language Interrupted: Signs of Non-Native Acquisition in Standard Language Grammars. Oxford University Press.
  21. Hashimoto (1986), Janhunen (1996), McWhorter (2007).
  22. For example, take replaced earlier niman in the lexical slot of a transitive verb for "to take", though archaic forms of to nim survived in England.
  23. The Genesis and Development of Brazilian Vernacular Portuguese Archived 2017-10-10 at the Wayback Machine Page 246, etc
  24. Lıngwa u lıngwıstıka. Borg, Karl. Valletta, Malta: Klabb Kotba Maltin. 1998. ISBN 99909-75-42-6. OCLC 82586980.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: others (link)
  25. Brincat, Joseph M. (2000). Il-Malti, elf sena ta' storja. Malta: Pubblikazzjonijiet Indipendenza. ISBN 99909-41-68-8. OCLC 223378429.

Further reading

  • Benedict, Paul K. (1990). Japanese/Austro-Tai. Ann Arbor: Karoma.
  • Cravens, Thomas D. (1994). "Substratum". The Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, ed. by R. E. Asher et al. Vol. 1, pp. 4396–4398. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
  • Hashimoto, Mantaro J. (1986). "The Altaicization of Northern Chinese". Contributions to Sino-Tibetan studies, eds John McCoy & Timothy Light, 76–97. Leiden: Brill.
  • Janhunen, Juha (1996). Manchuria: An Ethnic History. Helsinki: Finno-Ugrian Society.
  • Jungemann, Frédéric H. (1955). La teoría del substrato y los dialectos Hispano-romances y gascones. Madrid.
  • Lewin, Bruno (1976). "Japanese and Korean: The Problems and History of a Linguistic Comparison". Journal of Japanese Studies 2:2.389–412
  • Matsumoto, Katsumi (1975). "Kodai nihongoboin soshikikõ: naiteki saiken no kokoromi". Bulletin of the Faculty of Law and Letters (Kanazawa University) 22.83–152.
  • McWhorter, John (2007). Language Interrupted: Signs of Non-Native Acquisition in Standard Language Grammars. USA: Oxford University Press.
  • Miller, Roy Andrew (1967). The Japanese language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Murayama, Shichiro (1976). "The Malayo-Polynesian Component in the Japanese Language". Journal of Japanese Studies 2:2.413–436
  • Shibatani, Masayoshi (1990). The languages of Japan. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.
  • Singler, John Victor (1983). "The influence of African languages on pidgins and creoles". Current Approaches to African Linguistics (vol. 2), ed. by J. Kaye et al., 65–77. Dordrecht.
  • Singler, John Victor (1988). "The homogeneity of the substrate as a factor in pidgin/creole genesis". Language 64.27–51.
  • Vovin, Alexander (1994). "Long-distance relationships, reconstruction methodology and the origins of Japanese". Diachronica 11:1.95–114.
  • Wartburg, Walter von (1939). Réponses au Questionnaire du Ve Congrès international des Linguistes. Bruges.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  • Weinreich, Uriel (1979) . Languages in contact: findings and problems. New York: Mouton Publishers. ISBN 978-90-279-2689-0.
Categories: