Misplaced Pages

Identification studies of UFOs: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 23:18, 21 November 2008 editCydebot (talk | contribs)6,812,251 editsm Robot - Moving category UFO-related vehicles to Alleged UFOs per CFD at Misplaced Pages:Categories for discussion/Log/2008 November 12.← Previous edit Latest revision as of 10:12, 3 November 2024 edit undoCommonsDelinker (talk | contribs)Bots, Template editors1,015,473 edits Replacing Chinese_surveillance_balloon_over_Billings,_MT.jpg with File:Chinese_balloon_over_Billings,_MT-1_February_2023.jpg (by CommonsDelinker because: File renamed: Criterion 3 (obvio 
(252 intermediate revisions by 91 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|none}}
{{POV|date=June 2008}}
{{Mergeto|Unidentified flying object|date=June 2008}} {{More citations needed|date=April 2020}}
{{Ufo}}
{{Need-Consensus|date=May 2008}}
{{otheruses4||the album|Identified Flying Object||}}


Identifying ]s (UFOs) is a difficult task due to the normally poor quality of the evidence provided by those who report ] the unknown object.<ref>{{cite book
An '''Identified Flying Object''', or '''IFO''', is any unusual or puzzling object or optical phenomenon observed in the sky which has been identified as a known or conventional phenomenon after being investigated by qualified persons. This is in contrast to an ], or UFO, which has not been identified following investigation.
|title=The Cosmos: Astronomy in the New Millennium
|author=Pasachoff, Jay M and Alex Filippenko
|year=2004
|publisher=Brooks/Cole div. of Thomson Learning |isbn=0-534-39550-3
|pages=428
|quote=bservations are usually anecdotal, are not controlled in a scientific experiment, and are not accessible to study by sophisticated instruments.
}}</ref> Observations and subsequent reporting are often made by those untrained in astronomy, atmospheric phenomena, aeronautics, physics, and perception.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Nickell |first1=Joe |author-link1=Joe Nickell |last2=McGaha |first2=James |title=UFO Identification Process |journal=] |date=2018 |volume=42 |issue=6 |pages=34–37 |url=https://www.csicop.org/si/show/ufo_identification_process |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20181223143834/https://www.csicop.org/si/show/ufo_identification_process |access-date=23 December 2018|archive-date=2018-12-23 }}</ref> Nevertheless, most officially investigated UFO sightings, such as from the ]'s ], have been identified as being due to honest misidentifications of natural phenomena, aircraft, or other prosaic explanations. In early U.S. Air Force attempts to explain UFO sightings, unexplained sightings routinely numbered over one in five reports. However, in early 1953, right after the CIA's ], percentages of unexplained sightings dropped precipitously, usually being only a few percent in any given year. When Project Blue Book closed down in 1970, only 6% of all cases were classified as being truly unidentified.


UFOs that can be explained are sometimes termed '''identified flying objects''' ('''IFOs''').
==UFO studies and result differences==


== UFO studies ==
It has been estimated from various studies (such as those cited below) that 50-90% of all reported UFO sightings are eventually identified, while 10-20% remain unidentified (the rest being "garbage cases" listed as having "insufficient information" to enable classification). Various studies (such as the U.S. Air Force's ]) have also shown that only a small percentage of UFO reports are deliberate hoaxes (typically less than 1%). Instead, the vast majority are honest misidentifications of natural or man-made phenomena.


The following are some major studies undertaken during the past 70+ years that reported on identification of UFOs:
The actual percentage of IFOs vs. UFOs depends on who is doing the study and can vary widely depending on criteria and cases examined. Politics can also play an important role. For example, in early U.S. Air Force UFO studies such as ], the unknowns were consistently over 20%. However, in early 1953, right after the CIA's debunking ], the USAF issued Air Force Regulation 200-2 ordering the unknowns reduced to a minimum. As a result, percentages of unknowns dropped precipitously, usually being only a few percent in any given year. When Blue Book closed down in 1970, only 6% of cases overall were now classified as unknowns (the vast majority of the final unknowns arising from before the issuance of AFR 200-2, which ordered Air Force personnel to publicly discuss only IFOs and not "unidentifieds."
* ''']'''
* ''']''' (referred to further below as '''BBSR''') was a massive statistical study the ] did for the ] of 3,200 UFO cases between 1952 and 1954. Of these, 22% were classified as unidentified ("true UFOs"). Another 69% were deemed identified (IFOs). There was insufficient information to make a determination in the remaining 9%.
* The official French government UFO investigation '''(GEPAN/SEPRA)''', run within the French space agency ] between 1977 and 2004, scientifically investigated about 6000 cases and found that 13% defied any rational explanation (UFOs), while 46% were deemed readily identifiable and 41%, lacked sufficient information for classification.
* The USAF-sponsored ''']''' study reported that all 117 cases studied were or could probably be explained. A 1971 review of the results by the ] concluded that 30% of the 117 cases remained unexplained.{{Citation needed|date=April 2020}}
* Of about 5,000 cases submitted to and studied by the civilian UFO organization ''']''', 16% were judged unknowns.{{Citation needed|date=April 2020}}
* The ''']''''','' (or colloquially known as the Pentagon UFO Report) is a United States federally mandated assessment summarizing information regarding ] (UFOs), also known as unidentified aerial phenomenon (UAPs). On June 25, 2021, a nine-page preliminary assessment was issued. It states that the UAPTF focused on 144 observations of "unidentified aerial phenomena" by the ], mostly from ] personnel, from 2004 to 2021. No details are given in the preliminary assessment. The report found that the UAPTF was unable to identify 143 of these objects, but it included five categories of potential explanations; "Airborne Clutter" includes objects like birds and balloons, "Natural Atmospheric Phenomena" includes atmospheric effect like ice crystals, "USG or Industry Developmental Programs" includes US military technology, "Foreign Adversary Systems" includes technologies developed by foreign governments such as Russia or China, and "Other", described as a "catchall" for other explanations.<ref name="dni_Preliminary_Assessment">{{Cite web |title=Preliminary Assessment: Unidentified Aerial Phenomena |publisher=] |date=June 25, 2021 |access-date=June 25, 2021 |url= https://www.dni.gov/files/ODNI/documents/assessments/Prelimary-Assessment-UAP-20210625.pdf}}</ref>
* In contrast, much more conservative numbers for the percentage of UFOs were arrived at individually by astronomer ], who was the chief investigator for the ] (CUFOS). CUFOS was founded by astronomer ] (who had been a consultant for the Air Force's Project Blue Book) to provide a serious scientific investigation into UFOs. Hendry spent 15 months personally investigating 1,307 UFO reports. In 1979, Hendry published his conclusions in ''The UFO Handbook: A Guide to Investigating, Evaluating, and Reporting UFO Sightings''.<ref>{{Cite book|last=Hendry, Allan.|title=The UFO handbook : a guide to investigating, evaluating, and reporting UFO sightings|date=1979|publisher=Doubleday|isbn=0-385-14348-6|edition=1st|location=Garden City, N.Y.|oclc=4642190}}</ref> Hendry admitted that he would like to find evidence for extraterrestrials but noted that the vast majority of cases had prosaic explanations. He found 89% of reports definitely or probably identifiable and only 9% unidentified. "Hardcore" cases—well-documented events which defied any conceivable conventional explanation—made up only 1.5% of the reports.


=== Project Sign ===
The following are some major scientific studies undertaken during the past 50 years and the proportion of IFOs vs. UFOs:
{{Main|Project Sign}}
*] (referred to further below as '''BBSR''') was a massive statistical study the ] did for the ] of 3,200 UFO cases between 1952 and 1954. Of these, 22% remained unidentified (“true UFOs”), using the stringent criteria that all four scientific analysts had to agree that the case had no prosaic explanation, whereas agreement of only two analysts was needed to list the case as explained. Another 69% were deemed identified (IFOs), and for the remainder, 9%, there was insufficient information to make a determination.
Project Sign lists that "in order to investigate the credibility of their existence the following factors must be considered in any technical analysis":<ref>]</rorce)</ref>
*The official French government UFO investigation (GEPAN/SEPRA), run within the French space agency ] between 1977 and 2004, scientifically investigated about 6000 cases and found about 13% defied any rational explanation (UFOs), while about 46% were deemed readily identifiable, or IFOs. (The remainder, or 41%, lacked sufficient information.)
*When the ] in 1971 reviewed the results of the 1966-1969 USAF-sponsored ] study, 30% of the 117 cases remained unexplained. (This is another example of politics clearly affecting outcomes. Condon had claimed in his summary that all the cases studied were or could probably be explained.)
*Of about 5,000 cases submitted to and studied by the civilian UFO organization ], 16% were judged unknowns.


Method of support (])
In contrast, much more conservative numbers for the percentage of UFOs were arrived at individually by astronomer ], who was the chief investigator for the ] (CUFOS). CUFOS was founded by astronomer ] (who had been a consultant for the Air Force’s Project Blue Book) to provide a serious scientific investigation into UFOs. Hendry spent 15 months personally investigating 1,307 UFO reports.


# ]
In 1979, Hendry published his conclusions in ''The UFO Handbook: A Guide to Investigating, Evaluating, and Reporting UFO Sightings''. Hendry admitted that he would like to find evidence for extraterrestrials but noted that the vast majority of cases had prosaic explanations. He deemed 89% IFOs and only 9% unidentified. If only “hardcore” cases -- well-documented events which defied any conceivable conventional explanation -- the figure for UFOs dropped to only 1.5%.
# Fuselage lift (])
# ]
# ]
# ]
# ] (lighter-than-air craft)


Method of propulsion (])
One possible reason for Hendry's more conservative results might be that he was operating from a very different set of data. Hendry examined almost exclusively civilian reports, mostly from inexperienced witnesses. In contrast, government studies, such as the U.S. Project Blue Book or the French GEPAN/SEPRA, or the civilian NICAP study, contained large numbers of civilian and military pilot sightings and other military sightings, usually considered to be higher evidentiary cases because of the greater experience of the witnesses and the presence of corroborating data such as radar.


# ]
As an example of the difference, military personnel made up only 1% of Hendry's witnesses, but 38% of the Battelle / Air Force study. The military witnesses also contributed a much higher percentage of “excellent” or “good” cases (58% for the military vs. only 33% for the civilian cases), which were more likely to be judged unknowns in the Battelle study. Overall, 29% of military cases were judged as unknowns vs. 17% for civilian cases.
# ]
# ]
# ] (for example, the Katzmayer effect,<ref>https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc53897/m2/1/high_res_d/19930081006.pdf {{Bare URL PDF|date=March 2022}}</ref> defined as "the reduction in drag of an airfoil when an air stream is oscillating").


=== Project Blue Book Special Report No. 14 ===
Because the results for the Battelle BBSR study and Hendry’s CUFOS study are readily available and contain many statistical breakdowns of cases, they will be contrasted in detail below.
{{Main|Project Blue Book Special Report No. 14}}
] was compiled between 1951 and 1954, and included 3201 reported UFO sightings. Battelle employed four scientific analysts, who sought to divide cases into "knowns", "unknowns", and a third category of "insufficient information." They also broke down knowns and unknowns into four categories of quality, from excellent to poor. In order for a case to be deemed "identified", two analysts had to independently agree on a solution and for a case to be called "unidentified", all four analysts had to agree. A report classified as "unidentified" was defined as: "Those reports of sightings wherein the description of the object and its maneuvers could not be fitted to the pattern of any known object or phenomenon."


Out of 3,201 cases, 69% were judged to be ''identified'', 22% were ''unidentified'', and 9% had insufficient information to make a determination.{{Citation needed|date=April 2020}}
==Battelle Memorial Institute breakdown of cases==
Out of 3,201 cases, 69% were judged to be '''identified''', 22% were ], and 9% had insufficient information to make a determination. A report classified as "unidentified" was defined as: "Those reports of sightings wherein the description of the object and its maneuvers could not be fitted to the pattern of any known object or phenomenon."


==== Breakdown by category of IFO and case quality ====
Only two of four scientific analysts had to agree for a case to be listed as an IFO, but all four analysts had to agree for it to be judged a UFO. About twice as many of the excellent cases were judged UFOs as the poorest cases. The difference was accounted for mostly by cases judged having “insufficient information”, which was only 4% for the best cases but 21% for the worst. Quality of cases didn’t seem to have much effect on the various category percentages for the IFOs, except in the “psychological” category, in which the poorest cases had much higher relative rates.

===Breakdown by category of IFO and case quality===


{|class="wikitable" align="right" style="margin-left: 0.25em" {|class="wikitable" align="right" style="margin-left: 0.25em"
Line 59: Line 77:
|} |}


BBSR further broke these results down based on whether the identification was considered certain or merely doubtful. For example, in both the ''astronomical'' and ''aircraft'' IFO categories, 12+% were considered certain and 9+% were doubtful. Overall, of the 69% listed as IFOs, 42% were thought to be solved with certainty, while 27% were still considered doubtful. BBSR further broke these results down based on whether the identification was considered certain or merely doubtful. For example, in both the ''astronomical'' and ''aircraft'' IFO categories, 12% were considered certain and 9% were doubtful. Overall, of the 69% listed as IFOs, 42% were thought to be solved with certainty, while 27% were still considered doubtful.


In addition, if a case was lacking in adequate data, it was placed in the ''insufficient information'' category, separate from both IFOs and UFOs. In addition, if a case was lacking in adequate data, it was placed in the ''insufficient information'' category, separate from both IFOs and UFOs.


==== Military vs. civilian breakdown ====
Based on the Battelle statistical analysis, the percentages of unknowns increased significantly with the quality of the cases. This is contrary to the contention of some skeptical analyses, such as the 1953 ] ], that the remaining unidentified cases were the result of poor quality reports or insufficient information.

===Military vs. civilian breakdown===


{| class="wikitable" align="left" {| class="wikitable" align="left"
Line 71: Line 87:
! align="center" width="100px" colspan=3 | IFO ! align="center" width="100px" colspan=3 | IFO
! align="center" width="100px" colspan=3 | UFO ! align="center" width="100px" colspan=3 | UFO
! align="center" width="100px" colspan=3 | Insufficient Information ! align="center" width="150px" colspan=3 | Insufficient Information
|- |-
! || Mil || Civ || All || Mil || Civ || All || Mil || Civ || All ! || Mil || Civ || All || Mil || Civ || All || Mil || Civ || All
Line 78: Line 94:
|- |-
|Worst reports || 70% || 70% || 70% || 24% || 14% || 16% || 7% || 17% || 14% |Worst reports || 70% || 70% || 70% || 24% || 14% || 16% || 7% || 17% || 14%
|} |}


The Battelle BBSR study consisted of many internal military reports; fully 38% of the cases were designated as military. Military witnesses tended to submit better quality reports, had much fewer reports rated as having insufficient information, and had higher percentages of unknowns. As in the previous breakdown, the percentage of UFOs again rose with case quality for both the military and civilian subcategories. The Battelle BBSR study included many internal military reports; 38% of the cases were designated as military. Military witnesses tended to submit better quality reports, had much fewer reports rated as having insufficient information, and had higher percentages of unknowns. As in the previous breakdown, the percentage of UFOs again rose with case quality for both the military and civilian subcategories.


In the summary table, ''best reports'' are those rated excellent and good; ''worst reports'' are doubtful” and poor. In the summary table, ''best reports'' are those rated excellent and good; ''worst reports'' are doubtful and poor.


===Comparison of IFOs to UFOs by characteristics=== ==== Comparison of IFOs to UFOs by characteristics ====
A key study of BBSR was to statistically compare IFOs and UFOs by six characteristics: color, number of objects, shape, duration of observations, speed, and light brightness. If there were no significant differences, the two classes were probably the same, the UFOs then representing merely a failure to properly identify prosaic phenomena that could already account for the IFOs. On the other hand, if the differences were statistically significant, this would suggest IFOs and UFOs were indeed distinctly different phenomena. A key study of BBSR was to statistically compare IFOs and UFOs by six characteristics: color, number of objects, shape, duration of observations, speed, and light brightness. If there were no significant differences, the two classes were probably the same, the UFOs then representing merely a failure to properly identify prosaic phenomena that could already account for the IFOs. On the other hand, if the differences were statistically significant, this would suggest IFOs and UFOs were indeed distinctly different phenomena.


In the initial results, all characteristics except brightness tested significant at less or much less than 1% (brightness was greater than 5%). By removing "astronomical" sightings from the "knowns" and redoing the test, just two categories, number and speed, were significant at less than 1%, the remainder having results between 3% and 5%. This indicated that there was a statistically significant difference between the characteristics ascribed to UFOs and IFOs, but perhaps not as significant as the initial results suggested. However, for two characteristics, brightness and speed, the significance actually increased with the revised test. Furthermore, even with revision, statistically significant results for six independent characteristics is highly improbable (less than one in a billion, by multiplying all probabilities together), strongly suggesting there was something fundamentally different between the UFOs and IFOs <!-- -->. In the initial results, all characteristics except brightness tested significant at less or much less than 1% (brightness was greater than 5%). By removing "astronomical" sightings from the "knowns" and redoing the test, just two categories, number and speed, were significant at less than 1%, the remainder having results between 3% and 5%. This indicated that there was a statistically significant difference between the characteristics ascribed to UFOs and IFOs, but perhaps not as significant as the initial results suggested. For two characteristics, brightness and speed, the significance actually increased with the revised test.


==Allan Hendry study== === Allan Hendry study ===
Like the Air Force, astronomer ] found that only a small percentage of cases were hoaxes and that most sightings were actually honest misidentifications of prosaic phenomena. Hendry attributed most of these to inexperience or misperception. Like the Air Force, astronomer ] found that only a small percentage of cases were hoaxes and that most sightings were actually honest misidentifications of prosaic phenomena. Hendry attributed most of these to inexperience or misperception.


Out of 1,307 cases Hendry deemed 88.6% had clear prosaic explanations (IFOs) and only 8.6% were unknowns (UFOs). Of the UFOs, Hendry thought that 7.1%, might still have a prosaic explanation while 1.5% (20 cases) had no possible plausible explanation and were completely unexplained. The remaining miscellaneous cases (2.8%) were “garbage” cases, where Hendry deemed the witnesses unreliable, the reports hopelessly contradictory, or lacking in sufficient information. Out of 1,307 cases Hendry deemed 88.6% had clear prosaic explanations (IFOs) and only 8.6% were unknowns (UFOs). Of the UFOs, Hendry reported that 7.1%, might still have a prosaic explanation while 1.5% (20 cases) had no possible plausible explanation and were completely unexplained. The remaining miscellaneous cases (2.8%) were "garbage" cases, where Hendry deemed the witnesses unreliable, the reports hopelessly contradictory, or lacking in sufficient information.


Overall, in the three major categories, 42% of all cases had astronomical explanations, 37% were aircraft, and 5% were balloons. A further breakdown allowed 77% to be readily explained by five main classes of objects: 29% were bright stars or planets, 19% were advertising planes, 15% were other aircraft, 9% were meteors and reentering space debris, and 5% were balloons of various types (mostly weather or advertising balloons but also a few prank balloons). Overall, in the three major categories, 42% of all cases had astronomical explanations, 37% were aircraft, and 5% were balloons. A further breakdown allowed 77% to be readily explained by five main classes of objects: 29% were bright stars or planets, 19% were advertising planes, 15% were other aircraft, 9% were meteors and reentering space debris, and 5% were balloons of various types (mostly weather or advertising balloons but also a few prank balloons).


===Breakdown of cases=== ==== Breakdown of cases ====
Hendry also used a case classification system developed by his mentor Dr. J. Allen Hynek, who established ], where the study was carried out. In this summary table: Hendry also used a case classification system developed by his mentor J. Allen Hynek, who established ], where the study was carried out. In this summary table:
* NL = “Nocturnal Lights”, lights seen in the sky at night. * NL = "Nocturnal Lights", lights seen in the sky at night.
* DD = “Daylight Discs”, objects seen in daytime (but not necessarily disc in shape). * DD = "Daylight Discs", objects seen in daytime (but not necessarily disc in shape).
* RV = “Radar/Visual” cases, objects observed by both witnesses and radar. * RV = "Radar/Visual" cases, objects observed by both witnesses and radar.
* CE = “Close Encounter” cases. For convenience, CE cases listed below are combined totals of Hynek’s CE1, CE2, and CE3 cases, where: * CE = "Close Encounter" cases. For convenience, CE cases listed below are combined totals of Hynek's CE1, CE2, and CE3 cases, where:
** CE1 cases were objects thought to be seen up close (within 500 feet). ** CE1 cases where objects were thought to be seen up close (within 500 feet).
** CE2 had purported physical interactions with the environment (physical trace cases or electromagnetic interference). ** CE2 had purported physical interactions with the environment (physical trace cases or electromagnetic interference).
** CE3 cases were supposed to involve sightings of occupants. ** CE3 cases were supposed to involve sightings of occupants.
Line 143: Line 159:
|'''clouds, dust''' || 10 || 2 || 0 || 0 || 12 || 0.9% |'''clouds, dust''' || 10 || 2 || 0 || 0 || 12 || 0.9%
|- |-
|'''light phenomena''' (], ], ground lights, ]s, etc.) || 9 || 1 || 4 || 0 || 14 || 1.1% |'''light phenomena''' (], ], ground lights, ]s, etc.) || 9 || 1 || 4 || 0 || 14 || 1.1%
|- |-
|'''other''' (]s, flares, reflections, windblown debris, etc.) || 12 || 3 || 1 || 0 || 16 || 1.2% |'''other''' (]s, flares, reflections, windblown debris, etc.) || 12 || 3 || 1 || 0 || 16 || 1.2%
|- |-
| |
|- |-
|'''TOTALS IFOs''' || || || || || || |'''Total Identified''' || || || || || ||
|- |-
|Cases || 1024 || 71 || 58 || 5 || 1158 || 88.6% |Cases || 1024 || 71 || 58 || 5 || 1158 || 88.6%
Line 157: Line 173:
| |
|- |-
|'''TOTALS UFOs''' || || || || || || |'''Total Unidentified''' || || || || || ||
|- |-
|Cases || 79 || 18 || 16 || 0 || 113 || 8.6% |Cases || 79 || 18 || 16 || 0 || 113 || 8.6%
|- |-
|Percent || 6% || 1.4% || 1.2% || 0% || 8.6% || |Percent || 6% || 1.4% || 1.2% || 0% || 8.6% ||
|- |-
| |
Line 169: Line 185:
| |
|- |-
|'''TOTAL all cases''' || || || || || || |'''Total all cases''' || || || || || ||
|- |-
|Cases || 1103 || 89 || 74 || 5 || 1307 || 100% |Cases || 1103 || 89 || 74 || 5 || 1307 || 100%
Line 176: Line 192:
|} |}


== Common causes of misidentification and UFOs ==
==Analysis==
]|thumb]]
===Common causes of misidentification and IFOs===
Both BBSR and Hendry found that the vast majority of IFOs were caused by three classes of objects or phenomena: Astronomical, aircraft, or balloons. Of all IFOs, 86% were accounted for by these three groups in the BBSR study vs. 83% in the Hendry study. However, there were significant differences in the percentages attributed to each group:
*Astronomical: BBSR = 22%; Hendry = 42%. Since BBSR predated satellites, subtracting these out of Hendry’s results leaves 40%. In Hendry’s study, bright stars and planets, like ], made up 29% of all cases while ] (and to a much lesser extent, re-entering space debris) made up 9%.
*Aircraft: BBSR = 22%; Hendry = 37%. The high percentage of military people and pilots in the BBSR study may have something to do with the far lower aircraft misidentification values. Hendry had large numbers of advertising plane misidentifications (19%), perhaps reflecting a study bias toward urban centers, particularly Chicago, where CUFOS is located. Hovering aircraft such as helicopters or blimps, or aircraft that appear to be hovering, such as airplanes seen at night from the front with their headlights on as they approach for landing can often confuse the inexperienced witness, as can aircraft strobe lights.
*Balloons: BBSR = 15%; Hendry = 5%. Possibly the large numbers of big experimental balloons, such as the ], launched in the period that BBSR studied, contributed to the higher BBSR percentage.


Both BBSR and Hendry found that three classes of objects or phenomena—astronomical, aircraft, or balloons—accounted for a large majority of identifiable UFO reports (referred to as IFOs), 86% and 83% in the two studies. For example, in Hendry's study, bright ]s and ] made up 29% of all cases while ] (and to a much lesser extent, re-entering space debris) made up 9%. Hovering aircraft such as helicopters or ], or aircraft that appear to be hovering, such as airplanes seen at night from the front with their headlights on as they approach for landing can often confuse witnesses, as can aircraft strobe lights. BBSR reported a much higher percentage of balloons than Hendry.
===Rare causes of misidentification===
*Birds: BBSR = 1%; Hendry = 0.5%
*Light phenomena: BBSR = 2%; Hendry = 1.1%. Might include ]s, ], ], ], ground lights such as street lights, and ] reflected off of clouds. Extremely rare light phenomena such as possible ] or ] may very rarely trigger UFO reports.
*Clouds, dust, fog, etc.: BBSR = 0.4%; Hendry = 0.9%. Might include unusual cloud formations such as ]s, ]s, rainbow effects, and high-altitude ice crystals.
*Other causes: BBSR = 5%; Hendry = 1.2%. Hendry mentioned some of these, such as kites, flares, reflections off windows, and windborn debris.


] at sunset]]
===Some misperceptions===
Light distortion from air turbulence can cause celestial bodies to move to a limited degree as can a visual perceptual effect called the ], caused by small, involuntary eye movements after staring at a star-like light against a black background without a frame of reference. To some observers, these may cause stars and planets to appear to start and stop, change direction, or dart around.


Claims of misidentification are after-the-fact analyses, not direct observations, and are often misconstrued by skeptics and UFO advocates alike: They do not suggest that the experiences did not exist, but merely that they can be explained by prosaic causes. For instance, retrospective analyses of the ] of 1969 connect the sighting with the known position of the planet Venus for that time, date, and location.<ref> It is worth noting that Carter himself never claimed that the sighting was anything more than an odd aerial or electrical phenomenon</ref> ], a strong advocate of the ] (ETH), claimed to have been fooled by the planet Venus when he was a fighter pilot, thinking it a distant enemy plane, and the 1967 "flying cross" of Devon, England<ref>{{cite web| url = http://www.ianridpath.com/ufo/flyingcross.htm| title = Devon flying cross UFO of 1967| access-date = 2007-10-02| author = Ian Ridpath}}</ref> and the 1966 ] case<ref>{{cite web|url=http://ufocasebook.com/portage.html|title=The Portage County, Ohio, UFO Chase – UFO Casebook Files|work=ufocasebook.com}}</ref> have both been associated with astronomical sources.
Hendry and many skeptics often attribute regular patterns such as “figure eights,” “meandering in a square pattern,” or “falling leaf motion” to these mechanisms and dismiss the sighting as an IFO, but this is far from certain. Movement caused by atmospheric effects or autokinesis is erratic and very limited in range. Therefore, in principle, such effects cannot cause true regular geometric motion such as a square pattern or figure eight, although perhaps those with vivid imaginations may attribute such patterns to random motion. In the case of autokinesis, the effect is very temporary and is destroyed by refixating one’s gaze. Also autokinesis can happen only in the absence of nearby objects. Thus if somebody was viewing a bright object near a visible horizon or in a field of nearby stars, it is very unlikely that autokinesis would be the cause of perceived motion.


In 2009, Peter Davenport, Director of the ], posted this complaint online:
Atmospheric distortion also tends to cause motion limited to only few seconds of a degree (causing the familiar twinkling of a star), and autokinesis is typically less than one degree. As Hendry himself noted, very large and prolonged excursions of motion would rule out either mechanism as a suitable explanation.
{{Quotation|We are receiving hundreds of reports every month of normal, terrestrial events, e.g. over-flights of the International Space Station, the Space Shuttle, or satellites; "flares" of light from "Iridium" satellites; the appearance of typical meteors; and observations of normal, "twinkling" stars, planets, contrails, clusters of balloons, etc.&nbsp; In fact, the overwhelming majority of reports that we receive now are of these normal objects and events, and processing the reports is taking a huge amount of our time... I believe the majority of time I spend on the Hotline is devoted to trying to convince people who have been staring for hours at a star or planet that the object of interest is not a UFO.<ref>Davenport, Peter. {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130408233941/http://www.nuforc.org/Statement090830.html |date=April 8, 2013 }}</ref>}}

There are several natural and man-made objects that are commonly suggested as explanations for UFO sightings:

=== Venus ===
] of ]|thumb]]
With the exception of the sun, moon and the ], ] is the brightest object in the sky and is often visible in the early morning and evening sky. Even experienced witnesses, especially when they are in unfamiliar surroundings or unusual atmospheric conditions, can fail to identify Venus correctly; however, the location of Venus is easily calculable, and professional astronomers have said that many of the UFO reports received from concerned citizens are due to observations of Venus.<ref>Paschoff and Filipenko</ref> Astronomer ], in particular, has suggested that Venus is responsible for the majority of all UFO reports<ref>(Plait, 205)</ref>

=== Meteors ===
]|left|thumb]]
The brightest meteors known as ]s are long lasting fireballs that leave a trail in the sky which can be visible for up to an hour after passing. Such events are relatively rare but can be witnessed by a large area of the Earth since most events occur kilometers up in the atmosphere. Those witnessing such events who are not familiar with meteors can be easily fooled into thinking that the meteor is a UFO. Because meteors are not predictable with the same degree of accuracy as planets, stars, or man-made objects such as satellites, these occurrences are more difficult to prove in retrospect, though UFO sightings during meteor showers, or where there are astronomical reports of bolides, are likely to be explained as such.

=== Balloons, aircraft, satellites and other man-made objects ===
]|thumb]]<!--
] balloon|thumb]]
-->

Many reports capture conventional, man-made objects. A ] has been postulated as an explanation for the Mantell UFO Incident, which led to the death of ].

Project Loon was a Google secret effort to bring internet services to isolated areas. Test balloons of different shapes were used and reported as UFOs. The Google Project Manager stated in an article in that the team used UFO reports to track the balloons progress for one of the launches.

Aircraft shapes have radically changed as we continue to develop stealth technologies. These new test designs appear unusual and cause reports. The CIA released a report indicating that many UFO reports in the 1950s were classified aircraft like the SR-71 and U-2.{{citation needed|reason=CIA report described yet no reference is provided|date=February 2020}}

The increase in civilian drone usage and popularity throughout the 2010s may be moving identification of objects away from UFOs and towards drones.<ref>{{cite web |url=https://slate.com/technology/2019/01/drone-paranoia-gatwick-heathrow-airport-sightings-ufos.html |title=Drones Are the New Flying Saucers |last=Greenwood |first=Faine |date=16 January 2019 |website=Slate |access-date=20 February 2020}}</ref>

=== Lenticular clouds ===
]s have been reported as UFOs due to their peculiar shape.]]

These stationary cloud formations often appear above mountains, but can happen when winds and "eddies" help shape clouds into lens shaped clouds and people see these as "flying saucers".

=== Misperception ===
]s in the ]|thumb]]

Light distortion from air turbulence can cause celestial bodies to move to a limited degree as can a visual perceptual effect called the ], caused by small, involuntary eye movements after staring at a star-like light against a black background without a frame of reference. To some observers, these may cause stars and planets to appear to start and stop, change direction, or dart around. Hendry and other UFO skeptics attribute complex patterns of apparent motion in UFO reports to the ].<ref>Ridpath, Ian. </ref> Its opposite, autostasis<ref>{{Cite journal|url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.2466/pms.1968.26.2.417|doi = 10.2466/pms.1968.26.2.417|title = A Case of "Autostasis" or Reverse Autokinesis|year = 1968|last1 = Wertheimer|first1 = Michael|journal = Perceptual and Motor Skills|volume = 26|issue = 2|pages = 417–418|pmid = 5654860|s2cid = 39932855}}</ref> is one of the causes as well.

Another type of misperceived motion sometimes occurs when people are driving in a vehicle. Witnesses may believe the "UFO" was following them even though the celestial body was actually stationary. Even police and other normally reliable witnesses can occasionally be fooled by sightings of bright stars and planets.

In about 10% of Hendry's cases caused by celestial bodies, witnesses greatly underestimated distances to the objects, giving distance estimates ranging from 200 feet to 125 miles (60&nbsp;m to 200&nbsp;km).


According to Hendry, moving clouds may also sometimes confuse observers by creating induced motion. Hendry believes this occasionally makes observers also believe objects have suddenly disappeared or make a rapid departure. According to Hendry, moving clouds may also sometimes confuse observers by creating induced motion. Hendry believes this occasionally makes observers also believe objects have suddenly disappeared or make a rapid departure.


==== Fata Morgana ====
Another type of misperceived motion sometimes occurs when people are driving in a vehicle. Witnesses may believe the “UFO” was following them even though the celestial body was actually stationary. Even police and other normally reliable witnesses can occasionally be fooled by sightings of bright stars and planets.
{{Main|Fata Morgana (mirage)}}


] of a boat below the horizon produces the illusion of a solid form floating in the sky.]]
Similarly, in about 10% of Hendry’s cases caused by celestial bodies, witnesses greatly underestimated distances to the objects, giving distance estimates ranging from 200 feet to 125 miles (60 m to 200 km).
Fata Morgana is a type of ] responsible for some UFO sightings, by making objects located ''below'' the astronomical ] appear to be hovering in the sky. It also magnifies images and makes them look unrecognizable.


The UFOs seen on ] can also be due to Fata Morgana, since water vapor in the air can create radar mirages more readily than temperature inversions can create optical mirages. According to GEPAN/SEPRA, the official UFO investigation in France,
Reentering space debris or meteors may appear as a string of lights. This can occasionally be misinterpreted as lights coming from windows, creating the illusion of a spacecraft. However, such mistakes are actually extremely rare. The effect was first noted in a widely observed 1969 re-entry of a Soviet satellite. The Air Force collected hundreds of reports from witnesses. From these, debunker and astronomer ] found three anecdotal cases where witnesses reported the effect. (''UFO’s, A Scientific Debate'', 155-161). This suggests that perhaps only 1% of all witnesses actually interpret similar events in this way. It is thus a mistake to assert that all reports of elongated objects with windows are due to misidentified meteor trains or space debris.


{{Quotation|
===Venus as an IFO===
As is well known, atmospheric ducting is the explanation for certain optical mirages, and in particular the arctic illusion called "fata morgana" where distant ocean or surface ice, which is essentially flat, appears to the viewer in the form of vertical columns and spires, or "castles in the air." <br />People often assume that mirages occur only rarely. This may be true of optical mirages, but conditions for radar mirages are more common, due to the role played by water vapor which strongly affects the atmospheric refractivity in relation to radio waves. Since clouds are closely associated with high levels of water vapor, optical mirages due to water vapor are often rendered undetectable by the accompanying opaque cloud. On the other hand, radar propagation is essentially unaffected by the water droplets of the cloud so that changes in water vapor content with altitude are very effective in producing atmospheric ducting and radar mirages.<ref name="Official UFO Investigations in France: the GEPAN/SEPRA Project"> {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20081022004627/http://www.scientificexploration.org/jse/articles/ufo_reports/sturrock/5.html |date=2008-10-22 }} BY V. R. ESHLEMAN</ref>}}
Because Venus is the brightest object in the sky (except for the sun and moon) it is frequently misidentified. Contributing to this, Venus is often visible in the early evening and morning sky, and thus seen by many people. Even experienced witnesses, especially when they are in unfamiliar surroundings or atmospheric conditions are unusual, may be confused, at least temporarily.


Fata Morgana was named as a hypothesis for the mysterious Australian phenomenon ].<ref name=perrigrew1>{{cite journal |last1=Pettigrew |first1=JD. |date=2003 |title=The Min Min light and the Fata Morgana. An optical account of a mysterious Australian phenomenon. |journal=Clinical and Experimental Optometry |volume=86 |issue=2 |pages=109–20 |doi=10.1111/j.1444-0938.2003.tb03069.x |pmid=12643807 |doi-access=free}}<!--|access-date=23 March 2013 --></ref>
For example, ] ], himself a strong advocate of the ], related that he had once been momentarily fooled by the planet Venus when he was a fighter pilot, thinking it a distant enemy plane, and pursued it for several minutes. Other famous cases involving Venus are the ] of 1969 (though the skeptical explanation of Venus is hotly contested, including by Carter himself) and the ] chased by two policemen in Devon, England, in 1967.


=== Other misidentifications ===
Although there is no doubt that Venus frequently triggers UFO reports, it is often overplayed as a UFO culprit by many skeptics. For example, astronomer ] claimed that Venus was responsible for a “majority” of all UFO reports (Plait, 205). But the studies cited here do not support this. BBSR attributed only 22% of sightings to astronomical causes of ''all'' types, stars, planets, meteors, etc. Venus constituted some unspecified fraction of these. “Certain” astronomical identifications were only 13% of cases (the other 9% of astronomical IDs being “doubtful”), which would further reduce the number of actual “Venus cases.” Hendry attributed a larger 29% of sightings collectively to “bright stars or planets.” Again the fraction believed caused specifically by Venus isn’t broken down, but likely didn’t exceed 20%.
] to the left of the sun|thumb]]


The BBSR and Hendry studies identified as rare causes for UFO reports based on misidentification, such objects and phenomena as birds, light phenomena (including ]s, ], ], ], ground lights such as street lights, and ] reflected off of clouds), and atmospheric phenomena such as clouds, dust and fog (including unusual cloud formations such as ]s, ]s, rainbow effects, and high-altitude ice crystals). Other identified causes included kites, flares, reflections off windows, and windborne debris.
In other cases, some skeptics will claim Venus (or perhaps Jupiter or a bright star) to be responsible for a sighting when they aren’t even visible or are in the wrong part of the sky. An infamous example occurred in August 1965 when the U.S. Air Force tried to explain away widespread sightings in the midwest as bright stars in or near the constellation Orion. However, Orion, a winter constellation, was still well below the horizon at the time, a fact quickly pointed out by some astronomers, and the Air Force was forced to make a hasty retraction.


==== Upper atmospheric lightning ====
Further, sometimes impossible properties are ascribed to Venus in order to debunk prominent UFO cases. In one famous case, known as the ] from 1966, two policemen in their car chased a brilliant UFO for an extended period of time; in the end, seven police officers were involved in the pursuit, and about half a dozen civilians claimed to have seen the same or a similar object. The deputies said the UFO was as clearly defined and metallic, roughly the size of a house when they first saw it up close, bathed them and the surrounding countryside in bright light, and flew directly over them at one point. Another set of policemen saw the initial two officers' car and the UFO approaching rapidly from the west while Venus was in the east. Despite this, the USAF claimed, after a cursory investigation (only one of the police officers and none of the civilians was interviewed) the police chased a satellite and then Venus. (This incident was the inspiration for the police UFO chase in ]’s '']''). However, in another famous police chase, the "flying cross" UFO of Devon, England, in October 1967, the culprit was definitely identified as Venus.<ref>{{cite web| url = http://www.ianridpath.com/ufo/flyingcross.htm| title = Devon flying cross UFO of 1967| accessdate = 2007-10-02| author = Ian Ridpath| format = HTML}}</ref>
]


More recently, Professor Colin Price head of the Geophysics and Planetary Sciences Department at ] has commented that occurrences of ] such as ], ] and ] could account for some of the strange reports of UFO sightings.<ref name="physorg">{{cite web|url=http://www.physorg.com/news154615655.html|title=A sprightly explanation for UFO sightings?|date=February 23, 2009|publisher=Physorg.com|access-date=2009-02-26}}</ref>
==Conclusions==
In many cases, whether an unknown flying object is ultimately called an IFO or UFO is a judgment call of the researchers involved, and thus the proportion of IFOs to UFOs will inevitably fluctuate and their significance will remain controversial. As Hendry himself noted, “Reasonable UFO proponents admit that ‘genuine’ UFO sightings are in the minority, around 10-20%; the skeptics say, ‘If 90% of the reports are IFOs, why not 100%? Actually, there is no way to determine the absolute percentage of IFO and UFO—they keep changing from sample to sample and year to year and are dependent on the biases of the judges.” <!-- citation? -->


==== Electronic Warfare ====
Many ] argue that IFOs represent the inevitable “noise” encountered in analysis of any phenomenon. Those cases that still defy conventional explanation, even if their percentages are relatively small, constitute the hardcore “signal” of the UFO enigma.


According to ] writer for the ''Digital Intelligencer'', Jeff Wise, advanced ] techniques similar to early "]" used by the US military could deceive sensors to give false velocity and position information. Wise worries that US adversaries have developed EW capabilities that exploit weaknesses in US systems that allow information to be missed or created erroneously. Wise speculates that admitting the US has "gaps in its electronic warfare capabilities" would allow it to be looked at objectively. As Navy Spokesman Joseph Gradisher puts it, "The more data you have, the better you are to analyze it and turn that data into information into knowledge."<ref name="Wise">{{cite web |last1=Wise |first1=Jeff |title=How to Decipher the Pentagon's UFO Report The crucial, missing context for what military pilots might actually be seeing. |url=https://nymag.com/intelligencer/article/ufo-report-decipher.html |website=nymag.com |date=3 July 2021 |publisher=The Intelligencer, ] |access-date=30 August 2021}}</ref>
==References==

{{reflist|1}}
According to ] contributor David Hambling, the U.S. Navy has filed patents for technology to produce a "self-focusing laser pulse to create a glowing filament or channel of plasma" that could create "mid-air images to fool infrared and other sensors". Hambling speculates that such technology "may also provide a clue about the source of some recent UFO sightings by military aircraft".<ref name="Hambling">{{cite web |last1=Hambling |first1=David |title=U.S. Navy Laser Creates Plasma 'UFOs' |url=https://www.forbes.com/sites/davidhambling/2020/05/11/us-navy-laser-creates-plasma-ufos/?sh=427f04fb1074 |website=] |access-date=30 August 2021}}</ref>

==== Psychological ====
{{Main|psychosocial UFO hypothesis}}

Greg Eghigian writing for the ] suggests that, as technology has evolved, so has the nature of the UFO phenomenon. According to Eghigian, "fears of Zeppelins, rockets and drones have replaced the "celestial wonders" of ancient times", and "affairs here on earth have consistently colored our perceptions of what is going on over our heads".<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.smithsonianmag.com/history/how-ufo-reports-change-with-technology-times-180968011/|title=How UFO Reports Change With the Technology of the Times|website=Smithsonian Magazine}}</ref>

According to sociologist David L. Miller, experts such as ] and Orrin Klapp attribute many UFO sightings to social contagion or ], and as an overall cause of the UFO phenomenon.<ref name="Miller">{{cite book |last1=Miller |first1=David L. |title=Introduction to Collective Behavior and Collective Action: Third Edition |date=August 6, 2013 |publisher=Waveland Press |isbn=9781478610953 |page=174 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=YS9NAAAAQBAJ&dq=ufos+mass+hysteria&pg=PA174 |access-date=26 September 2021}}</ref>

==See also==
{{div col|colwidth=30em}}
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
{{div col end}}

== References ==
{{Reflist}}
{{refbegin}} {{refbegin}}
* ], ''The UFO Handbook: A Guide to Investigating, Evaluating, and Reporting UFO Sightings'', 1979, Doubleday & Co., ISBN 0-385-14348-6 * ], ''The UFO Handbook: A Guide to Investigating, Evaluating, and Reporting UFO Sightings'', 1979, Doubleday & Co., {{ISBN|0-385-14348-6}}
* Philip Plait, ''Bad Astronomy: Misconceptions and Misuses Revealed, from Astrology to the Moon Landing "Hoax"'', 2002 John Wiley & Sons, ISBN 0-471-40976-6. (Chapter 20: Misidentified Flying Objects: UFOs and Illusions of the Mind and Eye) * ], '']'', 2002 John Wiley & Sons, {{ISBN|0-471-40976-6}}. (Chapter 20: Misidentified Flying Objects: UFOs and Illusions of the Mind and Eye)
* ] & Thornton Page, editors, ''UFO's: A Scientific Debate'', 1972, Cornell University Press, 1996, Barnes & Noble Books, ISBN 0801407400 * ] & Thornton Page, editors, ''UFO's: A Scientific Debate'', 1972, Cornell University Press, 1996, Barnes & Noble Books, {{ISBN|0-8014-0740-0}}
{{refend}} {{refend}}


==External links== == External links ==
* (]) * (])
*
* *
* (]) * (Robert Moore)
* (]) * (])
* (Steven Levy)


{{UFOs}} {{UFOs}}


] ]
] ]

]
]

Latest revision as of 10:12, 3 November 2024

This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.
Find sources: "Identification studies of UFOs" – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (April 2020) (Learn how and when to remove this message)
UFOs and ufology
Flying saucer from the National Archives and Records Administration
Notable sightings and hoaxes
Conspiracy theories
Religions
Lists of organizations, sightings, studies, etc.

Identifying unidentified flying objects (UFOs) is a difficult task due to the normally poor quality of the evidence provided by those who report sighting the unknown object. Observations and subsequent reporting are often made by those untrained in astronomy, atmospheric phenomena, aeronautics, physics, and perception. Nevertheless, most officially investigated UFO sightings, such as from the U.S. Air Force's Project Blue Book, have been identified as being due to honest misidentifications of natural phenomena, aircraft, or other prosaic explanations. In early U.S. Air Force attempts to explain UFO sightings, unexplained sightings routinely numbered over one in five reports. However, in early 1953, right after the CIA's Robertson Panel, percentages of unexplained sightings dropped precipitously, usually being only a few percent in any given year. When Project Blue Book closed down in 1970, only 6% of all cases were classified as being truly unidentified.

UFOs that can be explained are sometimes termed identified flying objects (IFOs).

UFO studies

The following are some major studies undertaken during the past 70+ years that reported on identification of UFOs:

  • Project Sign
  • Project Blue Book Special Report No. 14 (referred to further below as BBSR) was a massive statistical study the Battelle Memorial Institute did for the USAF of 3,200 UFO cases between 1952 and 1954. Of these, 22% were classified as unidentified ("true UFOs"). Another 69% were deemed identified (IFOs). There was insufficient information to make a determination in the remaining 9%.
  • The official French government UFO investigation (GEPAN/SEPRA), run within the French space agency CNES between 1977 and 2004, scientifically investigated about 6000 cases and found that 13% defied any rational explanation (UFOs), while 46% were deemed readily identifiable and 41%, lacked sufficient information for classification.
  • The USAF-sponsored Condon Committee study reported that all 117 cases studied were or could probably be explained. A 1971 review of the results by the American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics concluded that 30% of the 117 cases remained unexplained.
  • Of about 5,000 cases submitted to and studied by the civilian UFO organization NICAP, 16% were judged unknowns.
  • The Pentagon UFO Report, (or colloquially known as the Pentagon UFO Report) is a United States federally mandated assessment summarizing information regarding unidentified flying objects (UFOs), also known as unidentified aerial phenomenon (UAPs). On June 25, 2021, a nine-page preliminary assessment was issued. It states that the UAPTF focused on 144 observations of "unidentified aerial phenomena" by the U.S Armed Forces, mostly from U.S. Navy personnel, from 2004 to 2021. No details are given in the preliminary assessment. The report found that the UAPTF was unable to identify 143 of these objects, but it included five categories of potential explanations; "Airborne Clutter" includes objects like birds and balloons, "Natural Atmospheric Phenomena" includes atmospheric effect like ice crystals, "USG or Industry Developmental Programs" includes US military technology, "Foreign Adversary Systems" includes technologies developed by foreign governments such as Russia or China, and "Other", described as a "catchall" for other explanations.
  • In contrast, much more conservative numbers for the percentage of UFOs were arrived at individually by astronomer Allan Hendry, who was the chief investigator for the Center for UFO Studies (CUFOS). CUFOS was founded by astronomer J. Allen Hynek (who had been a consultant for the Air Force's Project Blue Book) to provide a serious scientific investigation into UFOs. Hendry spent 15 months personally investigating 1,307 UFO reports. In 1979, Hendry published his conclusions in The UFO Handbook: A Guide to Investigating, Evaluating, and Reporting UFO Sightings. Hendry admitted that he would like to find evidence for extraterrestrials but noted that the vast majority of cases had prosaic explanations. He found 89% of reports definitely or probably identifiable and only 9% unidentified. "Hardcore" cases—well-documented events which defied any conceivable conventional explanation—made up only 1.5% of the reports.

Project Sign

Main article: Project Sign

Project Sign lists that "in order to investigate the credibility of their existence the following factors must be considered in any technical analysis":

Method of support (lift)

  1. Wings
  2. Fuselage lift (Wingless)
  3. Rotor
  4. Vertical Jet
  5. Magnus effect
  6. Aerostatic (lighter-than-air craft)

Method of propulsion (thrust)

  1. Propeller reciprocating engine combination jet
  2. Rocket
  3. Ramjet
  4. Aerodynamic (for example, the Katzmayer effect, defined as "the reduction in drag of an airfoil when an air stream is oscillating").

Project Blue Book Special Report No. 14

Main article: Project Blue Book Special Report No. 14

Project Blue Book Special Report No. 14 was compiled between 1951 and 1954, and included 3201 reported UFO sightings. Battelle employed four scientific analysts, who sought to divide cases into "knowns", "unknowns", and a third category of "insufficient information." They also broke down knowns and unknowns into four categories of quality, from excellent to poor. In order for a case to be deemed "identified", two analysts had to independently agree on a solution and for a case to be called "unidentified", all four analysts had to agree. A report classified as "unidentified" was defined as: "Those reports of sightings wherein the description of the object and its maneuvers could not be fitted to the pattern of any known object or phenomenon."

Out of 3,201 cases, 69% were judged to be identified, 22% were unidentified, and 9% had insufficient information to make a determination.

Breakdown by category of IFO and case quality

Category/Case Quality All Excellent Good Doubtful Poor
Astronomical 22% 24% 23% 19% 23%
Aircraft 22% 19% 22% 25% 16%
Balloon 15% 12% 17% 17% 13%
Light phenomena 2.2% .9% 2.4% 2.9% 1.1%
Birds 1.0% 0.9% 1.0% 1.2% 0.7%
Clouds, dust, etc. 0.4% 0% 1.0% 0.4% 0%
Psychological 2.0% 0% 0.5% 3.3% 3.3%
Other 5% 5% 5% 5% 6%
Insufficient information 9% 4% 4% 14% 21%
Unknown origin 22% 33% 25% 13% 17%

BBSR further broke these results down based on whether the identification was considered certain or merely doubtful. For example, in both the astronomical and aircraft IFO categories, 12% were considered certain and 9% were doubtful. Overall, of the 69% listed as IFOs, 42% were thought to be solved with certainty, while 27% were still considered doubtful.

In addition, if a case was lacking in adequate data, it was placed in the insufficient information category, separate from both IFOs and UFOs.

Military vs. civilian breakdown

IFO UFO Insufficient Information
Mil Civ All Mil Civ All Mil Civ All
Best reports 65% 72% 68% 32% 24% 28% 2% 4% 3%
Worst reports 70% 70% 70% 24% 14% 16% 7% 17% 14%

The Battelle BBSR study included many internal military reports; 38% of the cases were designated as military. Military witnesses tended to submit better quality reports, had much fewer reports rated as having insufficient information, and had higher percentages of unknowns. As in the previous breakdown, the percentage of UFOs again rose with case quality for both the military and civilian subcategories.

In the summary table, best reports are those rated excellent and good; worst reports are doubtful and poor.

Comparison of IFOs to UFOs by characteristics

A key study of BBSR was to statistically compare IFOs and UFOs by six characteristics: color, number of objects, shape, duration of observations, speed, and light brightness. If there were no significant differences, the two classes were probably the same, the UFOs then representing merely a failure to properly identify prosaic phenomena that could already account for the IFOs. On the other hand, if the differences were statistically significant, this would suggest IFOs and UFOs were indeed distinctly different phenomena.

In the initial results, all characteristics except brightness tested significant at less or much less than 1% (brightness was greater than 5%). By removing "astronomical" sightings from the "knowns" and redoing the test, just two categories, number and speed, were significant at less than 1%, the remainder having results between 3% and 5%. This indicated that there was a statistically significant difference between the characteristics ascribed to UFOs and IFOs, but perhaps not as significant as the initial results suggested. For two characteristics, brightness and speed, the significance actually increased with the revised test.

Allan Hendry study

Like the Air Force, astronomer Allan Hendry found that only a small percentage of cases were hoaxes and that most sightings were actually honest misidentifications of prosaic phenomena. Hendry attributed most of these to inexperience or misperception.

Out of 1,307 cases Hendry deemed 88.6% had clear prosaic explanations (IFOs) and only 8.6% were unknowns (UFOs). Of the UFOs, Hendry reported that 7.1%, might still have a prosaic explanation while 1.5% (20 cases) had no possible plausible explanation and were completely unexplained. The remaining miscellaneous cases (2.8%) were "garbage" cases, where Hendry deemed the witnesses unreliable, the reports hopelessly contradictory, or lacking in sufficient information.

Overall, in the three major categories, 42% of all cases had astronomical explanations, 37% were aircraft, and 5% were balloons. A further breakdown allowed 77% to be readily explained by five main classes of objects: 29% were bright stars or planets, 19% were advertising planes, 15% were other aircraft, 9% were meteors and reentering space debris, and 5% were balloons of various types (mostly weather or advertising balloons but also a few prank balloons).

Breakdown of cases

Hendry also used a case classification system developed by his mentor J. Allen Hynek, who established CUFOS, where the study was carried out. In this summary table:

  • NL = "Nocturnal Lights", lights seen in the sky at night.
  • DD = "Daylight Discs", objects seen in daytime (but not necessarily disc in shape).
  • RV = "Radar/Visual" cases, objects observed by both witnesses and radar.
  • CE = "Close Encounter" cases. For convenience, CE cases listed below are combined totals of Hynek's CE1, CE2, and CE3 cases, where:
    • CE1 cases where objects were thought to be seen up close (within 500 feet).
    • CE2 had purported physical interactions with the environment (physical trace cases or electromagnetic interference).
    • CE3 cases were supposed to involve sightings of occupants.
Category NL DD CE RV Total Cases Percent
Astronomical
bright stars or planets 360 2 16 2 380 29%
meteors, re-entering man-made spacecraft 113 5 4 0 122 9%
artificial satellites 24 0 0 0 24 2%
moon 22 0 0 0 22 2%
TOTAL (all cases) 519 7 22 2 550 42%
Aircraft
advertising planes 230 0 22 0 252 19%
other aircraft 196 22 6 0 224 17%
missile launches 9 0 1 0 10 0.7%
TOTAL 435 22 29 0 486 37%
balloons 23 35 2 3 63 5%
birds 5 1 0 0 6 0.5%
clouds, dust 10 2 0 0 12 0.9%
light phenomena (mirage, moon dog, ground lights, searchlights, etc.) 9 1 4 0 14 1.1%
other (kites, flares, reflections, windblown debris, etc.) 12 3 1 0 16 1.2%
Total Identified
Cases 1024 71 58 5 1158 88.6%
Percent 78.3% 5.4% 4.4% .4% 88.6%
Total Unidentified
Cases 79 18 16 0 113 8.6%
Percent 6% 1.4% 1.2% 0% 8.6%
MISC (insufficient information, inconsistent accounts, unreliable witnesses) 36 2.8%
Total all cases
Cases 1103 89 74 5 1307 100%
Percent 84.4% 6.8% 5.7% 0.4% 100%

Common causes of misidentification and UFOs

Unmanned airship

Both BBSR and Hendry found that three classes of objects or phenomena—astronomical, aircraft, or balloons—accounted for a large majority of identifiable UFO reports (referred to as IFOs), 86% and 83% in the two studies. For example, in Hendry's study, bright stars and planets made up 29% of all cases while meteors (and to a much lesser extent, re-entering space debris) made up 9%. Hovering aircraft such as helicopters or blimps, or aircraft that appear to be hovering, such as airplanes seen at night from the front with their headlights on as they approach for landing can often confuse witnesses, as can aircraft strobe lights. BBSR reported a much higher percentage of balloons than Hendry.

Green flash at sunset

Claims of misidentification are after-the-fact analyses, not direct observations, and are often misconstrued by skeptics and UFO advocates alike: They do not suggest that the experiences did not exist, but merely that they can be explained by prosaic causes. For instance, retrospective analyses of the Jimmy Carter UFO incident of 1969 connect the sighting with the known position of the planet Venus for that time, date, and location. Gordon Cooper, a strong advocate of the extraterrestrial hypothesis (ETH), claimed to have been fooled by the planet Venus when he was a fighter pilot, thinking it a distant enemy plane, and the 1967 "flying cross" of Devon, England and the 1966 Portage County UFO Chase case have both been associated with astronomical sources.

In 2009, Peter Davenport, Director of the National UFO Reporting Center, posted this complaint online:

We are receiving hundreds of reports every month of normal, terrestrial events, e.g. over-flights of the International Space Station, the Space Shuttle, or satellites; "flares" of light from "Iridium" satellites; the appearance of typical meteors; and observations of normal, "twinkling" stars, planets, contrails, clusters of balloons, etc.  In fact, the overwhelming majority of reports that we receive now are of these normal objects and events, and processing the reports is taking a huge amount of our time... I believe the majority of time I spend on the Hotline is devoted to trying to convince people who have been staring for hours at a star or planet that the object of interest is not a UFO.

There are several natural and man-made objects that are commonly suggested as explanations for UFO sightings:

Venus

Crescent phase of Venus

With the exception of the sun, moon and the International Space Station, Venus is the brightest object in the sky and is often visible in the early morning and evening sky. Even experienced witnesses, especially when they are in unfamiliar surroundings or unusual atmospheric conditions, can fail to identify Venus correctly; however, the location of Venus is easily calculable, and professional astronomers have said that many of the UFO reports received from concerned citizens are due to observations of Venus. Astronomer Phil Plait, in particular, has suggested that Venus is responsible for the majority of all UFO reports

Meteors

Bolide

The brightest meteors known as bolides are long lasting fireballs that leave a trail in the sky which can be visible for up to an hour after passing. Such events are relatively rare but can be witnessed by a large area of the Earth since most events occur kilometers up in the atmosphere. Those witnessing such events who are not familiar with meteors can be easily fooled into thinking that the meteor is a UFO. Because meteors are not predictable with the same degree of accuracy as planets, stars, or man-made objects such as satellites, these occurrences are more difficult to prove in retrospect, though UFO sightings during meteor showers, or where there are astronomical reports of bolides, are likely to be explained as such.

Balloons, aircraft, satellites and other man-made objects

High-altitude balloon

Many reports capture conventional, man-made objects. A Skyhook balloon has been postulated as an explanation for the Mantell UFO Incident, which led to the death of Captain Thomas Mantell.

Project Loon was a Google secret effort to bring internet services to isolated areas. Test balloons of different shapes were used and reported as UFOs. The Google Project Manager stated in an article in Wired Magazine that the team used UFO reports to track the balloons progress for one of the launches.

Aircraft shapes have radically changed as we continue to develop stealth technologies. These new test designs appear unusual and cause reports. The CIA released a report indicating that many UFO reports in the 1950s were classified aircraft like the SR-71 and U-2.

The increase in civilian drone usage and popularity throughout the 2010s may be moving identification of objects away from UFOs and towards drones.

Lenticular clouds

Lenticular clouds have been reported as UFOs due to their peculiar shape.

These stationary cloud formations often appear above mountains, but can happen when winds and "eddies" help shape clouds into lens shaped clouds and people see these as "flying saucers".

Misperception

Classical planets in the night sky

Light distortion from air turbulence can cause celestial bodies to move to a limited degree as can a visual perceptual effect called the autokinetic effect, caused by small, involuntary eye movements after staring at a star-like light against a black background without a frame of reference. To some observers, these may cause stars and planets to appear to start and stop, change direction, or dart around. Hendry and other UFO skeptics attribute complex patterns of apparent motion in UFO reports to the autokinetic effect. Its opposite, autostasis is one of the causes as well.

Another type of misperceived motion sometimes occurs when people are driving in a vehicle. Witnesses may believe the "UFO" was following them even though the celestial body was actually stationary. Even police and other normally reliable witnesses can occasionally be fooled by sightings of bright stars and planets.

In about 10% of Hendry's cases caused by celestial bodies, witnesses greatly underestimated distances to the objects, giving distance estimates ranging from 200 feet to 125 miles (60 m to 200 km).

According to Hendry, moving clouds may also sometimes confuse observers by creating induced motion. Hendry believes this occasionally makes observers also believe objects have suddenly disappeared or make a rapid departure.

Fata Morgana

Main article: Fata Morgana (mirage)
A Fata Morgana of a boat below the horizon produces the illusion of a solid form floating in the sky.

Fata Morgana is a type of mirage responsible for some UFO sightings, by making objects located below the astronomical horizon appear to be hovering in the sky. It also magnifies images and makes them look unrecognizable.

The UFOs seen on radar can also be due to Fata Morgana, since water vapor in the air can create radar mirages more readily than temperature inversions can create optical mirages. According to GEPAN/SEPRA, the official UFO investigation in France,

As is well known, atmospheric ducting is the explanation for certain optical mirages, and in particular the arctic illusion called "fata morgana" where distant ocean or surface ice, which is essentially flat, appears to the viewer in the form of vertical columns and spires, or "castles in the air."
People often assume that mirages occur only rarely. This may be true of optical mirages, but conditions for radar mirages are more common, due to the role played by water vapor which strongly affects the atmospheric refractivity in relation to radio waves. Since clouds are closely associated with high levels of water vapor, optical mirages due to water vapor are often rendered undetectable by the accompanying opaque cloud. On the other hand, radar propagation is essentially unaffected by the water droplets of the cloud so that changes in water vapor content with altitude are very effective in producing atmospheric ducting and radar mirages.

Fata Morgana was named as a hypothesis for the mysterious Australian phenomenon Min Min light.

Other misidentifications

A sun dog to the left of the sun

The BBSR and Hendry studies identified as rare causes for UFO reports based on misidentification, such objects and phenomena as birds, light phenomena (including mirages, moondogs, sundogs, auroras, ground lights such as street lights, and searchlights reflected off of clouds), and atmospheric phenomena such as clouds, dust and fog (including unusual cloud formations such as lenticular clouds, noctilucent clouds, rainbow effects, and high-altitude ice crystals). Other identified causes included kites, flares, reflections off windows, and windborne debris.

Upper atmospheric lightning

A sprite.

More recently, Professor Colin Price head of the Geophysics and Planetary Sciences Department at Tel Aviv University has commented that occurrences of upper-atmospheric lightning such as sprites, elves and blue jets could account for some of the strange reports of UFO sightings.

Electronic Warfare

According to New York Magazine writer for the Digital Intelligencer, Jeff Wise, advanced Electronic Warfare techniques similar to early "radar spoofing" used by the US military could deceive sensors to give false velocity and position information. Wise worries that US adversaries have developed EW capabilities that exploit weaknesses in US systems that allow information to be missed or created erroneously. Wise speculates that admitting the US has "gaps in its electronic warfare capabilities" would allow it to be looked at objectively. As Navy Spokesman Joseph Gradisher puts it, "The more data you have, the better you are to analyze it and turn that data into information into knowledge."

According to Forbes contributor David Hambling, the U.S. Navy has filed patents for technology to produce a "self-focusing laser pulse to create a glowing filament or channel of plasma" that could create "mid-air images to fool infrared and other sensors". Hambling speculates that such technology "may also provide a clue about the source of some recent UFO sightings by military aircraft".

Psychological

Main article: psychosocial UFO hypothesis

Greg Eghigian writing for the Smithsonian Magazine suggests that, as technology has evolved, so has the nature of the UFO phenomenon. According to Eghigian, "fears of Zeppelins, rockets and drones have replaced the "celestial wonders" of ancient times", and "affairs here on earth have consistently colored our perceptions of what is going on over our heads".

According to sociologist David L. Miller, experts such as Neil Smelser and Orrin Klapp attribute many UFO sightings to social contagion or mass hysteria, and as an overall cause of the UFO phenomenon.

See also

References

  1. Pasachoff, Jay M and Alex Filippenko (2004). The Cosmos: Astronomy in the New Millennium. Brooks/Cole div. of Thomson Learning. p. 428. ISBN 0-534-39550-3. bservations are usually anecdotal, are not controlled in a scientific experiment, and are not accessible to study by sophisticated instruments.
  2. Nickell, Joe; McGaha, James (2018). "UFO Identification Process". Skeptical Inquirer. 42 (6): 34–37. Archived from the original on 2018-12-23. Retrieved 23 December 2018.
  3. "Preliminary Assessment: Unidentified Aerial Phenomena" (PDF). Office of the Director of National Intelligence. June 25, 2021. Retrieved June 25, 2021.
  4. Hendry, Allan. (1979). The UFO handbook : a guide to investigating, evaluating, and reporting UFO sightings (1st ed.). Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday. ISBN 0-385-14348-6. OCLC 4642190.
  5. Project SIGN</rorce)
  6. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc53897/m2/1/high_res_d/19930081006.pdf
  7. It is worth noting that Carter himself never claimed that the sighting was anything more than an odd aerial or electrical phenomenon
  8. Ian Ridpath. "Devon flying cross UFO of 1967". Retrieved 2007-10-02.
  9. "The Portage County, Ohio, UFO Chase – UFO Casebook Files". ufocasebook.com.
  10. Davenport, Peter. National UFO Reporting Center Statement August 30, 2009 Archived April 8, 2013, at the Wayback Machine
  11. Paschoff and Filipenko
  12. (Plait, 205)
  13. Greenwood, Faine (16 January 2019). "Drones Are the New Flying Saucers". Slate. Retrieved 20 February 2020.
  14. Ridpath, Ian. How stars become UFOs
  15. Wertheimer, Michael (1968). "A Case of "Autostasis" or Reverse Autokinesis". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 26 (2): 417–418. doi:10.2466/pms.1968.26.2.417. PMID 5654860. S2CID 39932855.
  16. Electromagnetic-Wave Ducting Archived 2008-10-22 at the Wayback Machine BY V. R. ESHLEMAN
  17. Pettigrew, JD. (2003). "The Min Min light and the Fata Morgana. An optical account of a mysterious Australian phenomenon". Clinical and Experimental Optometry. 86 (2): 109–20. doi:10.1111/j.1444-0938.2003.tb03069.x. PMID 12643807.
  18. "A sprightly explanation for UFO sightings?". Physorg.com. February 23, 2009. Retrieved 2009-02-26.
  19. Wise, Jeff (3 July 2021). "How to Decipher the Pentagon's UFO Report The crucial, missing context for what military pilots might actually be seeing". nymag.com. The Intelligencer, New York Magazine. Retrieved 30 August 2021.
  20. Hambling, David. "U.S. Navy Laser Creates Plasma 'UFOs'". Forbes. Retrieved 30 August 2021.
  21. "How UFO Reports Change With the Technology of the Times". Smithsonian Magazine.
  22. Miller, David L. (August 6, 2013). Introduction to Collective Behavior and Collective Action: Third Edition. Waveland Press. p. 174. ISBN 9781478610953. Retrieved 26 September 2021.

External links

UFOs
Claimed sightings
General
Pre-20th century
20th century
21st century
Confirmed hoaxes
Sightings by country
Types of UFOs
Types of alleged
extraterrestrial beings
Studies and timeline
Hypotheses
Conspiracy theories
Involvement
Abduction claims
Other
Culture
Skepticism
Government & Law
Categories: