Misplaced Pages

Mismatch negativity: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 15:32, 9 January 2009 editRjanag (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users58,857 edits rv. message left at user talk page. please discuss at Talk before edit warring← Previous edit Latest revision as of 10:21, 30 November 2024 edit undoJohnnyBflat (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users7,084 edits Adding short description: "Component in a sequence of stimuli"Tag: Shortdesc helper 
(183 intermediate revisions by 59 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Component in a sequence of stimuli}}
:''"Mismatch field" and "MMNM" redirect here''.
The '''mismatch negativity''' ('''MMN''') or '''mismatch field''' ('''MMF''') is a component of the ] (ERP) to an odd stimulus in a sequence of stimuli. It arises from electrical activity in the ] and is studied within the field of ] and ]. It can occur in any ], but has most frequently been studied for ] and for ], in which case it is abbreviated to '''vMMN'''.<ref name="Stefanicset2014">{{cite journal |last1=Stefanics |first1=G |last2=Kremláček |first2=J |last3=Czigler |first3=I |title=Visual mismatch negativity: A predictive coding view |journal=Frontiers in Human Neuroscience |date=2014 |volume=8 |issue=666 |page=666 |doi=10.3389/fnhum.2014.00666 |pmid=25278859 |pmc=4165279 |doi-access=free }}</ref> The (v)MMN occurs after an infrequent change in a repetitive sequence of stimuli (sometimes the entire sequence is called an ''oddball sequence''.) For example, a rare deviant (d) stimulus can be interspersed among a series of frequent standard (s) stimuli (e.g., ''s s s s s s s s s d s s s s s s d s s s d s s s s''...). In hearing, a deviant sound can differ from the standards in one or more perceptual features such as ], duration, ], or location.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Tiitinen H, Jiang D, Alho K | title = Attention and mismatch negativity | journal = Psychophysiology | volume = 30 | issue = 5 | pages = 436–50 | date = September 1993 | pmid = 8416070 | doi = 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02067.x }}</ref> The MMN can be elicited regardless of whether someone is paying attention to the sequence.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Brattico E, Tervaniemi M, Näätänen R, Peretz I | title = Musical scale properties are automatically processed in the human auditory cortex | journal = Brain Research | volume = 1117 | issue = 1 | pages = 162–74 | date = October 2006 | pmid = 16963000 | doi = 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.023 | s2cid = 8401429 | url = http://www.brainmusic.org/EducationalActivitiesFolder/Brattico_pitch2006.pdf }}</ref> During auditory sequences, a person can be reading or watching a silent subtitled movie, yet still show a clear MMN. In the case of visual stimuli, the MMN occurs after an infrequent change in a repetitive sequence of images.

The '''mismatch negativity''' (MMN) or '''mismatch field''' (MMF) is a component of the ] (ERP) to an odd stimulus in a sequence of stimuli. It arises from electrical activity in the ] and is studied within the field of ] and ]. It can occur in any sensory system, but has most frequently been studied for ] and for ]. In the case of auditory stimuli, the MMN occurs after an infrequent change in a repetitive sequence of sounds (sometimes the entire sequence is called an ''oddball sequence''.) For example, a rare deviant (d) sound can be interspersed among a series of frequent standard (s) sounds (e.g., ''s s s s s s s s s d s s s s s s d s s s d s s s s''...). The deviant sound can differ from the standards in one or more perceptual features such as ], duration, or ]. The MMN can be elicited regardless of whether the subject is paying attention to the sequence.<ref name="pulvermullershtyrov2003">{{cite journal | last=Pulvermüller | first=Friedemann | author2-last=Shtyrov | author2-first=Yury | year=2003 | journal=NeuroImage | volume=20 | title=Automatic processing of grammar in the human brain as revealed by the mismatch negativity | page=159}}</ref><ref name="pulvermulleretal2008">{{cite journal | title=Syntax as a reflex: Neurophysiological evidence for the early automaticity of syntactic processing | journal=Brain and Language | year=2008 | volume=104 | last=Pulvermüller | first=Friedemann | author2-last=Shtyrov | author2-first=Yury | author3-last=Hasting | author3-first=Anna S. | author4-last=Carlyton | author4-first=Robert P. | pages=244&ndash;253}}</ref> During auditory sequences, a person can be reading or watching a silent subtitled movie, yet still show a clear MMN. In the case of visual stimuli, the MMN occurs after an infrequent change in a repetitive sequence of images.


MMN refers to the mismatch response in ] (EEG); MMF or MMNM refer to the mismatch response in ] (MEG). MMN refers to the mismatch response in ] (EEG); MMF or MMNM refer to the mismatch response in ] (MEG).
Line 7: Line 6:
==History== ==History==


The auditory MMN was discovered in 1978 by ], ], and ] at the ], ], ].<ref>Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A.W.K., & Mäntysalo, S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica, 42, 313-329.</ref> The auditory MMN was discovered in 1978 by ], A. W. K. Gaillard, and S. Mäntysalo at the Institute for Perception, ] in ].<ref name="Näätänen, 1978"/>


The first report of a visual MMN was in 1990 by Rainer Cammer.<ref>Cammann, R. (1990). Is there no MMN in the visual modality? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 234-234.</ref> For a history of the development of the visual MMN, see Pazo-Alvarez et al. (2003).<ref>Pazo-Alvarez, P., Cadaveira, F., & Amenedo, E. (2003). MMN in the visual modality: A review. Biological Psychology, 63, 199-236.</ref> The first report of a visual MMN was in 1990 by Rainer Cammer.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Cammann R | year = 1990 | title = Is there no MMN in the visual modality? | journal = Behavioral and Brain Sciences | volume = 13 | issue = 2| pages = 234–235 | doi=10.1017/s0140525x00078420| s2cid = 143448593 }}</ref> For a history of the development of the visual MMN, see Pazo-Alvarez et al. (2003).<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F, Amenedo E | title = MMN in the visual modality: a review | journal = Biological Psychology | volume = 63 | issue = 3 | pages = 199–236 | date = July 2003 | pmid = 12853168 | doi = 10.1016/s0301-0511(03)00049-8 | s2cid = 14404599 }}</ref>


==Characteristics== ==Characteristics==
The MMN is a response to a deviant within a sequence of otherwise regular stimuli; thus, in an experimental setting, it is produced when stimuli are presented in a many-to-one ratio; for example, in a sequence of sounds ''s s s s s s s d s s s s d s s s...'', the ''d'' is the deviant or oddball stimulus, and will elicit an MMN response. The mismatch negativity occurs even if the subject is not consciously paying attention to the stimuli.<ref name="Näätänen, 1978">{{cite journal | vauthors = Näätänen R, Gaillard AW, Mäntysalo S | title = Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted | journal = Acta Psychologica | volume = 42 | issue = 4 | pages = 313–29 | date = July 1978 | pmid = 685709 | doi = 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9 }}</ref> Processing of sensory stimulus features is essential for humans in determining their responses and actions. If behaviourally relevant aspects of the environment are not correctly represented in the brain, then the organism's behaviour cannot be appropriate. Without these representations our ability to understand spoken language, for example, would be seriously impaired. Cognitive neuroscience has consequently emphasised the importance of understanding brain mechanisms of sensory information processing, that is, the sensory prerequisites of cognition. Most of the data obtained, unfortunately, do not allow the objective measurement of the accuracy of these stimulus representations.<ref name="Näätänen, 1992">{{cite book | last = Näätänen | first = Risto | name-list-style = vanc |title=Attention and brain function |publisher=L. Erlbaum |location=Hillsdale, N.J |year=1992 |isbn=978-0-8058-0984-8 |oclc=25832590 |url=https://www.questia.com/PM.qst?a=o&d=14362952 }}</ref> In addition, recent cognitive neuroscience seems to have succeeded in extracting such a measure, however. This is the mismatch negativity (MMN), a component of the event-related potential (ERP), first reported by Näätänen, Gaillard, and Mäntysalo (1978).<ref name="Näätänen, 1978"/> An in-depth review of MMN research can be found in Näätänen (1992)<ref name="Näätänen, 1992"/> while other recent reviews also provide information on the generator mechanisms of MMN,<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Alho K | title = Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes | journal = Ear and Hearing | volume = 16 | issue = 1 | pages = 38–51 | date = February 1995 | pmid = 7774768 | doi = 10.1097/00003446-199502000-00004 | s2cid = 31698419 }}</ref> its magnetic counterpart, MMNm (Näätänen, Ilmoniemi & Alho, 1994),<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Näätänen R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Alho K | title = Magnetoencephalography in studies of human cognitive brain function | journal = Trends in Neurosciences | volume = 17 | issue = 9 | pages = 389–95 | date = September 1994 | pmid = 7529443 | doi = 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90048-5 | s2cid = 10224526 }}</ref> and its clinical applicability.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Näätänen R, Alho K | title = Mismatch negativity--a unique measure of sensory processing in audition | journal = The International Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 80 | issue = 1–4 | pages = 317–37 | year = 1995 | pmid = 7775056 | doi = 10.3109/00207459508986107 }}</ref>
The MMN is a response to a deviant within a sequence of otherwise regular stimuli; thus, in an experimental setting, it is produced when stimuli are presented in a many-to-one ratio; for example, in a sequence of sounds ''s s s s s s s d s s s s d s s s...'', the ''d'' is the deviant or oddball stimulus, and will elicit an MMN response. Therefore, it has been frequently used in studies of ] perception, to test whether or not subjects neurologically distinguish between certain kinds of sounds.<ref>Phillips, Colin ''et al''. (2001). "Auditory Cortex Accesses Phonological Category: An MEG Mismatch Study." ''Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience'' 12:6. 1038-1055.</ref> The mismatch negativity occurs even if the subject is not consciously paying attention to the stimuli.<ref name="pulvermullershtyrov2003"/><ref name="pulvermulleretal2008"/>


The auditory MMN can occur in response to deviance in pitch, intensity, or duration. The auditory MMN is a fronto-central negative potential with sources in the primary and non-primary ] and a typical latency of 150-250 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus. Sources could also include one from the right opercular part of the ]. The amplitude and latency of the MMN is related to how different the deviant stimulus is from the standard. Large deviances elicit MMN at earlier latencies. For very large deviances, the MMN can even overlap the ] (e.g., Campbell et al., 2007).<ref>Campbell, T.A., Winkler, I., & Kujala, T. (2007). N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: Disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1. Psychophysiology, 44, 530-540. http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00529.x</ref> The auditory MMN can occur in response to deviance in pitch, intensity, or duration. The auditory MMN is a fronto-central negative potential with sources in the primary and non-primary ] and a typical latency of 150-250 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus. Sources could also include the ], and the ].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Rosburg T, Trautner P, Dietl T, Korzyukov OA, Boutros NN, Schaller C, Elger CE, Kurthen M | display-authors = 6 | title = Subdural recordings of the mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with focal epilepsy | journal = Brain | volume = 128 | issue = Pt 4 | pages = 819–28 | date = April 2005 | pmid = 15728656 | doi = 10.1093/brain/awh442 | doi-access = }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Phillips HN, Blenkmann A, Hughes LE, Kochen S, Bekinschtein TA, Rowe JB | title = Convergent evidence for hierarchical prediction networks from human electrocorticography and magnetoencephalography | journal = Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior | volume = 82 | pages = 192–205 | date = September 2016 | pmid = 27389803 | pmc = 4981429 | doi = 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.05.001 | first7 = James B. }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Blenkmann AO, Collavini S, Lubell J, Llorens A, Funderud I, Ivanovic J, Larsson PG, Meling TR, Bekinschtein T, Kochen S, Endestad T, Knight RT, Solbakk AK | display-authors = 6 | title = Auditory deviance detection in the human insula: An intracranial EEG study | journal = Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior | volume = 121 | pages = 189–200 | date = December 2019 | pmid = 31629197 | doi = 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.09.002 | s2cid = 202749677 | url = https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/487306v1 | hdl = 10852/75077 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> The amplitude and latency of the MMN is related to how different the deviant stimulus is from the standard. Large deviances elicit MMN at earlier latencies. For very large deviances, the MMN can even overlap the ].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Campbell T, Winkler I, Kujala T | title = N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1 | journal = Psychophysiology | volume = 44 | issue = 4 | pages = 530–40 | date = July 2007 | pmid = 17532805 | doi = 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00529.x | hdl = 10138/136216 | hdl-access = free }}</ref>


The visual MMN can occur in response to deviance in such aspects as color, size, or duration. The visual MMN is an occipital negative potential with sources in the primary ] and a typical latency of 150-250 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus. The visual MMN can occur in response to deviance in such aspects as color, size, or duration. The visual MMN is an occipital negative potential with sources in the primary ] and a typical latency of 150-250 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus.

==Neurolinguistics==

As kindred phenomena have been elicited with speech stimuli, under passive conditions that require very little active attention to the sound, a version of MMN has been frequently used in studies of ] perception, to test whether or not these participants neurologically distinguish between certain kinds of sounds.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Phillips C, Pellathy T, Marantz A, Yellin E, Wexler K, Poeppel D, McGinnis M, Roberts T | display-authors = 6 | title = Auditory cortex accesses phonological categories: an MEG mismatch study | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 12 | issue = 6 | pages = 1038–55 | date = November 2000 | pmid = 11177423 | doi = 10.1162/08989290051137567 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.201.5797 | s2cid = 8686819 }}</ref> The MMN response has been used to study how fetuses and newborns discriminate speech sounds.<ref>{{cite book | first1 = Louis A. | last1 = Schmidt | first2 = Sidney J | last2 = Segalowitz | name-list-style = vanc |title=Developmental psychophysiology: theory, systems, and methods |date=2008 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=978-0-511-49979-1 |location=Cambridge |oclc=190792301}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Draganova R, Eswaran H, Murphy P, Huotilainen M, Lowery C, Preissl H | title = Sound frequency change detection in fetuses and newborns, a magnetoencephalographic study | journal = NeuroImage | volume = 28 | issue = 2 | pages = 354–61 | date = November 2005 | pmid = 16023867 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.011 | s2cid = 19794076 }}</ref> In addition to these kinds of studies focusing on ] processing, some research has implicated the MMN in ] processing.<ref>{{cite book |title=Automaticity and Control in Language Processing |series=Advances in Behavioural Brain Science |year=2007 |chapter=The mismatch negativity as an objective tool for studying higher language functions |vauthors=Pulvermüller F, Shtyrov Y |pages=217–242 |veditors=Meyer AS, Wheeldon LR, Krott A |isbn=978-0-203-96851-2 |doi=10.4324/9780203968512}}<br />Specific experimental studies include the following:
* {{cite journal | vauthors = Hasting AS, Kotz SA, Friederici AD | title = Setting the stage for automatic syntax processing: the mismatch negativity as an indicator of syntactic priming | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 19 | issue = 3 | pages = 386–400 | date = March 2007 | pmid = 17335388 | doi = 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.386 | s2cid = 3046335 }}
* {{cite journal | vauthors = Hasting AS, Winkler I, Kotz SA | title = Early differential processing of verbs and nouns in the human brain as indexed by event-related brain potentials | journal = The European Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 27 | issue = 6 | pages = 1561–5 | date = March 2008 | pmid = 18364028 | doi = 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06103.x | s2cid = 24540547 }}
* {{cite journal | vauthors = Pulvermüller F, Shtyrov Y | title = Automatic processing of grammar in the human brain as revealed by the mismatch negativity | journal = NeuroImage | volume = 20 | issue = 1 | pages = 159–72 | date = September 2003 | pmid = 14527578 | doi = 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00261-1 | s2cid = 27124567 }}
* {{cite journal | vauthors = Pulvermüller F, Shtyrov Y, Hasting AS, Carlyon RP | title = Syntax as a reflex: neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing | journal = Brain and Language | volume = 104 | issue = 3 | pages = 244–53 | date = March 2008 | pmid = 17624417 | doi = 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.05.002 | s2cid = 13870754 }}</ref> Some of these studies have attempted to directly test the automaticity of the MMN, providing converging evidence for the understanding of the MMN as a task-independent and automatic response.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Pulvermüller F, Shtyrov Y, Hasting AS, Carlyon RP | title = Syntax as a reflex: neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing | journal = Brain and Language | volume = 104 | issue = 3 | pages = 244–53 | date = March 2008 | pmid = 17624417 | doi = 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.05.002 | s2cid = 13870754 }}</ref>

==For basic stimulus features==
MMN is evoked by an infrequently presented stimulus ("deviant"), differing from the frequently-occurring stimuli ("standards") in one or several physical parameters like duration, intensity, or frequency.<ref name="Näätänen, 1992"/> In addition, it is generated by a change in spectrally complex stimuli like phonemes, in synthesised instrumental tones, or in the spectral component of tone timbre. Also the temporal order reversals elicit an MMN when successive sound elements differ either in frequency, intensity, or duration. The MMN is not elicited by stimuli with deviant stimulus parameters when they are presented without the intervening standards. Thus, the MMN has been suggested to reflect change detection when a memory trace representing the constant standard stimulus and the neural code of the stimulus with deviant parameter(s) are discrepant.

==Vs. auditory sensory memory==
The MMN data can be understood as providing evidence that stimulus features are separately analysed and stored in the vicinity of auditory cortex (for a discussion, please see the theory section below). The close resemblance of the behaviour of the MMN to that of the previously behaviourally observed "echoic" memory system strongly suggests that the MMN provides a non-invasive, objective, task-independently measurable physiological correlate of stimulus-feature representations in auditory sensory memory.

==Relationship to attentional processes==
The experimental evidence suggests that the auditory sensory memory index MMN provides sensory data for attentional processes, and, in essence, governs certain aspects of attentive information processing. This is evident in the finding that the latency of the MMN determines the timing of behavioural responses to changes in the auditory environment.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Tiitinen H, May P, Reinikainen K, Näätänen R | title = Attentive novelty detection in humans is governed by pre-attentive sensory memory | journal = Nature | volume = 372 | issue = 6501 | pages = 90–2 | date = November 1994 | pmid = 7969425 | doi = 10.1038/372090a0 | bibcode = 1994Natur.372...90T | s2cid = 4255887 }}</ref> Furthermore, even individual differences in discrimination ability can be probed with the MMN. The MMN is a component of the chain of brain events causing attention switches to changes in the environment. Attentional instructions also affect MMN.<ref name="pmid1886962">{{cite journal | vauthors = Woldorff MG, Hackley SA, Hillyard SA | title = The effects of channel-selective attention on the mismatch negativity wave elicited by deviant tones | journal = Psychophysiology | volume = 28 | issue = 1 | pages = 30–42 | date = January 1991 | pmid = 1886962 | doi = 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb03384.x }}</ref><ref name="pmid9564748">{{cite journal | vauthors = Woldorff MG, Hillyard SA, Gallen CC, Hampson SR, Bloom FE | title = Magnetoencephalographic recordings demonstrate attentional modulation of mismatch-related neural activity in human auditory cortex | journal = Psychophysiology | volume = 35 | issue = 3 | pages = 283–92 | date = May 1998 | pmid = 9564748 | doi = 10.1017/s0048577298961601 }}</ref><ref name="pmid7662904">{{cite journal | vauthors = Oades RD, Dittmann-Balcar A | title = Mismatch negativity (MMN) is altered by directing attention | journal = NeuroReport | volume = 6 | issue = 8 | pages = 1187–90 | date = May 1995 | pmid = 7662904 | doi = 10.1097/00001756-199505300-00028 | s2cid = 37321558 | url = http://cogprints.org/1182/2/Neuroreport.pdf }}</ref><ref name="pmid10716203">{{cite journal | vauthors = Dittmann-Balcar A, Thienel R, Schall U | title = Attention-dependent allocation of auditory processing resources as measured by mismatch negativity | journal = NeuroReport | volume = 10 | issue = 18 | pages = 3749–53 | date = December 1999 | pmid = 10716203 | doi = 10.1097/00001756-199912160-00005 }}</ref><ref name="pmid25221494">{{cite journal | vauthors = Erlbeck H, Kübler A, Kotchoubey B, Veser S | title = Task instructions modulate the attentional mode affecting the auditory MMN and the semantic N400 | journal = Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | volume = 8 | pages = 654 | date = 2014 | pmid = 25221494 | pmc = 4145469 | doi = 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00654 | doi-access = free }}</ref>

==In clinical research==

The MMN has been documented in a number of studies to disclose neuropathological changes.
Presently, the accumulated body of evidence suggests that while the MMN offers unique opportunities to basic research of the information processing of a healthy brain, it might be useful in tapping neurodegenerative changes as well.

MMN, which is elicited irrespective of attention, provides an objective means for evaluating possible auditory discrimination and sensory-memory anomalies in such clinical groups as dyslexics and patients with aphasia, who have a multitude of symptoms including attentional problems. Recent results suggest that a major problem underlying the reading deficit in dyslexia might be an inability of the dyslexics' auditory cortex to adequately model complex sound patterns with fast temporal variation.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Kujala T, Myllyviita K, Tervaniemi M, Alho K, Kallio J, Näätänen R | title = Basic auditory dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstrated by brain activity measurements | journal = Psychophysiology | volume = 37 | issue = 2 | pages = 262–6 | date = March 2000 | pmid = 10731777 | doi = 10.1111/1469-8986.3720262 | url = https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/psychophysiology/article/basic-auditory-dysfunction-in-dyslexia-as-demonstrated-by-brain-activity-measurements/6D671B9289C62183C9C4EFDDF3306D22 }}</ref> According to the results of an ongoing study, MMN might also be used in the evaluation of auditory perception deficits in aphasia.

Alzheimer's patients demonstrate decreased amplitude of MMN, especially with long inter-stimulus intervals; this is thought to reflect reduced span of auditory sensory memory. Parkinsonian patients do demonstrate a similar deficit pattern, whereas alcoholism would appear to enhance the MMN response. This latter, seemingly contradictory, finding could be explained by hyperexcitability of CNS neurones resulting from neuroadaptive changes taking place during a heavy drinking bout.

While the results obtained thus far seem encouraging, several steps need to be taken before the MMN can be used as a clinical tool in patient treatment. A focus of research in the late 1990s aimed to tackle some of the key signal-analysis problems encountered in development of clinical use of MMN and challenges still remain. Nevertheless, as it stands, clinical research employing the MMN has already produced significant knowledge on the CNS functional changes related to cognitive decline in the aforementioned clinical disorders.

A 2010 study found that MMN durations were reduced in a group of schizophrenia patients who later went on to have psychotic episodes, suggesting that MMN durations may predict future psychosis.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bodatsch M, Ruhrmann S, Wagner M, Müller R, Schultze-Lutter F, Frommann I, Brinkmeyer J, Gaebel W, Maier W, Klosterkötter J, Brockhaus-Dumke A | display-authors = 6 | title = Prediction of psychosis by mismatch negativity | journal = Biological Psychiatry | volume = 69 | issue = 10 | pages = 959–66 | date = May 2011 | pmid = 21167475 | doi = 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.057 | s2cid = 26287894 }}</ref> Recent research advocates for the use of MMN in clinical intervention, because MMN can predict treatment response for patients with schizophrenia in the context of pro-cognitive therapeutics.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Hochberger|first1=William C.|last2=Joshi|first2=Yash B.|last3=Thomas|first3=Michael L.|last4=Zhang|first4=Wendy|last5=Bismark|first5=Andrew W.|last6=Treichler|first6=Emily B. H.|last7=Tarasenko|first7=Melissa|last8=Nungaray|first8=John|last9=Sprock|first9=Joyce|last10=Cardoso|first10=Lauren|last11=Swerdlow|first11=Neal|date=February 2019|title=Neurophysiologic measures of target engagement predict response to auditory-based cognitive training in treatment refractory schizophrenia|journal=Neuropsychopharmacology|language=en|volume=44|issue=3|pages=606–612|doi=10.1038/s41386-018-0256-9|issn=1740-634X|pmc=6333927|pmid=30377381}}</ref>


==Theory== ==Theory==


The mainstream "memory trace" interpretation of MMN is that it is elicited in response to violations of simple rules governing the properties of information. It is thought to arise from violation of an automatically formed, short-term neural model or memory trace of physical or abstract environmental regularities (Näätänen & Winkler, 1999).<ref>Näätänen, R., & Winkler, I. (1999). The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 826-859.</ref> However, other than MMN, there is no other neurophysiological evidence for the formation of the memory representation of those regularities.{{Fact|date=April 2008}} The mainstream "memory trace" interpretation of MMN is that it is elicited in response to violations of simple rules governing the properties of information. It is thought to arise from violation of an automatically formed, short-term neural model or memory trace of physical or abstract environmental regularities.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Rinne T, Alho K | title = The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review | journal = Clinical Neurophysiology | volume = 118 | issue = 12 | pages = 2544–90 | date = December 2007 | pmid = 17931964 | doi = 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026 | s2cid = 16167197 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Näätänen R, Winkler I | title = The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 125 | issue = 6 | pages = 826–59 | date = November 1999 | pmid = 10589304 | doi = 10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.826 }}</ref> However, other than MMN, there is no other neurophysiological evidence for the formation of the memory representation of those regularities.{{Citation needed|date=April 2008}}


Integral to this memory trace view is that there are: Integral to this memory trace view is that there are:
Line 26: Line 55:
ii) a separate population of memory neuronal elements that build a neural model of standard stimulation and respond more vigorously when the incoming stimulation violates that neural model, eliciting an MMN. ii) a separate population of memory neuronal elements that build a neural model of standard stimulation and respond more vigorously when the incoming stimulation violates that neural model, eliciting an MMN.


An alternative "fresh afferent" interpretation (Näätänen, 1992; Jääskeläinen et al., 2004)<ref>Näätänen, R. (1992). Attention and brain function. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.</ref><ref>Jääskeläinen, I. P., Ahveninen, J., Bonmassar, G., Dale, A. M., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Levanen, S., et al. (2004). Human posterior auditory cortex gates novel sounds to consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 6809–6814.</ref> is that there are no memory neuronal elements, but the sensory afferent neuronal elements that are tuned to properties of the standard stimulation respond less vigorously upon repeated stimulation. Thus when a deviant activates a distinct new population of neuronal elements that is tuned to the different properties of the deviant rather than the standard, these fresh afferents respond more vigorously, eliciting an MMN. An alternative "fresh afferent" interpretation<ref name="Näätänen, 1992"/><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Jääskeläinen IP, Ahveninen J, Bonmassar G, Dale AM, Ilmoniemi RJ, Levänen S, Lin FH, May P, Melcher J, Stufflebeam S, Tiitinen H, Belliveau JW | display-authors = 6 | title = Human posterior auditory cortex gates novel sounds to consciousness | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 101 | issue = 17 | pages = 6809–14 | date = April 2004 | pmid = 15096618 | pmc = 404127 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.0303760101 | bibcode = 2004PNAS..101.6809J | doi-access = free }}</ref> is that there are no memory neuronal elements, but the sensory afferent neuronal elements that are tuned to properties of the standard stimulation respond less vigorously upon repeated stimulation. Thus when a deviant activates a distinct new population of neuronal elements that is tuned to the different properties of the deviant rather than the standard, these fresh afferents respond more vigorously, eliciting an MMN.


A third view is that the sensory afferents are the memory neurons.<ref>{{cite thesis | vauthors = Ulanovsky N |date= February 2004 |title= Neuronal Adaptation in Cat Auditory Cortex | degree= Ph.D. | publisher = Hebrew University |url= http://icnc.huji.ac.il/phd/theses/files/NachumUlanovsky.pdf | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20160611022636/http://icnc.huji.ac.il/phd/theses/files/NachumUlanovsky.pdf | archive-date = 11 June 2016 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Jääskeläinen IP, Ahveninen J, Belliveau JW, Raij T, Sams M | title = Short-term plasticity in auditory cognition | journal = Trends in Neurosciences | volume = 30 | issue = 12 | pages = 653–61 | date = December 2007 | pmid = 17981345 | doi = 10.1016/j.tins.2007.09.003 | s2cid = 7660360 }}</ref>
A third view is that the sensory afferents are the memory neurons (Ulanovsky, 2004).<ref>Ulanovsky, N. (2004). Neuronal adaptation in cat auditory cortex. Doctoral dissertation. Jerusalem, Israel: Hebrew University.</ref>


==Further reading== == See also ==
{{div col|colwidth=22em}}
*{{cite journal | first=Risto | last=Näätänen | year=2007 | title=The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review | journal=Clinical Neurophysiology | volume=118 | pages=2544-2590 | accessdate=23 November 2008}}
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
{{div col end}}


==References== == References ==
{{reflist}} {{reflist}}


==External links== == External links ==
* at the Cognitive Brain Research Unit at the University of Helsinki * at the Cognitive Brain Research Unit at the University of Helsinki

{{EEG}}


]
] ]
]
] ]
] ]
]

]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 10:21, 30 November 2024

Component in a sequence of stimuli

The mismatch negativity (MMN) or mismatch field (MMF) is a component of the event-related potential (ERP) to an odd stimulus in a sequence of stimuli. It arises from electrical activity in the brain and is studied within the field of cognitive neuroscience and psychology. It can occur in any sensory system, but has most frequently been studied for hearing and for vision, in which case it is abbreviated to vMMN. The (v)MMN occurs after an infrequent change in a repetitive sequence of stimuli (sometimes the entire sequence is called an oddball sequence.) For example, a rare deviant (d) stimulus can be interspersed among a series of frequent standard (s) stimuli (e.g., s s s s s s s s s d s s s s s s d s s s d s s s s...). In hearing, a deviant sound can differ from the standards in one or more perceptual features such as pitch, duration, loudness, or location. The MMN can be elicited regardless of whether someone is paying attention to the sequence. During auditory sequences, a person can be reading or watching a silent subtitled movie, yet still show a clear MMN. In the case of visual stimuli, the MMN occurs after an infrequent change in a repetitive sequence of images.

MMN refers to the mismatch response in electroencephalography (EEG); MMF or MMNM refer to the mismatch response in magnetoencephalography (MEG).

History

The auditory MMN was discovered in 1978 by Risto Näätänen, A. W. K. Gaillard, and S. Mäntysalo at the Institute for Perception, TNO in The Netherlands.

The first report of a visual MMN was in 1990 by Rainer Cammer. For a history of the development of the visual MMN, see Pazo-Alvarez et al. (2003).

Characteristics

The MMN is a response to a deviant within a sequence of otherwise regular stimuli; thus, in an experimental setting, it is produced when stimuli are presented in a many-to-one ratio; for example, in a sequence of sounds s s s s s s s d s s s s d s s s..., the d is the deviant or oddball stimulus, and will elicit an MMN response. The mismatch negativity occurs even if the subject is not consciously paying attention to the stimuli. Processing of sensory stimulus features is essential for humans in determining their responses and actions. If behaviourally relevant aspects of the environment are not correctly represented in the brain, then the organism's behaviour cannot be appropriate. Without these representations our ability to understand spoken language, for example, would be seriously impaired. Cognitive neuroscience has consequently emphasised the importance of understanding brain mechanisms of sensory information processing, that is, the sensory prerequisites of cognition. Most of the data obtained, unfortunately, do not allow the objective measurement of the accuracy of these stimulus representations. In addition, recent cognitive neuroscience seems to have succeeded in extracting such a measure, however. This is the mismatch negativity (MMN), a component of the event-related potential (ERP), first reported by Näätänen, Gaillard, and Mäntysalo (1978). An in-depth review of MMN research can be found in Näätänen (1992) while other recent reviews also provide information on the generator mechanisms of MMN, its magnetic counterpart, MMNm (Näätänen, Ilmoniemi & Alho, 1994), and its clinical applicability.

The auditory MMN can occur in response to deviance in pitch, intensity, or duration. The auditory MMN is a fronto-central negative potential with sources in the primary and non-primary auditory cortex and a typical latency of 150-250 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus. Sources could also include the inferior frontal gyrus, and the insular cortex. The amplitude and latency of the MMN is related to how different the deviant stimulus is from the standard. Large deviances elicit MMN at earlier latencies. For very large deviances, the MMN can even overlap the N100.

The visual MMN can occur in response to deviance in such aspects as color, size, or duration. The visual MMN is an occipital negative potential with sources in the primary visual cortex and a typical latency of 150-250 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus.

Neurolinguistics

As kindred phenomena have been elicited with speech stimuli, under passive conditions that require very little active attention to the sound, a version of MMN has been frequently used in studies of neurolinguistic perception, to test whether or not these participants neurologically distinguish between certain kinds of sounds. The MMN response has been used to study how fetuses and newborns discriminate speech sounds. In addition to these kinds of studies focusing on phonological processing, some research has implicated the MMN in syntactic processing. Some of these studies have attempted to directly test the automaticity of the MMN, providing converging evidence for the understanding of the MMN as a task-independent and automatic response.

For basic stimulus features

MMN is evoked by an infrequently presented stimulus ("deviant"), differing from the frequently-occurring stimuli ("standards") in one or several physical parameters like duration, intensity, or frequency. In addition, it is generated by a change in spectrally complex stimuli like phonemes, in synthesised instrumental tones, or in the spectral component of tone timbre. Also the temporal order reversals elicit an MMN when successive sound elements differ either in frequency, intensity, or duration. The MMN is not elicited by stimuli with deviant stimulus parameters when they are presented without the intervening standards. Thus, the MMN has been suggested to reflect change detection when a memory trace representing the constant standard stimulus and the neural code of the stimulus with deviant parameter(s) are discrepant.

Vs. auditory sensory memory

The MMN data can be understood as providing evidence that stimulus features are separately analysed and stored in the vicinity of auditory cortex (for a discussion, please see the theory section below). The close resemblance of the behaviour of the MMN to that of the previously behaviourally observed "echoic" memory system strongly suggests that the MMN provides a non-invasive, objective, task-independently measurable physiological correlate of stimulus-feature representations in auditory sensory memory.

Relationship to attentional processes

The experimental evidence suggests that the auditory sensory memory index MMN provides sensory data for attentional processes, and, in essence, governs certain aspects of attentive information processing. This is evident in the finding that the latency of the MMN determines the timing of behavioural responses to changes in the auditory environment. Furthermore, even individual differences in discrimination ability can be probed with the MMN. The MMN is a component of the chain of brain events causing attention switches to changes in the environment. Attentional instructions also affect MMN.

In clinical research

The MMN has been documented in a number of studies to disclose neuropathological changes. Presently, the accumulated body of evidence suggests that while the MMN offers unique opportunities to basic research of the information processing of a healthy brain, it might be useful in tapping neurodegenerative changes as well.

MMN, which is elicited irrespective of attention, provides an objective means for evaluating possible auditory discrimination and sensory-memory anomalies in such clinical groups as dyslexics and patients with aphasia, who have a multitude of symptoms including attentional problems. Recent results suggest that a major problem underlying the reading deficit in dyslexia might be an inability of the dyslexics' auditory cortex to adequately model complex sound patterns with fast temporal variation. According to the results of an ongoing study, MMN might also be used in the evaluation of auditory perception deficits in aphasia.

Alzheimer's patients demonstrate decreased amplitude of MMN, especially with long inter-stimulus intervals; this is thought to reflect reduced span of auditory sensory memory. Parkinsonian patients do demonstrate a similar deficit pattern, whereas alcoholism would appear to enhance the MMN response. This latter, seemingly contradictory, finding could be explained by hyperexcitability of CNS neurones resulting from neuroadaptive changes taking place during a heavy drinking bout.

While the results obtained thus far seem encouraging, several steps need to be taken before the MMN can be used as a clinical tool in patient treatment. A focus of research in the late 1990s aimed to tackle some of the key signal-analysis problems encountered in development of clinical use of MMN and challenges still remain. Nevertheless, as it stands, clinical research employing the MMN has already produced significant knowledge on the CNS functional changes related to cognitive decline in the aforementioned clinical disorders.

A 2010 study found that MMN durations were reduced in a group of schizophrenia patients who later went on to have psychotic episodes, suggesting that MMN durations may predict future psychosis. Recent research advocates for the use of MMN in clinical intervention, because MMN can predict treatment response for patients with schizophrenia in the context of pro-cognitive therapeutics.

Theory

The mainstream "memory trace" interpretation of MMN is that it is elicited in response to violations of simple rules governing the properties of information. It is thought to arise from violation of an automatically formed, short-term neural model or memory trace of physical or abstract environmental regularities. However, other than MMN, there is no other neurophysiological evidence for the formation of the memory representation of those regularities.

Integral to this memory trace view is that there are: i) a population of sensory afferent neuronal elements that respond to sound, and; ii) a separate population of memory neuronal elements that build a neural model of standard stimulation and respond more vigorously when the incoming stimulation violates that neural model, eliciting an MMN.

An alternative "fresh afferent" interpretation is that there are no memory neuronal elements, but the sensory afferent neuronal elements that are tuned to properties of the standard stimulation respond less vigorously upon repeated stimulation. Thus when a deviant activates a distinct new population of neuronal elements that is tuned to the different properties of the deviant rather than the standard, these fresh afferents respond more vigorously, eliciting an MMN.

A third view is that the sensory afferents are the memory neurons.

See also

References

  1. Stefanics, G; Kremláček, J; Czigler, I (2014). "Visual mismatch negativity: A predictive coding view". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8 (666): 666. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00666. PMC 4165279. PMID 25278859.
  2. Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Tiitinen H, Jiang D, Alho K (September 1993). "Attention and mismatch negativity". Psychophysiology. 30 (5): 436–50. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02067.x. PMID 8416070.
  3. Brattico E, Tervaniemi M, Näätänen R, Peretz I (October 2006). "Musical scale properties are automatically processed in the human auditory cortex" (PDF). Brain Research. 1117 (1): 162–74. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.023. PMID 16963000. S2CID 8401429.
  4. ^ Näätänen R, Gaillard AW, Mäntysalo S (July 1978). "Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted". Acta Psychologica. 42 (4): 313–29. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9. PMID 685709.
  5. Cammann R (1990). "Is there no MMN in the visual modality?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 13 (2): 234–235. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00078420. S2CID 143448593.
  6. Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F, Amenedo E (July 2003). "MMN in the visual modality: a review". Biological Psychology. 63 (3): 199–236. doi:10.1016/s0301-0511(03)00049-8. PMID 12853168. S2CID 14404599.
  7. ^ Näätänen R (1992). Attention and brain function. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0-8058-0984-8. OCLC 25832590.
  8. Alho K (February 1995). "Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes". Ear and Hearing. 16 (1): 38–51. doi:10.1097/00003446-199502000-00004. PMID 7774768. S2CID 31698419.
  9. Näätänen R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Alho K (September 1994). "Magnetoencephalography in studies of human cognitive brain function". Trends in Neurosciences. 17 (9): 389–95. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(94)90048-5. PMID 7529443. S2CID 10224526.
  10. Näätänen R, Alho K (1995). "Mismatch negativity--a unique measure of sensory processing in audition". The International Journal of Neuroscience. 80 (1–4): 317–37. doi:10.3109/00207459508986107. PMID 7775056.
  11. Rosburg T, Trautner P, Dietl T, Korzyukov OA, Boutros NN, Schaller C, et al. (April 2005). "Subdural recordings of the mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with focal epilepsy". Brain. 128 (Pt 4): 819–28. doi:10.1093/brain/awh442. PMID 15728656.
  12. Phillips HN, Blenkmann A, Hughes LE, Kochen S, Bekinschtein TA, Rowe JB (September 2016). "Convergent evidence for hierarchical prediction networks from human electrocorticography and magnetoencephalography". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 82: 192–205. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.05.001. PMC 4981429. PMID 27389803.
  13. Blenkmann AO, Collavini S, Lubell J, Llorens A, Funderud I, Ivanovic J, et al. (December 2019). "Auditory deviance detection in the human insula: An intracranial EEG study". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 121: 189–200. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2019.09.002. hdl:10852/75077. PMID 31629197. S2CID 202749677.
  14. Campbell T, Winkler I, Kujala T (July 2007). "N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1". Psychophysiology. 44 (4): 530–40. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00529.x. hdl:10138/136216. PMID 17532805.
  15. Phillips C, Pellathy T, Marantz A, Yellin E, Wexler K, Poeppel D, et al. (November 2000). "Auditory cortex accesses phonological categories: an MEG mismatch study". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 12 (6): 1038–55. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.201.5797. doi:10.1162/08989290051137567. PMID 11177423. S2CID 8686819.
  16. Schmidt LA, Segalowitz SJ (2008). Developmental psychophysiology: theory, systems, and methods. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-511-49979-1. OCLC 190792301.
  17. Draganova R, Eswaran H, Murphy P, Huotilainen M, Lowery C, Preissl H (November 2005). "Sound frequency change detection in fetuses and newborns, a magnetoencephalographic study". NeuroImage. 28 (2): 354–61. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.011. PMID 16023867. S2CID 19794076.
  18. Pulvermüller F, Shtyrov Y (2007). "The mismatch negativity as an objective tool for studying higher language functions". In Meyer AS, Wheeldon LR, Krott A (eds.). Automaticity and Control in Language Processing. Advances in Behavioural Brain Science. pp. 217–242. doi:10.4324/9780203968512. ISBN 978-0-203-96851-2.
    Specific experimental studies include the following:
  19. Pulvermüller F, Shtyrov Y, Hasting AS, Carlyon RP (March 2008). "Syntax as a reflex: neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing". Brain and Language. 104 (3): 244–53. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.05.002. PMID 17624417. S2CID 13870754.
  20. Tiitinen H, May P, Reinikainen K, Näätänen R (November 1994). "Attentive novelty detection in humans is governed by pre-attentive sensory memory". Nature. 372 (6501): 90–2. Bibcode:1994Natur.372...90T. doi:10.1038/372090a0. PMID 7969425. S2CID 4255887.
  21. Woldorff MG, Hackley SA, Hillyard SA (January 1991). "The effects of channel-selective attention on the mismatch negativity wave elicited by deviant tones". Psychophysiology. 28 (1): 30–42. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb03384.x. PMID 1886962.
  22. Woldorff MG, Hillyard SA, Gallen CC, Hampson SR, Bloom FE (May 1998). "Magnetoencephalographic recordings demonstrate attentional modulation of mismatch-related neural activity in human auditory cortex". Psychophysiology. 35 (3): 283–92. doi:10.1017/s0048577298961601. PMID 9564748.
  23. Oades RD, Dittmann-Balcar A (May 1995). "Mismatch negativity (MMN) is altered by directing attention" (PDF). NeuroReport. 6 (8): 1187–90. doi:10.1097/00001756-199505300-00028. PMID 7662904. S2CID 37321558.
  24. Dittmann-Balcar A, Thienel R, Schall U (December 1999). "Attention-dependent allocation of auditory processing resources as measured by mismatch negativity". NeuroReport. 10 (18): 3749–53. doi:10.1097/00001756-199912160-00005. PMID 10716203.
  25. Erlbeck H, Kübler A, Kotchoubey B, Veser S (2014). "Task instructions modulate the attentional mode affecting the auditory MMN and the semantic N400". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 654. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00654. PMC 4145469. PMID 25221494.
  26. Kujala T, Myllyviita K, Tervaniemi M, Alho K, Kallio J, Näätänen R (March 2000). "Basic auditory dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstrated by brain activity measurements". Psychophysiology. 37 (2): 262–6. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3720262. PMID 10731777.
  27. Bodatsch M, Ruhrmann S, Wagner M, Müller R, Schultze-Lutter F, Frommann I, et al. (May 2011). "Prediction of psychosis by mismatch negativity". Biological Psychiatry. 69 (10): 959–66. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.057. PMID 21167475. S2CID 26287894.
  28. Hochberger, William C.; Joshi, Yash B.; Thomas, Michael L.; Zhang, Wendy; Bismark, Andrew W.; Treichler, Emily B. H.; Tarasenko, Melissa; Nungaray, John; Sprock, Joyce; Cardoso, Lauren; Swerdlow, Neal (February 2019). "Neurophysiologic measures of target engagement predict response to auditory-based cognitive training in treatment refractory schizophrenia". Neuropsychopharmacology. 44 (3): 606–612. doi:10.1038/s41386-018-0256-9. ISSN 1740-634X. PMC 6333927. PMID 30377381.
  29. Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Rinne T, Alho K (December 2007). "The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review". Clinical Neurophysiology. 118 (12): 2544–90. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026. PMID 17931964. S2CID 16167197.
  30. Näätänen R, Winkler I (November 1999). "The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience". Psychological Bulletin. 125 (6): 826–59. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.826. PMID 10589304.
  31. Jääskeläinen IP, Ahveninen J, Bonmassar G, Dale AM, Ilmoniemi RJ, Levänen S, et al. (April 2004). "Human posterior auditory cortex gates novel sounds to consciousness". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 101 (17): 6809–14. Bibcode:2004PNAS..101.6809J. doi:10.1073/pnas.0303760101. PMC 404127. PMID 15096618.
  32. Ulanovsky N (February 2004). Neuronal Adaptation in Cat Auditory Cortex (PDF) (Ph.D. thesis). Hebrew University. Archived from the original (PDF) on 11 June 2016.
  33. Jääskeläinen IP, Ahveninen J, Belliveau JW, Raij T, Sams M (December 2007). "Short-term plasticity in auditory cognition". Trends in Neurosciences. 30 (12): 653–61. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.09.003. PMID 17981345. S2CID 7660360.

External links

Electroencephalography (EEG)
Related tests
Evoked potentials
Negativity
Bereitschaftspotential
ELAN
N100
Visual N1
N170
N200
N2pc
N400
Contingent negative variation (CNV)
Mismatch negativity
Positivity
C1 & P1
P50
P200
P300
P3a
P3b
P600 (late positivity)
Late positive component
Neural oscillations
Topics
Categories: