Misplaced Pages

The Bell Curve: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 19:56, 20 October 2009 editRror (talk | contribs)Pending changes reviewers, Rollbackers24,040 editsm WikiCleaner 0.98 - Repairing link to disambiguation page - You can help!← Previous edit Latest revision as of 05:00, 3 January 2025 edit undoGreenLipstickLesbian (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Extended confirmed users, New page reviewers, Pending changes reviewers16,061 edits Moving from Category:English-language books to Category:English-language non-fiction books using Cat-a-lot 
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|1994 book by Richard J. Herrnstein and Charles Murray}}
{{POV|date=February 2009}}
{{two other uses|the book|the term in statistics|normal distribution|the term in grading|Bell curve grading}} {{About||the principal, mathematical meaning of the term "bell curve"|Normal distribution|other senses of the term|Bell curve (disambiguation)}}
{{Infobox book
{{Infobox_Book
| name = The Bell Curve |name = The Bell Curve
| image = ] |image = TheBellCurve.gif
|caption = Cover of the first edition
| author = ], ]
|authors = ]<br/>]
| cover_artist =
|cover_artist =
| publisher = ]
|subject = ]<br>]
| release_date = September ]
|publisher = ]
| media_type = Hardcover
| pages = 845 |pub_date = 1994
|media_type = Print (] and ])
| isbn = ISBN 0-02-914673-9
|pages = 845
| oclc= 30913157
|isbn = 0-02-914673-9
|dewey= 305.9/082 20
|congress= BF431 .H398 1994
|oclc= 30913157
}} }}
'''''The Bell Curve''''' is a controversial, best-selling ] book by the late ] ] ] and ] ] ]. Its central point is that intelligence is a better predictor of many factors including financial income, job performance, unwed ], and crime than parents' ] or education level. Also, the book argues that those with high intelligence, which it called the "]", are becoming separated from the general population of those with average and below-average intelligence and that this is a dangerous social trend. Much of the controversy concerns Chapters 13 and 14, in which the authors wrote about the enduring ] and discuss implications of those differences. The authors were reported throughout the popular press as arguing that these IQ differences are ], and they did indeed write in chapter 13: "It seems highly likely to us that both genes and the environment have something to do with racial differences." The introduction to the chapter more cautiously states, "The debate about whether and how much genes and environment have to do with ethnic differences remains unresolved." '''''The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life''''' is a 1994 book by the psychologist ] and the political scientist ] in which the authors argue that ] is substantially influenced by both inherited and environmental factors and that it is a better predictor of many personal outcomes, including financial income, job performance, birth out of ], and involvement in crime than are an individual's parental ]. They also argue that those with high intelligence, the "cognitive elite", are becoming separated from those of average and below-average intelligence, and that this separation is a source of social division within the United States.


The book has been, and remains, highly controversial, especially where the authors discussed purported connections between ] and suggested policy implications based on these purported connections. The authors claimed that average ] (IQ) differences between racial and ethnic groups are at least partly genetic in origin, a view that is now considered discredited by mainstream science.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Bird |first1=Kevin |last2=Jackson |first2=John P. |last3=Winston |first3=Andrew S. |date=2024 |title=Confronting Scientific Racism in Psychology: Lessons from Evolutionary Biology and Genetics |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Famp0001228 |journal=American Psychologist |volume=79 |issue=4 |pages=497–508 |doi=10.1037/amp0001228 |pmid=39037836 |quote=Recent articles claim that the folk categories of race are genetically meaningful divisions, and that evolved genetic differences among races and nations are important for explaining immutable differences in cognitive ability, educational attainment, crime, sexual behavior, and wealth; all claims that are opposed by a strong scientific consensus to the contrary. ... Despite the veneer of modern science, RHR psychologists’ recent efforts merely repeat discredited racist ideas of a century ago. The issue is truly one of scientific standards; if psychology embraced the scientific practices of evolutionary biology and genetics, current forms of RHR would not be publishable in reputable scholarly journals.}}</ref><ref name="VoxConsensus">{{cite web |last1=Turkheimer |first1=Eric |last2=Harden |first2=Kathryn Paige |last3=Nisbett |first3=Richard E. |date=June 15, 2017 |title=There's still no good reason to believe black-white IQ differences are due to genes |url=https://www.vox.com/the-big-idea/2017/6/15/15797120/race-black-white-iq-response-critics |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210504055356/https://www.vox.com/the-big-idea/2017/6/15/15797120/race-black-white-iq-response-critics |archive-date=May 4, 2021 |access-date=29 April 2021 |website=Vox |publisher=Vox Media}}</ref><ref name=":3">{{Cite journal |last1=Panofsky |first1=Aaron |last2=Dasgupta |first2=Kushan |last3=Iturriaga |first3=Nicole |year=2021 |title=How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |volume=175 |issue=2 |pages=387–398 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.24150 |pmc=9909835 |pmid=32986847 |s2cid=222163480 |quote=}}</ref> Many of the references and sources used in the book were advocates for ], whose research was funded by the ] organization ].<ref>Rosenthal, S. J. (1995). The Pioneer Fund: PROD. The American Behavioral Scientist, 39(1), 44.</ref>
The book draws heavily from mainstream science research. However, their analysis is their own. Though Richard Herrnstein had a strong background in ], co-author Charles Murray was not an expert in ].<ref>"Behind the Curve" by Leon J. Kamin in ''Race & IQ'' edited by Ashley Montagu (1999) pp.397-407.</ref>


Shortly after its publication, many people rallied both in criticism and in defense of the book. A number of critical texts were written in response to it. Several criticisms were collected in the book '']''.
The book's title comes from the bell-shaped ] of ] scores. The normal distribution is the limiting distribution of a random quantity which is the sum of smaller, independent random phenomena. The message in the title is that IQ scores are normally distributed because a person's intelligence is the sum of many small random variations in genetic and environmental factors.


==Synopsis==
Shortly after publication, many people rallied both in criticism and defense of the book. Some critics denounced the book and its authors as supporting ]. A number of critical texts, including '']'', were written in response to the book.
{{One source|section|date=December 2019}}


''The Bell Curve'', published in 1994, was written by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray to explain the variations in intelligence in American society, warn of some consequences of that variation, and propose social policies for mitigating the worst of the consequences. The book's title comes from the bell-shaped ] of ] (IQ) scores in a population.{{citation needed|date=June 2022}}
== Content ==


===Introduction===
''The Bell Curve'' contains 941 pages in the first printing and 879 in the revised ]. Much of its material is technical and academic. The book's statistical explanations are styled to appeal to a general audience. There are extensive notes, graphs, and tables.
''The Bell Curve'' is divided into four sections.
:'''Part I''' argues that social ] on the basis of intelligence has been increasing since the beginning of the twentieth century.
:'''Part II''' presents original research showing significant correlations between intelligence and various social and economic outcomes. For instance, based on data as of 1989 this section shows that among whites, intelligence level (cognitive class) is a better predictor of poverty than parents' socioeconomic class as shown on the summary table below:


The book starts with an introduction that appraises the history of the concept of intelligence from ] to modern times. Spearman's introduction of the ] and other early advances in research on intelligence are discussed along with a consideration of links between intelligence testing and racial politics. The 1960s are identified as the period in American history when social problems were increasingly attributed to forces outside the individual. This egalitarian ethos, Herrnstein and Murray argue, cannot accommodate biologically based individual differences.<ref name="devlin">{{cite book |title=Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to The Bell Curve |url=https://archive.org/details/intelligencegene00bern |url-access=registration |first1=Bernie |last1=Devlin |first2=Stephen E. |last2=Fienberg |first3=Daniel P. |last3=Resnick |first4=Kathryn |last4=Roeder |author4-link=Kathryn Roeder |publisher=Springer Science & Business Media |year=1997 |isbn=978-0-387-94986-4}}</ref>
::{|class="wikitable" style="background:lavender;color:black"
|+ Percentage in Poverty
! Category || Parents' socioeconomic class || Cognitive class
|-
|Very high/Very bright || 3 || 2
|-
|High/Bright || 3 || 3
|-
|Average || 7 || 6
|-
|Low/Dull || 12 || 16
|-
|Very low/Very dull || 24 || 30
|-
|Overall average|| 7 || 7
|}


The introduction states six of the authors' assumptions, which they claim to be "beyond significant technical dispute":<ref name="HerrnsteinMurray2010">{{cite book |first1=Richard J. |last1=Herrnstein |first2=Charles |last2=Murray |title=The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=s4CKqxi6yWIC |date=11 May 2010 |publisher=Simon and Schuster |isbn=978-1-4391-3491-7 |pages=22–23}}</ref>
:'''Part III''', by far the most controversial, examines what role IQ plays in contributing to social and economic differences between ethnic groups in the United States.
# There is such a difference as a general factor of cognitive ability on which human beings differ.
:'''Part IV''' discusses the implications of the findings for education and social policy in the United States.
# All standardized tests of academic aptitude or achievement measure this general factor to some degree, but IQ tests expressly designed for that purpose measure it most accurately.
# IQ scores match, to a first degree, whatever it is that people mean when they use the word intelligent, or smart in ordinary language.
# IQ scores are stable, although not perfectly so, over much of a person's life.
# Properly administered IQ tests are not demonstrably biased against social, economic, ethnic, or racial groups.
# Cognitive ability is substantially ], apparently no less than 40 percent and no more than 80 percent.
At the close of the introduction, the authors warn the reader against committing the ] of inferring things about individuals based on the aggregate data presented in the book. They also assert that intelligence is just one of many valuable human attributes and one whose importance among human virtues is overrated.<ref name="devlin"/>


===Part I. The Emergence of a Cognitive Elite===
Herrnstein and Murray in many ways follow in the footsteps of ] researcher ], whose controversial article on the subject appeared in 1969 in the ]. ''The Bell Curve'' argues that:
# Intelligence exists and is accurately measurable across racial, language, and national boundaries.
# Intelligence is one, if not the most, important correlative factor in economic, social, and overall success in the United States, and is becoming more important.
# Intelligence is largely (40% to 80%) genetically heritable.
# No one has so far been able to manipulate IQ long term to any significant degree through changes in environmental factors - except for child adoption - and in light of their failure such approaches are becoming less promising.
# The USA has been in denial regarding these facts, and in light of these findings a better public understanding of the nature of intelligence and its social correlates is necessary to guide future policy decisions in America.


In the first part of the book Herrnstein and Murray chart how American society was transformed in the 20th century. They argue that America evolved from a society where social origin largely determined one's social status to one where cognitive ability is the leading determinant of status. The growth in college attendance, a more efficient recruitment of cognitive ability, and the sorting of cognitive ability by selective colleges are identified as important drivers of this evolution. Herrnstein and Murray propose that the cognitive elite has been produced by a more technological society which offers enough high skill jobs for those with a higher intelligence to fill. They also propose that by removing ], ] or ] as criteria (via the establishment of free primary education and the prohibition of discrimination) the main criteria of success in academic and professional life is becoming primarily based on cognitive ability. Increased occupational sorting by cognitive ability is discussed. They maintain that cognitive ability is the best predictor of worker productivity.<ref name="devlin"/>
Their evidence comes from an analysis of data compiled in the ] (NLSY), a study conducted by the ] ] tracking thousands of Americans starting in the 1980s. All participants in the NLSY took the ] (ASVAB), a battery of ten tests taken by all who apply for entry into the armed services. Four of those tests comprise the ], a measure of cognitive ability comparable to an IQ test. (Some had taken an IQ test in high school, and the median correlation of the AFQT and those tests was .81.) Participants were later evaluated for social and economic outcomes. In general, IQ/AFQT scores were a better predictor of life outcomes than ] background. Similarly, after statistically controlling for differences in IQ, many outcome differences between racial-ethnic groups disappeared. (See also ].)


Herrnstein and Murray argue that due to increasing returns to cognitive ability, a cognitive elite is being formed in America. They argue that this elite is getting richer and progressively more segregated from the rest of society.<ref name="devlin"/>
{| class="wikitable"

|+ Economic and social correlates of IQ
===Part II. Cognitive Classes and Social Behavior===
! IQ || <75 || 75-90 || 90-110 || 110-125 || >125

|-
The second part describes how cognitive ability is related to social behaviors: high ability predicts socially desirable behavior, low ability undesirable behavior. The argument is made that group differences in social outcomes are better explained by intelligence differences rather than socioeconomic status, a perspective, the authors argue, that has been neglected in research.<ref name="devlin"/>

The analyses reported in this part of the book were done using data from the ] (NLSY), a study conducted by the ]'s ] tracking thousands of Americans starting in the 1980s. Only ]s are included in the analyses so as to demonstrate that the relationships between cognitive ability and social behavior are not driven by race or ethnicity.<ref name="devlin"/>

Herrnstein and Murray argue that intelligence is a better predictor of individuals' outcomes than parental socioeconomic status. This argument is based on analyses where individuals' IQ scores are shown to better predict their outcomes as adults than the socioeconomic status of their parents. Such results are reported for many outcomes, including poverty, dropping out of school, unemployment, marriage, divorce, illegitimacy, welfare dependency, criminal offending, and the probability of voting in elections.<ref name="devlin"/>

All participants in the NLSY took the ] (ASVAB), a battery of ten tests taken by all who apply for entry into the armed services. (Some had taken an IQ test in high school, and the median correlation of the ] scores and those IQ test scores was .81). Participants were later evaluated for social and economic outcomes. In general, Herrnstein and Murray argued, IQ/AFQT scores were a better predictor of life outcomes than ] background. Similarly, after statistically controlling for differences in IQ, they argued that many outcome differences between racial-ethnic groups disappeared.

{| class="wikitable"
|+ Economic and social correlates of IQ
! IQ || <75 || 75–90 || 90–110 || 110–125 || >125
|-
| US population distribution || 5 || 20 || 50 || 20 || 5 | US population distribution || 5 || 20 || 50 || 20 || 5
|- |-
| Married by age 30 || 72 || 81 || 81 || 72 || 67 | Married by age 30 || 72 || 81 || 81 || 72 || 67
|- |-
| Out of labor force more than 1 month out of year (men) || 22 || 19 || 15 || 14 || 10 | Out of labor force more than 1 month out of year (men) || 22 || 19 || 15 || 14 || 10
|- |-
| Unemployed more than 1 month out of year (men) || 12 || 10 || 7 || 7 || 2 | Unemployed more than 1 month out of year (men) || 12 || 10 || 7 || 7 || 2
|- |-
| Divorced in 5 years || 21 || 22 || 23 || 15 || 9 | Divorced in 5 years || 21 || 22 || 23 || 15 || 9
|- |-
| % of children w/ IQ in bottom decile (mothers) || 39 || 17 || 6 || 7 || - | % of children w/ IQ in bottom decile (mothers) || 39 || 17 || 6 || 7 ||
|- |-
| Had an ] baby (mothers) || 32 || 17 || 8 || 4 || 2 | Had an ] baby (mothers) || 32 || 17 || 8 || 4 || 2
|- |-
| Lives in poverty || 30 || 16 || 6 || 3 || 2 | Lives in poverty || 30 || 16 || 6 || 3 || 2
|- |-
| Ever ] (men) || 7 || 7 || 3 || 1 || 0 | Ever ] (men) || 7 || 7 || 3 || 1 || 0
|- |-
| Chronic welfare recipient (mothers) || 31 || 17 || 8 || 2 || 0 | Chronic welfare recipient (mothers) || 31 || 17 || 8 || 2 || 0
|- |-
| High school dropout || 55 || 35 || 6 || 4 || 0 | High school dropout || 55 || 35 || 6 || 0.4 || 0
|- |-
|Scored "Yes" on "Middle Class Values Index"{{refn|group=c|According to Herrnstein & Murray the "Middle Class Values Index" was intended "to identify among the NLSY population, in their young adulthood when the index was scored, those people who are getting along with their lives in ways that fit the middle-class stereotype." To score "Yes" on the index, a NLSY subject had to meet all four of the following criteria:
| colspan="6" | Values are the percentage of each IQ sub-population, among whites only, fitting each descriptor. Herrnstein & Murray (1994) pp. 171, 158, 163, 174, 230, 180, 132, 194, 247-248, 194, 146 respectively.
* Received at least a high-school diploma
* Never interviewed while incarcerated
* Still married to one's first spouse
* ''Men only:'' In the labor force, even if not employed
* ''Women only:'' Never gave birth outside of marriage
Excluded from the analysis were never-married individuals who satisfied all other components of the index, and men who were not in the labor force in 1989 or 1990 due to disability or still being in school.<ref>Herrnstein & Murray (1994) pp. 263–264</ref>}} || 16 || 30 || 50 || 67 || 74
|} |}
Values are the percentage of each IQ sub-population, among non-Hispanic whites only, fitting each descriptor.<ref>Herrnstein & Murray (1994) pp.&nbsp;171, 158, 163, 174, 230, 180, 132, 194, 247–248, 194, 146, 264 respectively.</ref>


{{reflist|group=c}}
== Policy recommendations==


===Part III. The National Context===
Herrnstein and Murray recommended the elimination of welfare policies that encourage poor women to have babies:


This part of the book discusses ethnic differences in cognitive ability and social behavior. Herrnstein and Murray report that Asian Americans have a higher mean IQ than white Americans, who in turn outscore black Americans. The book argues that the black-white gap is not due to test bias, noting that IQ tests do not tend to underpredict the school or job performance of black individuals and that the gap is larger on apparently culturally neutral test items than on more culturally loaded items. The authors also note that adjusting for socioeconomic status does not eliminate the black-white IQ gap. However, they argue that the gap is narrowing.<ref name="devlin"/>
:We can imagine no recommendation for using the government to manipulate ] that does not have dangers. But this highlights the problem: The United States already has policies that inadvertently social-engineer who has babies, and it is encouraging the wrong women. ''''If the United States did as much to encourage high-IQ women to have babies as it now does to encourage low-IQ women, it would rightly be described as engaging in aggressive manipulation of fertility.'' The technically precise description of America's fertility policy is that it subsidizes births among poor women, who are also disproportionately at the low end of the intelligence distribution. We urge generally that these policies, represented by the extensive network of cash and services for low-income women who have babies, be ended. The government should stop subsidizing births to anyone rich or poor. The other generic recommendation, as close to harmless as any government program we can imagine, is to make it easy for women to make good on their prior decision not to get pregnant by making available birth control mechanisms that are increasingly flexible, foolproof, inexpensive, and safe (p. 548-549)


According to Herrnstein and Murray, the high ] within races does not necessarily mean that the cause of differences between races is genetic. On the other hand, they discuss lines of evidence that have been used to support the thesis that the black-white gap is at least partly genetic, such as ]. They also discuss possible environmental explanations of the gap, such as the observed generational increases in IQ, for which they coin the term ]. At the close of this discussion, they write:<ref name="devlin"/>
This claim spurred later research in economics and ], which considered that welfare programs for women had a doubly negative effect on aggregate IQ within the transfer group, by allowing the female partner to forgo a full consideration of the male's ability to serve as a provider of familial resources, instead placing greater emphasis on desirable physical or social characteristics (presumed to be not as positively correlated with IQ). Neither of these claims, as originally embodied in text and the follow-on research, dealt with ] as such, but rather demonstrated concern that large numbers of minorities were positioned as recipients, leading to a continual worsening of the measured divergence in intelligence. However, two years later, the ] substantially cut these programs.


{{blockquote|If the reader is now convinced that either the genetic or environmental explanation has won out to the exclusion of the other, we have not done a sufficiently good job of presenting one side or the other. It seems highly likely to us that both genes and environment have something to do with racial differences. What might the mix be? We are resolutely agnostic on that issue; as far as we can determine, the evidence does not yet justify an estimate.}}
In a discussion of the future political outcomes of an intellectually stratified society, they stated that they "fear that a new kind of ] is becoming the dominant ideology of the affluent - not in the social tradition of an ] or in the economic tradition of an ] but 'conservatism' along Latin American lines, where to be conservative has often meant doing whatever is necessary to preserve the mansions on the hills from the ] of the slums below" (p. 518). Moreover, they fear that increasing welfare will create a "custodial state" in "a high-tech and more lavish version of the ] for some substantial minority of the nation's population." They also predict increasing ]: "It is difficult to imagine the United States preserving its heritage of individualism, equal rights before the law, free people running their own lives, once it is accepted that a significant part of the population must be made permanent wards of the states" (p. 526).


The authors also stress that regardless of the causes of differences, people should be treated no differently.<ref name="devlin"/>
==G factor==
Knowledge of the ] is important to evaluate the debates on testing. The ''g'' factor is an almost universally supported, and very important, construct in ].<ref> The g factor: the science of mental ability, A.R. Jensen</ref> In a battery of mental ability tests given to a group of people, all the tests are positively correlated with each other; those who are above average in one will, on average, be above average on the others.


In Part III, the authors also repeat many of the analyses from Part II, but now compare whites to blacks and Hispanics in the NLSY dataset. They find that after controlling for IQ, many differences in social outcomes between races are diminished.<ref name="devlin"/>
Factor analysis can extract a smaller number of factors to account for the variation in the scores; this is possible because the more two tests measure the same thing, the greater their correlations will be. One factor, ''g'' can then be extracted (sometimes after another layer of specific factors are removed). The correlation of the test scores with g is its ''g''-loading; a high one is desirable in a test. <!-- Why is it important to mention a random term like 'g-loading'? It's only used one other place in the article, and there it's pretty much clear from context. -->


The authors discuss the possibility that high birth rates among those with lower IQs may exert a downward pressure on the national distribution of cognitive ability. They argue that immigration may also have a similar effect.<ref name="devlin"/>
''g'' is correlated with a wide range of social outcomes; some are such as ], academic achievement, job performance, and career prestige, poverty, dropping out, and out-of-marriage childbirth. ''g'' correlates with both speed and consistency of performance on elementary cognitive tasks (simple ones that can be done by everybody without failure). All of this was mentioned in ''The Bell Curve'', and many biological and neurological correlates have been discovered since, in addition to the long known ones such as brain size. These include the frequency of alpha brain waves, latency and ] of evoked brain potentials, rate of brain ] metabolism, and general ] as some of the best established ones. Almost all of a test's predictive validity lies in ''g'', as opposed to the more specific factors. The AFQT and IQ tests are very highly g-loaded.


At the close of Part III, Herrnstein and Murray discuss the relation of IQ to social problems. Using the NLSY data, they argue that social problems are a monotonically decreasing function of IQ,<ref name="devlin"/> in other words at lower IQ scores the frequency of social problems increases.
== Responses ==


===Living Together===
Initially, ''The Bell Curve'' received a great deal of positive publicity, including cover stories in '']'' ("the science behind is overwhelmingly mainstream"), early publication (under protest by other writers and editors) in '']'' by its editor-in-chief at the time ], and '']'' (which suggested critics disliked its "appeal to sweet reason" and are "inclined to hang the defendants without a trial"). Early articles and editorials appeared in '']'', '']'' ("makes a strong case"), '']'', '']'', the '']'', and '']''. It received a respectful airing on such shows as '']'', the '']'', the '']'', '']'', '']'', and '']''.<ref>'''' By Jim Naureckas January/February 1995</ref> The book sold over 500,000 copies in hardcover.


In this final chapter, the authors discuss the relevance of cognitive ability for understanding major social issues in America.<ref name="devlin"/>
While the book's popularity was mostly propelled by its controversial claims regarding ], both the accuracy of those claims and the qualifications of the authors soon came under attack in the ]. Herrnstein died before the book was released, leaving its public defense to co-author Charles Murray. Although Herrnstein was a psychologist, Murray is a conservative think tank analyst with a ]. in ] and no credentials in ].


Evidence for experimental attempts to raise intelligence is reviewed. The authors conclude that currently there are no means to boost intelligence by more than a modest degree.<ref name="devlin"/>
Some scholars have condemned the book. ] Professor of ] ] wrote:


The authors criticize the "levelling" of general and secondary education and defend ]. They offer a critical overview of ] policies in colleges and workplaces, arguing that their goal should be equality of opportunity rather than equal outcomes.<ref name="devlin"/>
<blockquote>I believe this book is a ], that its authors must have known it was a fraud when they were writing it, and that Charles Murray must still know it's a fraud as he goes around defending it. After careful reading, I cannot believe its authors were not acutely aware of how they were distorting the material they did include.</blockquote>


Herrnstein and Murray offer a pessimistic portrait of America's future. They predict that a cognitive elite will further isolate itself from the rest of society, while the quality of life deteriorates for those at the bottom of the cognitive scale. As an antidote to this prognosis, they offer a vision of society where differences in ability are recognized and everybody can have a valued place, stressing the role of local communities and clear moral rules that apply to everybody.<ref name="devlin"/>
Professor ], a longtime critic of cognitive ability tests, said the book was "a disservice to and abuse of science." ] ], who teaches in the ], called the book's style of thought "scholarly brinkmanship":


===Policy recommendations===
<blockquote>The authors seem to show the evidence and leave the implications for the reader to figure out; discussing scientific work on intelligence, they never quite say that intelligence is all important and tied to one's genes, yet they signal that this is their belief and that readers ought to embrace the same conclusions.</blockquote>
{{Primary sources|section|date=December 2019}}
Herrnstein and Murray argued the average genetic IQ of the United States is declining, owing to the tendency of the more intelligent having fewer children than the less intelligent, the generation length to be shorter for the less intelligent, and the large-scale immigration to the United States of those with low intelligence. Discussing a possible future political outcome of an intellectually stratified society, the authors stated that they "fear that a new kind of conservatism is becoming the dominant ideology of the affluent—not in the social tradition of an Edmund Burke or in the economic tradition of an Adam Smith but 'conservatism' along Latin American lines, where to be conservative has often meant doing whatever is necessary to preserve the mansions on the hills from the menace of the slums below."<ref>p.&nbsp;518.</ref> Moreover, they fear that increasing welfare will create a "custodial state" in "a high-tech and more lavish version of the Indian reservation for some substantial minority of the nation's population." They also predict increasing ]: "It is difficult to imagine the United States preserving its heritage of individualism, equal rights before the law, free people running their own lives, once it is accepted that a significant part of the population must be made permanent wards of the states."<ref>p.&nbsp;526.</ref>


The authors recommended the elimination of welfare policies which they claim encourage poor women to have babies.<ref>pp. 548–549.</ref>
Columnist ], writing for '']'', described the book as "a scabrous piece of racial pornography masquerading as serious scholarship." "Mr. Murray can protest all he wants," wrote Herbert; "his book is just a genteel way of calling somebody a nigger."<ref name="nyt-herbert">{{cite web
| url = http://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res=9F01E5DE123FF935A15753C1A962958260
| title = In America; Throwing a Curve
| accessdate = 2007-01-09
| last = Herbert
| first = Bob
| authorlink = Bob Herbert
| date = 1994-10-26
| format = HTML
| publisher = ]
}}</ref>


==Reception==
Economist and conservative writer ] criticized the book's conclusions about race and the malleability of IQ, writing:<ref>{{cite journal|url=http://search.opinionarchives.com/Summary/AmericanSpectator/V28I2P32-1.htm|title=Ethnicity and IQ|first=Thomas|last=Sowell|year=1995|volume=28|issue=2|journal=]}}</ref>
{{Very long section|date=September 2024}}
<blockquote>When European immigrant groups in the United States scored below the national average on mental tests, they scored lowest on the abstract parts of those tests. So did white ] children in the United States tested back in the early 1930s... Strangely, Herrnstein and Murray refer to "folklore" that "Jews and other immigrant groups were thought to be below average in intelligence." It was neither folklore nor anything as subjective as thoughts. It was based on hard data, as hard as any data in The Bell Curve. These groups repeatedly tested below average on the mental tests of the ] era, both in the ] and in civilian life. For Jews, it is clear that later tests showed radically different results — during an ] when there was very little intermarriage to change the genetic makeup of American Jews.</blockquote>
''The Bell Curve'' received a great deal of media attention. The book was not distributed in advance to the media, except for a few select reviewers picked by Murray and the publisher, which delayed more detailed critiques for months and years after the book's release.<ref name=":0">{{Cite news |url=http://www.slate.com/articles/briefing/articles/1997/01/the_bell_curve_flattened.html |title=The Bell Curve Flattened |last=Lemann |first=Nicholas |date=1997-01-18 |newspaper=Slate |language=en-US |issn=1091-2339 |access-date=2016-09-10}}</ref> ], reviewing the book in '']'', said that the book "contains no new arguments and presents no compelling data to support its anachronistic ]" and said that the "authors omit facts, misuse statistical methods, and seem unwilling to admit the consequence of their own words."<ref name=":1">{{Cite magazine |url=https://chance.dartmouth.edu/course/topics/curveball.html |title=Curveball |last=Gould |first=Stephen Jay |date=1994-11-28 |magazine=The New Yorker |access-date=2016-09-10}}</ref>


A 1995 article by ] writer ] criticized the media response, saying that "While many of these discussions included sharp criticisms of the book, media accounts showed a disturbing tendency to accept Murray and Herrnstein's premises and evidence even while debating their conclusions".<ref> by Jim Naureckas January/February 1995</ref> A 1995 article by the ] argued that critics had narrowly focused their attention on the book's arguments regarding race and intelligence while ignoring other contents of the book, but added that "the book may have fared even worse had the discussion of race and genetics not distracted attention from some serious problems of analysis and logic in its main arguments. There are indeed some useful messages in the book. But there is also much wrong with it."<ref>{{Cite web |last1=T. Dickens |first1=William |last2=L. Schultze |first2=Charles |last3=J. Kane |first3=Thomas |date=1995-06-01 |title=Does The Bell Curve Ring True? A Closer Look at a Grim Portrait of American Society |url=https://www.brookings.edu/articles/does-the-bell-curve-ring-true-a-closer-look-at-a-grim-portrait-of-american-society/ |access-date=2022-09-25 |website=] |language=en-US}}</ref>
In its defense, fifty-two professors, most of them psychologists including researchers in the study of intelligence and related fields, signed an opinion statement titled "]" endorsing the views presented in ''The Bell Curve''. The statement was written by psychologist ] and published in '']'' in ] and reprinted in the '']''. Only seven of the 100 invitees contacted said the statement did not represent the mainstream view of intelligence. Some of the signers had been cited as sources for Murray and Herrnstein's book.


After reviewers had more time to review the book's research and conclusions, more significant criticisms began to appear.<ref name=":0" /> ], writing in ], said that later reviews showed the book was "full of mistakes ranging from sloppy reasoning to mis-citations of sources to outright mathematical errors."<ref name=":0" /> Lemann said that "Unsurprisingly, all the mistakes are in the direction of supporting the authors' thesis."<ref name=":0" />
===American Psychological Association task force report===
In response to the growing controversy surrounding ''The Bell Curve'', the ]'s Board of Scientific Affairs established a special task force to publish an investigative report on the research presented in the book. The final report, titled ], is available at an academic website.


Some of the task force's findings supported or were consistent with statements from ''The Bell Curve''. They agreed that: Many criticisms were collected in the book '']''.
* IQ scores have high predictive validity for individual differences in school achievement.
* IQ scores have predictive validity for adult occupational status, even when variables such as education and family background have been statistically controlled.
* Individual differences in intelligence are substantially influenced by both genetics and environment.
* There is little evidence to show that childhood diet influences intelligence except in cases of severe ].
* There are no statistically significant differences between the IQ scores of males and females.


===Lack of peer review===
Regarding Murray and Herrnstein's claims about racial differences and genetics, the APA task force stated:
Herrnstein and Murray did not submit their work to ] before publication, an omission many have seen as incompatible with their presentation of it as a scholarly text.<ref name=":0" /><ref>Arthur S. Goldberger and Charles F. Manski (1995) "Review Article: ''The Bell Curve by Herrnstein and Murray''", ''Journal of Economic Literature'', 36(2), June 1995, pp. 762–776. "HM and their publishers have done a disservice by circumventing peer review.... a process of scientific review is now under way. But, given the process to date, peer review of ''The Bell Curve'' is now an exercise in damage control{{nbsp}}..."</ref> ] noted that the book was not circulated in ], a common practice to allow potential reviewers and media professionals an opportunity to prepare for the book's arrival.<ref name=":0" />
<blockquote>There is certainly no such support for a genetic interpretation... . It is sometimes suggested that the Black/ White differential in psychometric intelligence is partly due to genetic differences (Jensen, 1972). There is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis.</blockquote>


==="Mainstream Science on Intelligence" statement===
Regarding statements about other explanations for racial differences, the APA task force stated:
{{Main article|Mainstream Science on Intelligence}}
<blockquote>The differential between the ] ] scores of Blacks and Whites (about one standard deviation, although it may be diminishing) does not result from any obvious biases in test construction and administration, nor does it simply reflect differences in socio-economic status. Explanations based on factors of ] and ] may be appropriate, but so far have little direct empirical support.</blockquote>


{{anchor|Mainstream Science on Intelligence|reason=Old section name, probably has incoming links.}}
Regarding statements about any explanations for racial differences, the APA task force stated:
An opinion statement endorsing a number of the views presented in ''The Bell Curve'' called "]"<ref name="Gottfredson 1997 13–23">{{cite journal |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S. |author-link=Linda Gottfredson |date=1997 |title=Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial) |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=24 |pages=13–23 |doi=10.1016/s0160-2896(97)90011-8 |issn=0160-2896}}</ref> was published in '']'' in 1994 and subsequently reprinted in the journal '']''. The statement was drafted by ], a professor of ] at the ]. It was sent to 131 researchers whom Gottfredsen described as "experts in intelligence and allied fields". Of these, 52 signed the statement, 48 returned the request with an explicit refusal to sign, and 31 ignored the request.<ref name="Gottfredson 1997 17–20">{{harvnb|Gottfredson|1997|pp=17–20}}</ref><ref name=":02">{{cite web |title=Linda Gottfredson |url=https://www.splcenter.org/fighting-hate/extremist-files/individual/linda-gottfredson |access-date=2018-02-11 |website=] |language=en}}</ref>
<blockquote>At present, no one knows what causes this differential.</blockquote>


According to a 1996 response by former ] president ], only ten of those who signed were actual experts in intelligence measurement.<ref name="Campbell 1996">{{harvnb|Campbell|1996}}</ref> The ] reports that 20 of the signers were recipients of funding from the ] organization the ], including Gottfredson herself.<ref name=":02" />
The APA journal that published the statement, '']'', subsequently published eleven critical responses in January 1997.


In subsequent years, both the substance and the interpretation of this letter have received widespread criticism from the scientific community.<ref name="Graves 1995 279–280">{{harvnb|Graves|Johnson|1995|pp=279–280}}</ref><ref name="Laosa 1996">{{harvnb|Laosa|1996}}</ref><ref name="Campbell 1996" /><ref name="Harrington 1997 116–118">{{harvnb|Harrington|1997|pp=116–118}}</ref><ref>{{harvtxt|Alderfer|2003}}</ref><ref>{{harvtxt|Armour-Thomas|2003}}</ref>
{{seealso|intelligence testing}}


===Criticisms=== ===APA task force report===
{{Main article|Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns}}
It is doubtful whether any book in the entire history of psychology has been so extensively criticized as ''The Bell Curve''.<ref>''The Attack on The Bell Curve'' By Richard Lynn Personality and Individual Differences 26, (1999), pp. 761-765</ref> Perhaps the most prominent critic of ''The Bell Curve'' was the late ], who in 1996 released a revised and expanded edition of his 1981 controversial book '']'' intended to more directly refute many of ''The Bell Curve'''s claims regarding ]. Specifically, Gould argued that the then current evidence showing heritability of IQ did not indicate a genetic origin to group differences in intelligence. Murray claims that Gould misstated his claims; for instance, Gould says Murray boils down intelligence to a single factor while Murray denies making such a claim.
{{Primary sources|section|date=December 2019}}
In response to the controversy surrounding ''The Bell Curve'', the ]'s Board of Scientific Affairs established a special task force chaired by ] to publish an investigative report focusing solely on the research presented in the book, not the policy recommendations that it made. The report, "]", was first released in 1995 and published in '']'' in 1996.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Neisser |first1=Ulric |date=1996 |title=Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns |url=http://www.gifted.uconn.edu/siegle/research/Correlation/Intelligence.pdf |journal=American Psychologist |volume=51 |issue=2 |pages=77–101 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77 |last2=Boodoo |first2=Gwyneth |last3=Bouchard |first3=Thomas J. Jr. |last4=Boykin |first4=A. Wade |last5=Brody |first5=Nathan |last6=Ceci |first6=Stephen J. |last7=Halpern |first7=Diane F. |last8=Loehlin |first8=John C. |last9=Perloff |first9=Robert |last10=Sternberg |first10=Robert J. |last11=Urbina |first11=Susana}}</ref>


Regarding explanations for racial differences, the APA task force stated:
The initial positive reception of ''The Bell Curve'' in media such as newspapers and television talk shows was troubling to critics such as economist ] and evolutionary biologist ] who felt that it indicated an acceptance of what Herman calls "deterministic racist doctrines."<ref></ref>


{{blockquote|text=The cause of that differential is not known; it is apparently not due to any simple form of bias in the content or administration of the tests themselves. The ] shows that environmental factors can produce differences of at least this magnitude, but that effect is mysterious in its own right. Several culturally based explanations of the Black/White IQ differential have been proposed; some are plausible, but so far none has been conclusively supported. There is even less empirical support for a genetic interpretation. In short, no adequate explanation of the differential between the IQ means of Blacks and Whites is presently available.}}
The second wave of reviews, which did not arrive until much later, was composed of expert opinion in the relevant fields. It provided a belated substitute for the ] process to which Murray and Herrnstein did not originally submit their work.


''American Psychologist'' subsequently published eleven critical responses in January 1997.<ref>{{harvtxt|Alderfer|2003}}</ref>
], professor of ] and associate professor of ] and ] at ], called ''Bell Curve'' a "deliberate assault on efforts to improve the school performance of African-Americans":
<blockquote>This book presented strong evidence that genes play a role in intelligence but linked it to the unsupported claim that genes explain the small but consistent black-white difference in IQ. The ] of good argument with a bad one seemed politically motivated, and persuasive refutations soon appeared. Actually, African-Americans have excelled in virtually every enriched environment they have been placed in, most of which they were previously barred from, and this in only the first decade or two of improved but still not equal opportunity. It is likely that the real curves for the two races will one day be superimposable on each other, but this may require decades of change and different environments for different people. Claims about genetic potential are meaningless except in light of this requirement.<ref>''The Tangled Wing Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit'' by Melvin Konner, 2nd edition, p. 428</ref></blockquote>


==== Assumptions ==== ===Criticism of assumptions===

Much of the criticism of ''The Bell Curve'' has focused on potential flaws in the basic assumptions made at the beginning of the book. William J. Matthews and ] list four basic assumptions of ''The Bell Curve'':
====Criticism by Stephen Jay Gould====

] wrote that the "entire argument" of the authors of ''The Bell Curve'' rests on four unsupported, and mostly false, assumptions about intelligence:<ref name=":1" /><ref>{{Cite web |url=https://bolesblogs.com/1998/03/23/a-review-of-the-bell-curve-bad-science-makes-for-bad-conclusions/ |title=A Review of ''The Bell Curve'': Bad Science Makes for Bad Conclusions |date=1998-03-23 |website=David Boles, Blogs |access-date=2016-09-10}}</ref>
# Intelligence must be reducible to a single number. # Intelligence must be reducible to a single number.
# Intelligence must be capable of rank ordering people in a linear order. # Intelligence must be capable of rank ordering people in a linear order.
# Intelligence must be primarily genetically based. # Intelligence must be primarily genetically based.
# Intelligence must be essentially immutable. # Intelligence must be essentially immutable.


In a 1995 interview with ] of '']'', Murray denied making each of these four assumptions.<ref name=miele-interview>{{cite news |last1=Miele |first1=Frank |author-link1=Frank Miele |title=An Interview with the Author of The Bell Curve |url=http://www.skeptic.com/archives24.html |access-date=November 26, 2019 |work=] |date=1995 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20050205010706/http://www.skeptic.com/archives24.html |archive-date=February 5, 2005}}</ref>
According to Gould, if any of these premises are false, then their entire argument disintegrates (Gould, 1994).<ref></ref> Similarly, in ''"Science" in the service of Racism,'' C. Loring Brace writes that ''The Bell Curve'' makes ''six'' basic assumptions at the beginning of the book:


====Criticism by James Heckman====
# Human Cognitive ability is a single general entity, depictable as a single number.
The ]-winning economist ] considers two assumptions made in the book to be questionable: that ''g'' accounts for ] across test scores and performance in society, and that ''g'' cannot be manipulated. Heckman's reanalysis of the evidence used in ''The Bell Curve'' found contradictions:
# Cognitive ability has a heritability of between 40 and 80 percent and is therefore primarily genetically based.
# The factors that explain wages receive different weights than the factors that explain test scores. More than ''g'' is required to explain either.
# IQ is essentially ], fixed over the course of a life span.
# Other factors besides ''g'' contribute to social performance, and they can be manipulated.<ref name=heckman>{{Cite journal |last=Heckman |first=James J. |date=1995 |title=Lessons from the Bell Curve |journal=] |volume=103 |issue=5 |pages=1091–1120 |doi=10.1086/262014 |s2cid=153463195}}.</ref>
# IQ tests measure how "smart" or "intelligent" people are and are capable of rank ordering people in a linear order.
In response, Murray argued that this was a ] and that the book does not argue that ''g'' or IQ are totally immutable or the only factors affecting outcomes.<ref>August 1995 letter exchange in '']'' magazine</ref>
# IQ tests can measure this accurately.
# IQ tests are not biased with regard to race ethnic group or socioeconomic status.


In a 2005 interview, Heckman praised ''The Bell Curve'' for breaking "a taboo by showing that differences in ability existed and predicted a variety of socioeconomic outcomes" and for playing "a very important role in raising the issue of differences in ability and their importance" and stated that he was "a bigger fan of than you might think." However, he also maintained that Herrnstein and Murray overestimated the role of heredity in determining intelligence differences.<ref name="theregion">''''. Douglas Clement. June 2005. ''The Region.''</ref>
Brace proceeds to argue that there are faults in every one of these assumptions. The ] winning economist ] writes that two assumptions made in the book are questionable:


====Criticism by Noam Chomsky====
# "g" accounts for ] across test scores and performance in society.
# "g" cannot be manipulated.


In 1995, ], a founder in the field of ], criticized the book and its assumptions on IQ. He takes issue with the idea that IQ is 60% heritable, arguing that the "statement is meaningless" because ] does not have to be genetic. Chomsky gives the example of women wearing ]:
Heckman writes that a reanalysis of the evidence used in ''The Bell Curve'' contradicts this story. The factors that explain wages receive different weights than the factors that explain test scores. More than "g" is required to explain either. Other factors besides "g" contribute to social performance, and they ''can'' be manipulated.<ref name=heckman>'''' James J. Heckman The Journal of Political Economy, Vol. 103, No. 5 (Oct., 1995), pp. 1091-1120</ref> Murray responded to a shorter version of Heckman's critique in an August 1995 letter exchange in '']'' magazine.


{{blockquote|To borrow an example from ], "some years ago when only women wore earrings, the heritability of having an earring was high because differences in whether a person had an earring was due to a chromosomal difference, XX vs. XY." No one has yet suggested that wearing earrings, or ties, is "in our genes," an inescapable fate that environment cannot influence, "dooming the liberal notion."<ref name=Rollback> Noam Chomsky, 1995</ref>}}He goes on to say there is almost no evidence of a genetic link, and greater evidence that environmental issues are what determine IQ differences.
==== Methodology ====
Michael Hout of the ], along with five colleagues, recalculated the effect of socioeconomic status, using the same variables as ''The Bell Curve,'' but weighting them differently. They found that if IQ scores are corrected, as Herrnstein and Murray did, to eliminate the effect of ], the ability of IQ to predict ] can be made to look dramatically overstated, by as much as 61 percent for whites and 74 percent for blacks. In other words, according to Hout et al., Herrnstein and Murray's finding, that IQ predicts poverty much better than socioeconomic status does, is substantially a result of the way they handled the statistics.<ref> Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos</ref>


==== Criticism by Ned Block ====
In August 1995, at the ] ] Sanders Korenman and ] sociologist ] found certain errors in Herrnstein's methodology. Korenman and Winship concluded:"... there is evidence of substantial bias due to measurement error in their estimates of the effects of parents` socioeconomic status. In addition, Herrnstein and Murray`s measure of parental socioeconomic status (SES) fails to capture the effects of important elements of family background (such as single-parent family structure at age 14). As a result, their analysis gives an exaggerated impression of the importance of IQ relative to parents` SES, and relative to family background more generally. Estimates based on a variety of methods, including analyses of siblings, suggest that parental family background is at least as important, and may be more important than IQ in determining socioeconomic success in adulthood."<ref> http://ssrn.com/abstract=225294 Korenman, Sanders and Winship, Christopher, "A Reanalysis of The Bell Curve" (August 1995). NBER Working Paper Series, Vol. w5230, 1995. </ref>
{{See also|Race and intelligence#heritability within and between groups}}
Philosopher ] argues that ''The Bell Curve'' misleads about intelligence as it conflates genetic determination with ].<ref name=":2">{{Cite web |last=Block |first=Ned |date=January 6, 1996 |title=How Heritability Misleads about Race |url=https://www.nyu.edu/gsas/dept/philo/faculty/block/papers/Heritability.html |website=]}}</ref> Genetic determination and heritability are not interchangeable as there are traits that are genetically determined but not heritable, and vice versa. For example, the number of fingers on a human hand are genetically determined as genes code for five fingers in nearly everybody. However, the heritability of the number of fingers is very low, as variations in numbers of fingers are usually environmentally caused. The aforementioned earring example quoted by Chomsky is an instance where the opposite is true: high heritability, but not genetic determination.<ref name=":2" /> Given that genetic determination and heritability are not equivalent, Block contends that IQ is one such trait that is heritable but not genetically determined. Finally, Block contends that utilizing twin studies to randomize the environment automatically fail: Black twins will always bring a part of their environment with them as they are both Black and will be treated as such.<ref name=":2" />


Canadian psychologist ] concurs with Block that twin studies fail to draw conclusions about heritability, and as a result Murray's work is methodologically flawed.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Segalowitz |first=Sidney J. |date=October 1999 |title=Why twin studies really don't tell us much about human heritability |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x99442207 |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |volume=22 |issue=5 |pages=904–905 |doi=10.1017/s0140525x99442207 |s2cid=143865688 |issn=0140-525X}}</ref>
In the book ''Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to The Bell Curve,'' a group of social scientists and statisticians analyzes the genetics-intelligence link, the concept of intelligence, the malleability of intelligence and the effects of education, the relationship between ], ]s and ], pathways to racial and ethnic inequalities in ], and the question of ]. This work argues that much of the public response was polemic, and failed to analyze the details of the science and validity of the statistical arguments underlying the book's conclusions.<ref>''Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to "the Bell Curve"'' Bernie Devlin, Richard J. Herrnstein ISBN 0387949860</ref>


=== Statistical methods ===
William J. Matthews writes that part of ''The Bell Curve'''s analysis is based on the AFQT "which is not an IQ test but designed to predict performance of certain criterion variables".<ref name="badscience">William J. Matthews, Ph.D. (1998) ''''</ref> ] winner ] observed that the AFQT was designed only to predict success in military training schools and that most of these tests appear to be achievement tests rather than ability tests, measuring factual knowledge and not pure ability. He continues:
], ], Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, ], and Kim Voss in the book '']'' recalculated the effect of socioeconomic status, using the same variables as ''The Bell Curve'', but weighting them differently. They found that if IQ scores are adjusted, as Herrnstein and Murray did, to eliminate the effect of ], the ability of IQ to predict ] can become dramatically larger, by as much as 61 percent for whites and 74 percent for blacks. According to the authors, Herrnstein and Murray's finding that IQ predicts poverty much better than socioeconomic status is substantially a result of the way they handled the statistics.<ref>'''' Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Princeton University Press, 1996.</ref>
:Ironically, the authors delete from their composite AFQT score a timed test of numerical operations because it is not highly correlated with the other tests. Yet it is well known that in the data they use, this subtest is the single best predictor of earnings of all the AFQT test components. The fact that many of the subtests are only weakly correlated with each other, and that the best predictor of earnings is only weakly correlated with their "g-loaded" score, only heightens doubts that a single-ability model is a satisfactory description of human intelligence. It also drives home the point that the "g-loading" so strongly emphasized by Murray and Herrnstein measures only agreement among tests&mdash;not predictive power for socioeconomic outcomes. By the same token, one could also argue that the authors have biased their empirical analysis against the conclusions they obtain by disregarding the test with the greatest predictive power.<ref name="reason">'''' James J. Heckman. March 1995. ''Reason''</ref><ref name=heckman/>


In August 1995, ] ] Sanders Korenman and ] sociologist ] argued that measurement error was not properly handled by Herrnstein and Murray. Korenman and Winship concluded: "...{{nbsp}}there is evidence of substantial bias due to measurement error in their estimates of the effects of parents' socioeconomic status. In addition, Herrnstein and Murray's measure of parental socioeconomic status (SES) fails to capture the effects of important elements of family background (such as single-parent family structure at age 14). As a result, their analysis gives an exaggerated impression of the importance of IQ relative to parents' SES, and relative to family background more generally. Estimates based on a variety of methods, including analyses of siblings, suggest that parental family background is at least as important, and may be more important than IQ in determining socioeconomic success in adulthood."<ref>http://ssrn.com/abstract=225294 Korenman, Sanders and Winship, Christopher, "A Reanalysis of The Bell Curve" (August 1995). NBER Working Paper Series, Vol. w5230, 1995.</ref>
==== Contradictory findings ====
'' Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser 2000</ref>]]


In the book ''Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to The Bell Curve'', a group of social scientists and statisticians analyzes the genetics-intelligence link, the concept of intelligence, the malleability of intelligence and the effects of education, the relationship between ], ]s and ], pathways to racial and ethnic ], and the question of ]. This work argues that much of the public response was polemic, and failed to analyze the details of the science and validity of the statistical arguments underlying the book's conclusions.<ref name="devlin"/>
A recent paper in the ''Psychological Review'', "," presents a mechanism by which environmental effects on IQ may be magnified by feedback effects. This approach may provide a resolution of the contradiction between the viewpoint of ''The Bell Curve'' and its supporters, and the ] of IQ believed to exist by its critics. Janet Currie and Duncan Thomas presented evidence suggesting AFQT scores are likely better markers for family background than "intelligence" in a 1999 Study. <blockquote>Herrnstein and Murray report that conditional on maternal "intelligence" (AFQT scores), child test scores are little affected by variations in socio-economic status. Using the same data, we demonstrate their finding is very fragile.<ref>''The Intergenerational Transmission of 'Intelligence' Down the Slippery Slopes of 'The Bell Curve' '' Industrial Relations: A Journal of Economy and Society, Vol. 38, No. 3, July 1999</ref></blockquote>
Charles R. Tittle, Thomas Rotolo found that the more that written, IQ-like examinations are used as screening devices for occupational access, the ''stronger'' the relationship between IQ and income. Thus, rather than higher IQ leading to status attainment because it indicates skills needed in a modern society, IQ may reflect the same test-taking abilities used in artificial screening devices by which status groups protect their domains.<ref>''IQ and Stratification: An Empirical Evaluation of Herrnstein and Murray's Social Change Argument'' Charles R. Tittle, Thomas Rotolo Social Forces, Vol. 79, No. 1 (Sep., 2000), pp. 1-28</ref> Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser write that Herrnstein and Murray provide scant evidence of growth in cognitive sorting. Using data from the General Social Survey, they tested each of these hypotheses using a short verbal ability test which was administered to about 12,500 American adults between 1974 and 1994; the results provided no support for any of the trend hypotheses advanced by Herrnstein and Murray. One chart in The Bell Curve purports to show that people with IQs above 120 have become "rapidly more concentrated" in high-IQ occupations since 1940. But Robert Hauser and his colleague Min-Hsiung Huang retested the data and came up with estimates that fell “well below those of Herrnstein and Murray." They add that the data, properly used, "do not tell us anything except that selected, highly educated occupation groups have grown rapidly since 1940."<ref>'''' Hauser R.M.; Huang M.H.</ref>


=== Use of Armed Forces Qualification Test ===
==== Fueling racism ====
William J. Matthews writes that part of ''The Bell Curve'''s analysis is based on the ] (AFQT) "which is not an IQ test but designed to predict performance of certain criterion variables".<ref name="badscience">William J. Matthews, Ph.D. (1998) ''''</ref> The AFQT covers subjects such as trigonometry.<ref name=":0" />
Dennis M. Rutledge suggests that through ]s of works like Jensen's famous study on the achievement gap, and Herrnstein and Murray's book ''The Bell Curve,'' the media "paints a picture of Blacks and other people of color as collective biological illiterates—as not only intellectually unfit but evil and criminal as well," thus providing, he says "the logic and justification for those who would further disenfranchise and exclude racial and ethnic minorities."<ref> Journal of Negro Education, The, Summer 1995 by Dennis, Rutledge M</ref>


Heckman observed that the AFQT was designed only to predict success in military training schools and that most of these tests appear to be achievement tests rather than ability tests, measuring factual knowledge and not pure ability. He continues:<ref name=heckman/><ref name="reason">'''' James J. Heckman. March 1995. ''Reason''</ref>
<!-- What on earth is the 'lookalike' bit supposed to mean? -->
Some critics point out that 17 of the researchers whose work is referenced by the book are also contributors to '']'', America's leading eugenics magazine, which reprints articles such as by Francis Galton. In his book ''The Bell Curve Wars: Race, Intelligence, and the Future of America'', Steven Fraser writes that "by scrutinizing the footnotes and bibliography in ''The Bell Curve'',
readers can more easily recognize the project for what it is: a chilly
synthesis of the work of disreputable race theorists and eccentric
]".<ref>'''' Book by Steven Fraser; Basic Books, 1995</ref>


{{blockquote|Ironically, the authors delete from their composite AFQT score a timed test of numerical operations because it is not highly correlated with the other tests. Yet it is well known that in the data they use, this subtest is the single best predictor of earnings of all the AFQT test components. The fact that many of the subtests are only weakly correlated with each other, and that the best predictor of earnings is only weakly correlated with their "g-loaded" score, only heightens doubts that a single-ability model is a satisfactory description of human intelligence. It also drives home the point that the "g-loading" so strongly emphasized by Murray and Herrnstein measures only agreement among tests—not predictive power for socioeconomic outcomes. By the same token, one could also argue that the authors have biased their empirical analysis against the conclusions they obtain by disregarding the test with the greatest predictive power.}}
Since the book promulgated the idea that blacks were on average less intelligent than whites, some people fear that ''The Bell Curve'' can be used by extremists to justify genocide and hate crimes.<ref> by Jerry Coyne</ref><ref></ref> Critics have noted much of the work referenced by the Bell Curve was funded by the ], which aims to advance the scientific study of heredity and human differences, and has been accused of promoting ].<ref></ref><ref>. ], ]</ref><ref>slate.com, , Stephen Metcalf, ], ].</ref>


] and Duncan Thomas presented evidence suggesting AFQT scores are likely better markers for family background than "intelligence" in a 1999 study:
==== Author's follow-up ====
{{blockquote|Herrnstein and Murray report that conditional on maternal "intelligence" (AFQT scores), child test scores are little affected by variations in socio-economic status. Using the same data, we demonstrate their finding is very fragile.<ref>"The Intergenerational Transmission of 'Intelligence' Down the Slippery Slopes of ''The Bell Curve''". ''Industrial Relations: A Journal of Economy and Society'', Vol. 38, No. 3, July 1999</ref>}}


=== Cognitive sorting and the "cognitive elite" concept ===
{| class="wikitable"
The cognitive elite concept has been widely criticized. Frank Wilson refuted "as cultural superstition and social science pornography ''The Bell Curve'''s theories on the role of intelligence in the social stratification of postindustrial America."<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Wilson |first1=Frank Harold |date=1995 |title=For Whom Does the Bell Toll?: Meritocracy, the Cognitive Elite, and the Continuing Significance of Race in Postindustrial America |journal=The Journal of Negro Education |volume=64 |issue=3 |pages=253–266 |doi=10.2307/2967207 |jstor=2967207}}</ref> Nicholas Lemann described the notion of a cognitive elite as "a sociological cartoon with political uses, not a phenomenon to be accepted at face value."<ref>{{cite book |last1=Lemann |first1=Nicholas |title=Intelligence, Genes, and Success |publisher=Springer New York |year=1997 |isbn=978-0-387-94986-4 |pages= |language=en |chapter=Is There a Cognitive Elite in America? |doi=10.1007/978-1-4612-0669-9_14 |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/intelligencegene00bern/page/315}}</ref>
|-
|+ '''Relation between IQ and earnings in the U.S.'''
! IQ || <75 || 75&ndash;90 || 90&ndash;110 || 110&ndash;125 || >125
|-
| Age 18 || 2,000 || 5,000 || 8,000 || 8,000 || 3,000
|-
| Age 26 || 3,000 || 10,000 || 16,000 || 20,000 || 21,000
|-
| Age 32 || 5,000 || 12,400 || 20,000 || 27,000 || 36,000
|-
| colspan="6" | Values are the average earnings (1993 US Dollars) of each IQ sub-population.<ref>Murray, C. (1997). IQ and economic success. Public Interest, 128, 21–35.</ref>
|}


{{ill|Charles R. Tittle|de}} and Thomas Rotolo found that the more the written, IQ-like, examinations are used as screening devices for occupational access, the stronger the relationship between IQ and income. Thus, rather than higher IQ leading to status attainment because it indicates skills needed in a modern society, IQ may reflect the same test-taking abilities used in artificial screening devices by which status groups protect their domains.<ref>"IQ and Stratification: An Empirical Evaluation of Herrnstein and Murray's Social Change Argument". Charles R. Tittle, Thomas Rotolo ''Social Forces'', Vol. 79, No. 1 (Sep. 2000), pp. 1–28</ref>
Murray responded to specific criticisms of the analysis of the practical importance of IQ compared to socio-economic status (Part II of ''The Bell Curve'') in a 1998 book ''Income Inequality and IQ''<ref name=murray98>Murray, C. (1998). . Washington: AEI Press.</ref> To circumvent criticisms surrounding their use of a statistical control for socioeconomic status (SES), Murray adopted a sibling design. Rather than statistically controlling for parental SES, Murray compared life outcome differences among full sibling pairs who met a number of criteria in which one member of the pair has an IQ in the "normal" range and the other siblings has an IQ in a higher or lower IQ category. According to Murray, this design controls for all aspects of family background (full siblings share the same family background, growing up together in the same home and the same community).


Min-Hsiung Huang and ] write that Herrnstein and Murray provide scant evidence of growth in cognitive sorting. Using data from the General Social Survey, they tested each of these hypotheses using a short verbal ability test which was administered to about 12,500 American adults between 1974 and 1994; the results provided no support for any of the trend hypotheses advanced by Herrnstein and Murray. One chart in ''The Bell Curve'' purports to show that people with IQs above 120 have become "rapidly more concentrated" in high-IQ occupations since 1940. But Robert Hauser and his colleague Min-Hsiung Huang retested the data and came up with estimates that fell "well below those of Herrnstein and Murray." They add that the data, properly used, "do not tell us anything except that selected, highly educated occupation groups have grown rapidly since 1940."<ref>'''' Hauser R. M.; Huang M. H.</ref>
{| class="wikitable"
|+ '''Comparison of ''The Bell Curve'' control for parental SES and the sibling fixed-effect model of ''IQ and Income Inequality'' for IQ regression'''
!Indicator||Bell Curve control for parental SES||Sibling fixed-effect model
|----
|Annual earnings, year-round workers
|5548
|5317
|----
|Years of schooling
|0.59
|0.45
|----
|Attainment of BA
|1.76
|1.87
|----
|High-IQ occupation
|1.39
|1.72
|----
|Out of labor force 1+ month
| -0.34
| -0.3
|----
|Unemployed 1+ month
| -0.52
| -0.47
|----
|}


In 1972, ] questioned Herrnstein's idea that society was developing towards a ]. Chomsky criticized the assumptions that people only seek occupations based on material gain. He argued that Herrnstein would not want to become a baker or lumberjack even if he could earn more money that way. He also criticized the assumption that such a society would be fair with pay based on value of contributions. He argued that because there are already substantial inequalities, people will often be paid at levels that preserve such inequalities rather than commensurately with their contribution to society.<ref name=Chomsky>Chomsky, Noam. 1972. " {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110511220049/http://newlearningonline.com/new-learning/chapter-6-the-nature-of-learning/chomsky-on-iq-and-inequality/ |date=2011-05-11 }}." ''Rampart'': 24–30. pp. 26–28, 30.</ref>
{| class="wikitable"
|-
|+ '''Relation between IQ and life outcomes in the U.S. among sibling pairs in a "Utopian" sample'''
! IQ || <75 || 75&ndash;90 || 90&ndash;110 || 110&ndash;125 || >125
|----
|Mean years of education
|11.4 (10.9)
|12.3 (11.9)
|13.4 (13.2)
|15.2 (15.0)
|16.5 (16.5)
|----
|Percentage obtaining B.A.
|1 (1)
|4 (3)
|19 (16)
|57 (50)
|80 (77)
|----
|Mean weeks worked
|35.8 (30.7)
|39.0 (36.5)
|43.0 (41.8)
|45.1 (45.2)
|45.6 (45.4)
|----
|Mean earned income
|11,000 (7,500)
|16,000 (13,000)
|23,000 (21,000)
|27,000 (27,000)
|38,000 (36,000)
|----
|Percentage with a spouse who has earned income
|30 (27)
|38 (39)
|53 (54)
|61 (59)
|58 (58)
|----
|Mean earned family income
|17,000 (12,000)
|25,000 (23,400)
|37,750 (37,000)
|47,200 (45,000)
|53,700 (53,000)
|----
|Percentage children born out of wedlock
|49 (50)
|33 (32)
|14 (14)
|6 (6)
|3 (5)
|----
|Fertility to date
|2.1 (2.3)
|1.7 (1.9)
|1.4 (1.6)
|1.3 (1.4)
|1.0 (1.0)
|----
|Mother's mean age at birth
|24.4 (22.8)
|24.5 (23.7)
|26.0 (25.2)
|27.4 (27.1)
|29.0 (28.5)
|----
| colspan="6" | Values are "Utopian sample" ("Full sample"). Earning values are the 1993 US Dollars.<ref name=murray98/>
|----
|}


== Notes == ===Race and intelligence===
{{see also|History of the race and intelligence controversy}}


One part of the controversy concerned the parts of the book which dealt with racial group differences on IQ and the consequences of this. In chapter 13, the authors state: "It seems highly likely to us that both genes and the environment have something to do with racial differences." The introduction to the chapter, however, provides the caveat that "The debate about whether and how much genes and environment have to do with ethnic differences remains unresolved".
<!--See http://en.wikipedia.org/Wikipedia:Footnotes for an explanation of how to generate footnotes using the <ref(erences/)> tags-->
{{reflist|2}}


In an article praising the book, economist ] criticized some of its aspects, including some of its arguments about race and the malleability of IQ:
== References ==


{{blockquote|When European immigrant groups in the United States scored below the national average on mental tests, they scored lowest on the abstract parts of those tests. So did white mountaineer children in the United States tested back in the early 1930s ... Strangely, Herrnstein and Murray refer to "folklore" that "Jews and other immigrant groups were thought to be below average in intelligence." It was neither folklore nor anything as subjective as thoughts. It was based on hard data, as hard as any data in ''The Bell Curve''. These groups repeatedly tested below average on the mental tests of the World War I era, both in the army and in civilian life. For Jews, it is clear that later tests showed radically different results—during an era when there was very little intermarriage to change the genetic makeup of American Jews.<ref>{{cite journal |url=http://search.opinionarchives.com/Summary/AmericanSpectator/V28I2P32-1.htm |title=Ethnicity and IQ |first=Thomas |last=Sowell |date=1995 |volume=28 |issue=2 |journal=]}}</ref>}}
* Montagu, Ashley, editor. Race and IQ. Oxford University Press, 1999.
* Russell Jacoby and Naomi Glauberman, editors, ''The Bell Curve Debate: History, Documents, Opinions'', Random House/Times Books (1995) 81 articles by 81 academics and journalists from the full spectrum of political views on title topic.
* Claude S. Fischer et al. '']'' Princeton University Press, 1996, ISBN 0-691-02898-2.
* Bernie Devlin et al. ''Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to The Bell Curve.'' Copernicus Books, 1997, ISBN 0-387-94986-0.
*Weiss, V. (1995). ''The emergence of a cognitive elite. Comment on 'The Bell Curve' by Herrnstein and Murray.'' Mankind Quarterly 35, 373-390.


In 1996, ] released a revised and expanded edition of his 1981 book '']'', intended to more directly refute many of ''The Bell Curve'''s claims regarding ], and argued that the evidence for ] did not indicate a genetic origin to group differences in intelligence.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=J. R. |date=1999 |title=Evidence against Rushton: The Genetic Loading of the Wisc-R Subtests and the Causes of Between-Group IQ Differences |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=26 |issue=2 |pages=373–393 |doi=10.1016/s0191-8869(98)00149-4}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Gould |first1=Stephen Jay |title=The mismeasure of man |date=1996 |publisher=Norton |location=New York |isbn=978-0-393-31425-0 |edition=Rev. and expanded}}</ref>
== External links ==
* . By political scientist Charles Murray.
*
*


Psychologist ] has suggested that the ] test used in the analyses of ''The Bell Curve'' correlates highly with measures of literacy, and argues that the ASVAB test in fact is not a measure of general intelligence but of literacy.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Marks |first=D. F. |year=2010 |title=IQ variations across time, race, and nationality: an artifact of differences in literacy skills |journal=Psychological Reports |volume=106 |issue=3 |pages=643–664 |doi=10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664 |pmid=20712152 |s2cid=12179547}}</ref><ref name="psychologytoday.com">{{Cite web |url=http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/beautiful-minds/201008/the-flynn-effect-and-iq-disparities-among-races-ethnicities-and-nations- |title=The Flynn Effect and IQ Disparities Among Races, Ethnicities, and Nations: Are There Common Links? |first=Scott Barry |last=Kaufman |date=August 23, 2010 |work=Psychology Today |access-date=May 4, 2020}}</ref>
=== Responses to and critiques of ''The Bell Curve'' ===

*
], professor of ] and associate professor of ] and ] at ], called ''Bell Curve'' a "deliberate assault on efforts to improve the school performance of African-Americans":
*

* ] report in response to ''The Bell Curve'']
{{blockquote|This book presented strong evidence that genes play a role in intelligence but linked it to the unsupported claim that genes explain the small but consistent black-white difference in IQ. The juxtaposition of good argument with a bad one seemed politically motivated, and persuasive refutations soon appeared. Actually, African-Americans have excelled in virtually every enriched environment they have been placed in, most of which they were previously barred from, and this in only the first decade or two of improved but still not equal opportunity. It is likely that the real curves for the two races will one day be superimposable on each other, but this may require decades of change and different environments for different people. Claims about genetic potential are meaningless except in light of this requirement.<ref>{{cite book |last=Konnor |first=Melvin |year=2003 |title=The Tangled Wing: Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit |edition=2nd |publisher=Henry Holt and Company |page=428}}</ref>}}
* ];388(6641):468-71]

*
The 2014 textbook ''Evolutionary Analysis'' by Herron and Freeman<ref>{{Cite book |title=Evolutionary Analysis |last=Herron and Freeman |first=Jon and Scott |publisher=Pearson |year=2014 |location=Boston |pages=360–363}}</ref> devoted an entire chapter to debunking what they termed the "Bell Curve fallacy", saying that "Murray and Herrnstein's argument amounts to little more than an appeal to
* The ], ], ], p. A18]
personal incredulity" and that it is a mistake to think that heritability can tell us something about the causes of differences between population means. In reference to the comparison of African-American with European-American IQ scores, the text states that only a common garden experiment, in which the two groups are raised in an environment typically experienced by European-Americans, would allow one to see if the difference is genetic. This kind of experiment, routine with plants and animals, cannot be conducted with humans. Nor is it possible to approximate this design with adoptions into families of the different groups, because the children would be recognizable and possibly be treated differently. The text concludes: "There is no way to assess whether genetics has anything to do with the difference in IQ score between ethnic groups."
* An article by Alan Reifman, which reviews pertinent research on the genetic contribution to intelligence, the relative contributions of intelligence and social factors to success in life, and the potential of educational experience to improve cognitive ability.

* by ] in ] Magazine, ], ]
Rutledge M. Dennis suggests that through ]s of works like ]'s famous study on the achievement gap, and Herrnstein and Murray's book ''The Bell Curve'', the media "paints a picture of Blacks and other people of color as collective biological illiterates—as not only intellectually unfit but evil and criminal as well", thus providing, he says "the logic and justification for those who would further disenfranchise and exclude racial and ethnic minorities".<ref>{{cite journal |last=Dennis |first=Rutledge M. |url=http://www33.homepage.villanova.edu/edward.fierros/pdf/Dennis,%20Social%20Darwinism.pdf |title=Social Darwinism, scientific racism, and the metaphysics of race |journal=Journal of Negro Education |date=Summer 1995 |volume=64 |issue=3 |pages=243–252 |doi=10.2307/2967206 |jstor=2967206}}</ref>
* Fairness and Accuracy in Reporting,

*
] pointed out that 17 of the researchers whose work is referenced by the book have also contributed to '']'', a journal of anthropology founded in 1960 in Edinburgh, which has been viewed as supporting the theory of the genetic superiority of white people.<ref>{{Cite news |url=http://www.nybooks.com/articles/archives/1994/dec/01/the-tainted-sources-of-the-bell-curve/ |title=The Tainted Sources of 'The Bell Curve' |last=Lane |first=Charles |date=1994-12-01 |newspaper=The New York Review of Books |issn=0028-7504 |access-date=2016-09-10}}</ref> David Bartholomew reports Murray's response as part of the controversy over the Bell Curve.<ref>{{cite book |last=Bartholomew |first=David J. |title=Measuring Intelligence: Facts and Fallacies |url=https://archive.org/details/measuringintelli00bart |url-access=limited |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge |year=2004 |page= |isbn=978-0-521-54478-8}}</ref> In his afterword to the 1996 Free Press edition of ''The Bell Curve'', Murray responded that the book "draws its evidence from more than a thousand scholars" and among the researchers mentioned in Lane's list "are some of the most respected psychologists of our time and that almost all of the sources referred to as tainted are articles published in leading refereed journals".<ref name="Curve">{{cite book |last1=Herrnstein |first1=Richard J. |last2=Murray |first2=Charles |title=The Bell Curve |publisher=Free Press |location=New York |year=1996 |page=564}}</ref>
*

* Nobel Prize-winner ], "", ''Journal of Political Economy'', October 1995.
''The Bell Curve Wars: Race, Intelligence, and the Future of America'' is a collection of articles published in reaction to the book. Edited by Steven Fraser, the writers of these essays do not have a specific viewpoint concerning the content of ''The Bell Curve'', but express their own critiques of various aspects of the book, including the research methods used, the alleged hidden biases in the research and the policies suggested as a result of the conclusions drawn by the authors.<ref>{{cite book |title=The Bell Curve Wars: race, intelligence, and the future of America |date=1995 |publisher=Basic Books |location=New York |isbn=978-0-465-00693-9 |url=https://archive.org/details/bellcurvewarsrac00fras/page/ |editor=Steven Fraser}}{{page needed|date=September 2019}}</ref> Fraser writes that "by scrutinizing the footnotes and bibliography in ''The Bell Curve'', readers can more easily recognize the project for what it is: a chilly synthesis of the work of disreputable race theorists and eccentric ]".<ref>{{cite book |editor-last=Fraser |editor-first=Steven |title=The Bell Curve Wars: Race, Intelligence, and the Future of America |date=1995 |publisher=Basic Books |isbn=978-0-465-00693-9 |oclc=782205959 |url=https://archive.org/details/bellcurvewarsrac00fras |access-date= |url-access= |via=}}</ref>
*

==== Allegations of racism ====
Since the book provided statistical data making the assertion that blacks were, on average, less intelligent than whites, some people have argued that ''The Bell Curve'' could be used by extremists to justify genocide and hate crimes.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.talkreason.org/articles/coultergeist.cfm |title=Ann Coulter and Charles Darwin. Coultergeist |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20090430200549/http://www.talkreason.org/articles/coultergeist.cfm |archive-date=2009-04-30 |url-status=live |first=Jerry |last=Coyne |date=August 2, 2006 |work=TalkReason |access-date=May 4, 2020}}</ref><!-- Student essay, not a reliable source: <ref>{{cite web |url=http://serendip.brynmawr.edu/biology/b103/f01/web3/bishay.html |title=The Bell Curve: An illustration of the existence of social science as a social problem |url=https://serendipstudio.org/biology/b103/f01/web3/bishay.html |year=2001 |first=Vivian |last=Bishay |publisher=Serendip Studio |access-date=May 4, 2020}}</ref>--> Much of the work referenced by ''The Bell Curve'' was funded by the ], which aims to advance the scientific study of heredity and human differences, and which has been accused of promoting ] views, particularly ].<ref>{{cite web |url=https://fair.org/home/racism-resurgent/ |title=Racism Resurgent |work=FAIR |date=January 1, 1995 |first=Jim |last=Naureckas |access-date=May 4, 2020}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.hartford-hwp.com/archives/45/049.html |title=The Bell Curve and the Pioneer Fund |format=transcript from ABC World News Tonight |date=November 22, 1994 |via=Hartford Web Publishing |access-date=May 4, 2020}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |title=Moral Courage: Is defending The Bell Curve an example of intellectual honesty? |first=Stephen |last=Metcalf |date=October 17, 2005 |work=Slate |url=http://www.slate.com/id/2128199/ |access-date=May 4, 2020}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Rosenthal |first=Steven J. |date=1995 |title=The Pioneer Fund: Financier of Fascist Research |url=http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0002764295039001006 |journal=American Behavioral Scientist |language=en |volume=39 |issue=1 |pages=44–61 |doi=10.1177/0002764295039001006 |issn=0002-7642}}</ref> Murray criticized the characterization of the Pioneer Fund as a racist organization, arguing that it has as much relationship to its founder as "] and today's ]".<ref>Herrnstein & Murray (1994) p. 564.</ref>

Evolutionary biologist ] described ''The Bell Curve'' as an example of racist science, containing all the types of errors in the application of scientific method that have characterized the history of ]:
# Claims that are not supported by the data given
# Errors in calculation that invariably support the hypothesis
# No mention of data that contradict the hypothesis
# No mention of theories and data that conflict with core assumptions
# Bold policy recommendations that are consistent with those advocated by racists.<ref>{{cite book |last=Graves |first=Joseph L. |year=2001 |title=The Emperor's New Clothes |url=https://archive.org/details/emperorsnewcloth00grav |url-access=registration |publisher=Rutgers University Press |page= |isbn=978-0-8135-2847-2}}</ref>

Eric Siegel wrote on the ''Scientific American'' blog that the book "endorses prejudice by virtue of what it does not say. Nowhere does the book address why it investigates racial differences in IQ. By never spelling out a reason for reporting on these differences in the first place, the authors transmit an unspoken yet unequivocal conclusion: Race is a helpful indicator as to whether a person is likely to hold certain capabilities. Even if we assume the presented data trends are sound, the book leaves the reader on his or her own to deduce how to best put these insights to use. The net effect is to tacitly condone the prejudgment of individuals based on race."<ref>{{cite web |url=https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/guest-blog/the-real-problem-with-charles-murray-and-the-bell-curve/ |title=The Real Problem with Charles Murray and 'The Bell Curve' |first=Eric |last=Siegel |date=April 12, 2017 |work=Scientific American |access-date=May 4, 2020}}</ref> Similarly, Howard Gardner accused the authors of engaging in "scholarly ]", arguing that "Whether concerning an issue of science, policy, or rhetoric, the authors come dangerously close to embracing the most extreme positions, yet in the end shy away from doing so&nbsp;... Scholarly brinkmanship encourages the reader to draw the strongest conclusions, while allowing the authors to disavow this intention."<ref>{{Cite magazine |last=Gardner |first=Howard |author-link=Howard Gardner |date=Winter 1995 |title=Cracking Open the IQ Box |url=http://prospect.org/article/cracking-open-iq-box |magazine=] |language=en}}</ref>

Columnist ], writing for '']'', described the book as "a scabrous piece of racial pornography masquerading as serious scholarship". "Mr. Murray can protest all he wants", wrote Herbert; "his book is just a genteel way of calling somebody a ]."<ref name="nyt-herbert">{{cite news |last=Herbert |first=Bob |author-link=Bob Herbert |date=1994-10-26 |title=In America; Throwing a Curve |newspaper=] |url=https://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res=9F01E5DE123FF935A15753C1A962958260 |access-date=2007-01-09}}</ref>

==See also==
* '']'' (2020) by Charles Murray

==References==
{{Reflist|30em}}

==Sources==
* {{citation |last=Alderfer |first=C.P. |title=The science and nonscience of psychologists' responses to The Bell Curve |journal=] |volume=34 |issue=3 |pages=287–293 |year=2003 |doi=10.1037/0735-7028.34.3.287 |author-link=Clayton Alderfer}}
* {{citation |last=Armour-Thomas |first=Eleanor |title=Assessment of psychometric intelligence for racial and ethnic minorities |pages=357–374 |year=2003 |series=Handbook of racial and ethnic minority psychology (ed. Guillermo Bernal) |publisher=SAGE |isbn=978-0-7619-1965-0}}
* {{citation |last=Campbell |first=Donald T. |title=Unresolved Issues in Measurement Validity : An Autobiographical Overview |journal=Psychological Assessment |volume=8 |issue=4 |pages=363–368 |year=1996 |doi=10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.363 |author-link=Donald T. Campbell}}
* Devlin, Bernie, et al. (1997). ''Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to The Bell Curve.'' Copernicus Books, {{ISBN|0-387-94986-0}}.
* Fischer, Claude S., et al. (1996). '']'' ], {{ISBN|0-691-02898-2}}.
* {{citation |last1=Graves |first1=Joseph L. |title=The Pseudoscience of Psychometry and The Bell Curve |date=1995 |journal=] |volume=64 |issue=3 |pages=277–294 |doi=10.2307/2967209 |jstor=2967209 |last2=Johnson |first2=Amanda |author-link=Joseph L. Graves}}
* {{citation |last=Harrington |first=Gordon M. |title=Psychological testing, IQ, and evolutionary fitness |journal=Genetica |volume=99 |issue=2–3 |pages=113–123 |year=1997 |doi=10.1007/bf02259515 |pmid=9463067 |s2cid=9866526}}
* {{cite book |ref=refRussell1995 |editor1-last=Jacoby |editor1-first=Russell |editor2-last=Glauberman |editor2-first=Naomi |title=The Bell Curve Debate: History, Documents, Opinions – 81 articles by 81 academics and journalists from the full spectrum of political views on title topic. |publisher=]/] |date=1995 |pages= |isbn=978-0-8129-2587-6 |url=https://archive.org/details/bellcurvedebateh00jaco |url-access=registration}}
* {{citation |last=Laosa |first=Luis M. |title=Intelligence testing and social policy |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1002/j.2333-8504.1995.tb01667.x |journal=Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology |volume=17 |issue=2 |pages=155–173 |year=1996 |doi=10.1016/S0193-3973(96)90023-4}}
* {{cite book |ref=refMontagu1999 |editor1-last=Montagu |editor1-first=Ashley |title=Race and IQ |publisher=] |date=1999 |pages= |isbn=978-0-19-510220-8 |url=https://archive.org/details/raceiq00ashl |url-access=registration}}

==Further reading==
* {{cite journal |vauthors=Devlin B, Daniels M, ] |title=The heritability of IQ |journal=Nature |volume=388 |issue=6641 |pages=468–471 |date=July 1997 |pmid=9242404 |doi=10.1038/41319 |bibcode=1997Natur.388..468D |s2cid=4313884 |doi-access=free}}
* {{cite journal |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346 |vauthors=Dickens WT, Flynn JR |title=Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: the IQ paradox resolved |journal=Psychol Rev |volume=108 |issue=2 |pages=346–369 |date=April 2001 |pmid=11381833 |url=http://content.apa.org/journals/rev/108/2/346 |citeseerx=10.1.1.139.2436}}
* {{cite encyclopedia |last=Doherty |first=Brian |author-link=Brian Doherty (journalist) |editor-first=Ronald |editor-last=Hamowy |editor-link=Ronald Hamowy |encyclopedia=The Encyclopedia of Libertarianism |title=Murray, Charles (1943–) |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=yxNgXs3TkJYC |date=2008 |publisher=]; ] |location=Thousand Oaks, CA |doi=10.4135/9781412965811.n211 |isbn=978-1-4129-6580-4 |oclc=750831024 |lccn=2008009151 |pages=344–345}}
* {{Cite book |title=Race: The Reality of Human Difference |last=Sarich |first=Vincent M. |author-link=Vincent Sarich |publisher=] |date=2004 |isbn=978-0-8133-4086-9 |title-link=Race: The Reality of Human Difference}}
* {{cite book |last=Tucker |first=William H. |title=The funding of scientific racism: Wickliffe Draper and the Pioneer Fund |author-link=William H. Tucker (psychologist) |publisher=] |date=2007 |isbn=978-0-252-07463-9 |orig-date=first published 2002}}
**{{cite journal |author=Andrew S. Winston |title=The Funding of Scientific Racism: Wickliffe Draper and the Pioneer Fund (review) |journal=Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences |volume=58 |issue=3 |pages=391–392 |date=July 2003 |doi=10.1093/jhmas/jrg016 |url=http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/journal_of_the_history_of_medicine_and_allied_sciences/summary/v058/58.3winston.html}}

==External links==
* read by Murray, (Internet Archive).

===Interviews with Murray===
* , from ]'s '']'', aired December 4, 1994.
* '']'' interview with Charles Murray, 1995 - ,
* {{cite web |last=Miele |first=Frank |author-link=Frank Miele |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20050205010706/http://www.skeptic.com/archives24.html |url=https://www.skeptic.com/archives24.html |date=1995 |archive-date=February 5, 2005 |title=Interview with Charles Murray (1995) |work=Skeptic Magazine}}
* , ] blog, October 16, 2014
* ] (April 2017). - interview of Murray.

===Responses to ''The Bell Curve''===
* {{cite journal |last=Rothbard |first=Murray N. |title=Race! That Murray Book |journal=The Rothbard-Rockwell Report |author-link=Murray Rothbard |date=December 1994 |pages=1–9}}
* ], (January/February 1995)
* {{cite web |last1=Heckman |first1=James |author1-link=James Heckman |title=Cracked Bell |url=https://reason.com/1995/03/01/cracked-bell/ |website=Reason.com |access-date=September 20, 2021 |date=March 1, 1995}} - critique of Murray and Herrnstein's statistical techniques
* Heckman, James "", ''Journal of Political Economy'', October 1995.
* Reifman, Alan (2000). , '']'' 11, #99
* Siegel, Eric (2017). , ''Scientific American blog'', April 2017.
* Krenz, Claudia -

{{Authority control}}


{{DEFAULTSORT:Bell Curve, The}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Bell Curve, The}}
] ]
] ]
] ]
] ]
]
]
]
] ]
] ]
]

]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 05:00, 3 January 2025

1994 book by Richard J. Herrnstein and Charles Murray For the principal, mathematical meaning of the term "bell curve", see Normal distribution. For other senses of the term, see Bell curve (disambiguation).
The Bell Curve
Cover of the first edition
AuthorsRichard J. Herrnstein
Charles Murray
SubjectIntelligence
Social class
PublisherFree Press
Publication date1994
Media typePrint (hardcover and paperback)
Pages845
ISBN0-02-914673-9
OCLC30913157
Dewey Decimal305.9/082 20
LC ClassBF431 .H398 1994

The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life is a 1994 book by the psychologist Richard J. Herrnstein and the political scientist Charles Murray in which the authors argue that human intelligence is substantially influenced by both inherited and environmental factors and that it is a better predictor of many personal outcomes, including financial income, job performance, birth out of wedlock, and involvement in crime than are an individual's parental socioeconomic status. They also argue that those with high intelligence, the "cognitive elite", are becoming separated from those of average and below-average intelligence, and that this separation is a source of social division within the United States.

The book has been, and remains, highly controversial, especially where the authors discussed purported connections between race and intelligence and suggested policy implications based on these purported connections. The authors claimed that average intelligence quotient (IQ) differences between racial and ethnic groups are at least partly genetic in origin, a view that is now considered discredited by mainstream science. Many of the references and sources used in the book were advocates for racial hygiene, whose research was funded by the white supremacist organization Pioneer Fund.

Shortly after its publication, many people rallied both in criticism and in defense of the book. A number of critical texts were written in response to it. Several criticisms were collected in the book The Bell Curve Debate.

Synopsis

This section relies largely or entirely on a single source. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please help improve this article by introducing citations to additional sources.
Find sources: "The Bell Curve" – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (December 2019)

The Bell Curve, published in 1994, was written by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray to explain the variations in intelligence in American society, warn of some consequences of that variation, and propose social policies for mitigating the worst of the consequences. The book's title comes from the bell-shaped normal distribution of intelligence quotient (IQ) scores in a population.

Introduction

The book starts with an introduction that appraises the history of the concept of intelligence from Francis Galton to modern times. Spearman's introduction of the general factor of intelligence and other early advances in research on intelligence are discussed along with a consideration of links between intelligence testing and racial politics. The 1960s are identified as the period in American history when social problems were increasingly attributed to forces outside the individual. This egalitarian ethos, Herrnstein and Murray argue, cannot accommodate biologically based individual differences.

The introduction states six of the authors' assumptions, which they claim to be "beyond significant technical dispute":

  1. There is such a difference as a general factor of cognitive ability on which human beings differ.
  2. All standardized tests of academic aptitude or achievement measure this general factor to some degree, but IQ tests expressly designed for that purpose measure it most accurately.
  3. IQ scores match, to a first degree, whatever it is that people mean when they use the word intelligent, or smart in ordinary language.
  4. IQ scores are stable, although not perfectly so, over much of a person's life.
  5. Properly administered IQ tests are not demonstrably biased against social, economic, ethnic, or racial groups.
  6. Cognitive ability is substantially heritable, apparently no less than 40 percent and no more than 80 percent.

At the close of the introduction, the authors warn the reader against committing the ecological fallacy of inferring things about individuals based on the aggregate data presented in the book. They also assert that intelligence is just one of many valuable human attributes and one whose importance among human virtues is overrated.

Part I. The Emergence of a Cognitive Elite

In the first part of the book Herrnstein and Murray chart how American society was transformed in the 20th century. They argue that America evolved from a society where social origin largely determined one's social status to one where cognitive ability is the leading determinant of status. The growth in college attendance, a more efficient recruitment of cognitive ability, and the sorting of cognitive ability by selective colleges are identified as important drivers of this evolution. Herrnstein and Murray propose that the cognitive elite has been produced by a more technological society which offers enough high skill jobs for those with a higher intelligence to fill. They also propose that by removing race, gender or class as criteria (via the establishment of free primary education and the prohibition of discrimination) the main criteria of success in academic and professional life is becoming primarily based on cognitive ability. Increased occupational sorting by cognitive ability is discussed. They maintain that cognitive ability is the best predictor of worker productivity.

Herrnstein and Murray argue that due to increasing returns to cognitive ability, a cognitive elite is being formed in America. They argue that this elite is getting richer and progressively more segregated from the rest of society.

Part II. Cognitive Classes and Social Behavior

The second part describes how cognitive ability is related to social behaviors: high ability predicts socially desirable behavior, low ability undesirable behavior. The argument is made that group differences in social outcomes are better explained by intelligence differences rather than socioeconomic status, a perspective, the authors argue, that has been neglected in research.

The analyses reported in this part of the book were done using data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Labor Market Experience of Youth (NLSY), a study conducted by the United States Department of Labor's Bureau of Labor Statistics tracking thousands of Americans starting in the 1980s. Only non-Hispanic whites are included in the analyses so as to demonstrate that the relationships between cognitive ability and social behavior are not driven by race or ethnicity.

Herrnstein and Murray argue that intelligence is a better predictor of individuals' outcomes than parental socioeconomic status. This argument is based on analyses where individuals' IQ scores are shown to better predict their outcomes as adults than the socioeconomic status of their parents. Such results are reported for many outcomes, including poverty, dropping out of school, unemployment, marriage, divorce, illegitimacy, welfare dependency, criminal offending, and the probability of voting in elections.

All participants in the NLSY took the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery (ASVAB), a battery of ten tests taken by all who apply for entry into the armed services. (Some had taken an IQ test in high school, and the median correlation of the Armed Forces Qualification Test (AFQT) scores and those IQ test scores was .81). Participants were later evaluated for social and economic outcomes. In general, Herrnstein and Murray argued, IQ/AFQT scores were a better predictor of life outcomes than social class background. Similarly, after statistically controlling for differences in IQ, they argued that many outcome differences between racial-ethnic groups disappeared.

Economic and social correlates of IQ
IQ <75 75–90 90–110 110–125 >125
US population distribution 5 20 50 20 5
Married by age 30 72 81 81 72 67
Out of labor force more than 1 month out of year (men) 22 19 15 14 10
Unemployed more than 1 month out of year (men) 12 10 7 7 2
Divorced in 5 years 21 22 23 15 9
% of children w/ IQ in bottom decile (mothers) 39 17 6 7
Had an illegitimate baby (mothers) 32 17 8 4 2
Lives in poverty 30 16 6 3 2
Ever incarcerated (men) 7 7 3 1 0
Chronic welfare recipient (mothers) 31 17 8 2 0
High school dropout 55 35 6 0.4 0
Scored "Yes" on "Middle Class Values Index" 16 30 50 67 74

Values are the percentage of each IQ sub-population, among non-Hispanic whites only, fitting each descriptor.

  1. According to Herrnstein & Murray the "Middle Class Values Index" was intended "to identify among the NLSY population, in their young adulthood when the index was scored, those people who are getting along with their lives in ways that fit the middle-class stereotype." To score "Yes" on the index, a NLSY subject had to meet all four of the following criteria:
    • Received at least a high-school diploma
    • Never interviewed while incarcerated
    • Still married to one's first spouse
    • Men only: In the labor force, even if not employed
    • Women only: Never gave birth outside of marriage
    Excluded from the analysis were never-married individuals who satisfied all other components of the index, and men who were not in the labor force in 1989 or 1990 due to disability or still being in school.

Part III. The National Context

This part of the book discusses ethnic differences in cognitive ability and social behavior. Herrnstein and Murray report that Asian Americans have a higher mean IQ than white Americans, who in turn outscore black Americans. The book argues that the black-white gap is not due to test bias, noting that IQ tests do not tend to underpredict the school or job performance of black individuals and that the gap is larger on apparently culturally neutral test items than on more culturally loaded items. The authors also note that adjusting for socioeconomic status does not eliminate the black-white IQ gap. However, they argue that the gap is narrowing.

According to Herrnstein and Murray, the high heritability of IQ within races does not necessarily mean that the cause of differences between races is genetic. On the other hand, they discuss lines of evidence that have been used to support the thesis that the black-white gap is at least partly genetic, such as Spearman's hypothesis. They also discuss possible environmental explanations of the gap, such as the observed generational increases in IQ, for which they coin the term Flynn effect. At the close of this discussion, they write:

If the reader is now convinced that either the genetic or environmental explanation has won out to the exclusion of the other, we have not done a sufficiently good job of presenting one side or the other. It seems highly likely to us that both genes and environment have something to do with racial differences. What might the mix be? We are resolutely agnostic on that issue; as far as we can determine, the evidence does not yet justify an estimate.

The authors also stress that regardless of the causes of differences, people should be treated no differently.

In Part III, the authors also repeat many of the analyses from Part II, but now compare whites to blacks and Hispanics in the NLSY dataset. They find that after controlling for IQ, many differences in social outcomes between races are diminished.

The authors discuss the possibility that high birth rates among those with lower IQs may exert a downward pressure on the national distribution of cognitive ability. They argue that immigration may also have a similar effect.

At the close of Part III, Herrnstein and Murray discuss the relation of IQ to social problems. Using the NLSY data, they argue that social problems are a monotonically decreasing function of IQ, in other words at lower IQ scores the frequency of social problems increases.

Living Together

In this final chapter, the authors discuss the relevance of cognitive ability for understanding major social issues in America.

Evidence for experimental attempts to raise intelligence is reviewed. The authors conclude that currently there are no means to boost intelligence by more than a modest degree.

The authors criticize the "levelling" of general and secondary education and defend gifted education. They offer a critical overview of affirmative action policies in colleges and workplaces, arguing that their goal should be equality of opportunity rather than equal outcomes.

Herrnstein and Murray offer a pessimistic portrait of America's future. They predict that a cognitive elite will further isolate itself from the rest of society, while the quality of life deteriorates for those at the bottom of the cognitive scale. As an antidote to this prognosis, they offer a vision of society where differences in ability are recognized and everybody can have a valued place, stressing the role of local communities and clear moral rules that apply to everybody.

Policy recommendations

This section relies excessively on references to primary sources. Please improve this section by adding secondary or tertiary sources.
Find sources: "The Bell Curve" – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (December 2019) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

Herrnstein and Murray argued the average genetic IQ of the United States is declining, owing to the tendency of the more intelligent having fewer children than the less intelligent, the generation length to be shorter for the less intelligent, and the large-scale immigration to the United States of those with low intelligence. Discussing a possible future political outcome of an intellectually stratified society, the authors stated that they "fear that a new kind of conservatism is becoming the dominant ideology of the affluent—not in the social tradition of an Edmund Burke or in the economic tradition of an Adam Smith but 'conservatism' along Latin American lines, where to be conservative has often meant doing whatever is necessary to preserve the mansions on the hills from the menace of the slums below." Moreover, they fear that increasing welfare will create a "custodial state" in "a high-tech and more lavish version of the Indian reservation for some substantial minority of the nation's population." They also predict increasing totalitarianism: "It is difficult to imagine the United States preserving its heritage of individualism, equal rights before the law, free people running their own lives, once it is accepted that a significant part of the population must be made permanent wards of the states."

The authors recommended the elimination of welfare policies which they claim encourage poor women to have babies.

Reception

This section may be too long to read and navigate comfortably. Consider splitting content into sub-articles, condensing it, or adding subheadings. Please discuss this issue on the article's talk page. (September 2024)

The Bell Curve received a great deal of media attention. The book was not distributed in advance to the media, except for a few select reviewers picked by Murray and the publisher, which delayed more detailed critiques for months and years after the book's release. Stephen Jay Gould, reviewing the book in The New Yorker, said that the book "contains no new arguments and presents no compelling data to support its anachronistic social Darwinism" and said that the "authors omit facts, misuse statistical methods, and seem unwilling to admit the consequence of their own words."

A 1995 article by Fairness and Accuracy in Reporting writer Jim Naureckas criticized the media response, saying that "While many of these discussions included sharp criticisms of the book, media accounts showed a disturbing tendency to accept Murray and Herrnstein's premises and evidence even while debating their conclusions". A 1995 article by the Brookings Institution argued that critics had narrowly focused their attention on the book's arguments regarding race and intelligence while ignoring other contents of the book, but added that "the book may have fared even worse had the discussion of race and genetics not distracted attention from some serious problems of analysis and logic in its main arguments. There are indeed some useful messages in the book. But there is also much wrong with it."

After reviewers had more time to review the book's research and conclusions, more significant criticisms began to appear. Nicholas Lemann, writing in Slate, said that later reviews showed the book was "full of mistakes ranging from sloppy reasoning to mis-citations of sources to outright mathematical errors." Lemann said that "Unsurprisingly, all the mistakes are in the direction of supporting the authors' thesis."

Many criticisms were collected in the book The Bell Curve Debate.

Lack of peer review

Herrnstein and Murray did not submit their work to peer review before publication, an omission many have seen as incompatible with their presentation of it as a scholarly text. Nicholas Lemann noted that the book was not circulated in galley proofs, a common practice to allow potential reviewers and media professionals an opportunity to prepare for the book's arrival.

"Mainstream Science on Intelligence" statement

Main article: Mainstream Science on Intelligence

An opinion statement endorsing a number of the views presented in The Bell Curve called "Mainstream Science on Intelligence" was published in The Wall Street Journal in 1994 and subsequently reprinted in the journal Intelligence. The statement was drafted by Linda Gottfredson, a professor of educational psychology at the University of Delaware. It was sent to 131 researchers whom Gottfredsen described as "experts in intelligence and allied fields". Of these, 52 signed the statement, 48 returned the request with an explicit refusal to sign, and 31 ignored the request.

According to a 1996 response by former American Psychological Association president Donald Campbell, only ten of those who signed were actual experts in intelligence measurement. The Southern Poverty Law Center reports that 20 of the signers were recipients of funding from the white-supremacist organization the Pioneer Fund, including Gottfredson herself.

In subsequent years, both the substance and the interpretation of this letter have received widespread criticism from the scientific community.

APA task force report

Main article: Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns
This section relies excessively on references to primary sources. Please improve this section by adding secondary or tertiary sources.
Find sources: "The Bell Curve" – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (December 2019) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

In response to the controversy surrounding The Bell Curve, the American Psychological Association's Board of Scientific Affairs established a special task force chaired by Ulric Neisser to publish an investigative report focusing solely on the research presented in the book, not the policy recommendations that it made. The report, "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns", was first released in 1995 and published in American Psychologist in 1996.

Regarding explanations for racial differences, the APA task force stated:

The cause of that differential is not known; it is apparently not due to any simple form of bias in the content or administration of the tests themselves. The Flynn effect shows that environmental factors can produce differences of at least this magnitude, but that effect is mysterious in its own right. Several culturally based explanations of the Black/White IQ differential have been proposed; some are plausible, but so far none has been conclusively supported. There is even less empirical support for a genetic interpretation. In short, no adequate explanation of the differential between the IQ means of Blacks and Whites is presently available.

American Psychologist subsequently published eleven critical responses in January 1997.

Criticism of assumptions

Criticism by Stephen Jay Gould

Stephen Jay Gould wrote that the "entire argument" of the authors of The Bell Curve rests on four unsupported, and mostly false, assumptions about intelligence:

  1. Intelligence must be reducible to a single number.
  2. Intelligence must be capable of rank ordering people in a linear order.
  3. Intelligence must be primarily genetically based.
  4. Intelligence must be essentially immutable.

In a 1995 interview with Frank Miele of Skeptic, Murray denied making each of these four assumptions.

Criticism by James Heckman

The Nobel Memorial Prize-winning economist James Heckman considers two assumptions made in the book to be questionable: that g accounts for correlation across test scores and performance in society, and that g cannot be manipulated. Heckman's reanalysis of the evidence used in The Bell Curve found contradictions:

  1. The factors that explain wages receive different weights than the factors that explain test scores. More than g is required to explain either.
  2. Other factors besides g contribute to social performance, and they can be manipulated.

In response, Murray argued that this was a straw man and that the book does not argue that g or IQ are totally immutable or the only factors affecting outcomes.

In a 2005 interview, Heckman praised The Bell Curve for breaking "a taboo by showing that differences in ability existed and predicted a variety of socioeconomic outcomes" and for playing "a very important role in raising the issue of differences in ability and their importance" and stated that he was "a bigger fan of than you might think." However, he also maintained that Herrnstein and Murray overestimated the role of heredity in determining intelligence differences.

Criticism by Noam Chomsky

In 1995, Noam Chomsky, a founder in the field of cognitive science, criticized the book and its assumptions on IQ. He takes issue with the idea that IQ is 60% heritable, arguing that the "statement is meaningless" because heritability does not have to be genetic. Chomsky gives the example of women wearing earrings:

To borrow an example from Ned Block, "some years ago when only women wore earrings, the heritability of having an earring was high because differences in whether a person had an earring was due to a chromosomal difference, XX vs. XY." No one has yet suggested that wearing earrings, or ties, is "in our genes," an inescapable fate that environment cannot influence, "dooming the liberal notion."

He goes on to say there is almost no evidence of a genetic link, and greater evidence that environmental issues are what determine IQ differences.

Criticism by Ned Block

See also: Race and intelligence § heritability within and between groups

Philosopher Ned Block argues that The Bell Curve misleads about intelligence as it conflates genetic determination with heritability. Genetic determination and heritability are not interchangeable as there are traits that are genetically determined but not heritable, and vice versa. For example, the number of fingers on a human hand are genetically determined as genes code for five fingers in nearly everybody. However, the heritability of the number of fingers is very low, as variations in numbers of fingers are usually environmentally caused. The aforementioned earring example quoted by Chomsky is an instance where the opposite is true: high heritability, but not genetic determination. Given that genetic determination and heritability are not equivalent, Block contends that IQ is one such trait that is heritable but not genetically determined. Finally, Block contends that utilizing twin studies to randomize the environment automatically fail: Black twins will always bring a part of their environment with them as they are both Black and will be treated as such.

Canadian psychologist Sidney Segalowitz concurs with Block that twin studies fail to draw conclusions about heritability, and as a result Murray's work is methodologically flawed.

Statistical methods

Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Voss in the book Inequality by Design recalculated the effect of socioeconomic status, using the same variables as The Bell Curve, but weighting them differently. They found that if IQ scores are adjusted, as Herrnstein and Murray did, to eliminate the effect of education, the ability of IQ to predict poverty can become dramatically larger, by as much as 61 percent for whites and 74 percent for blacks. According to the authors, Herrnstein and Murray's finding that IQ predicts poverty much better than socioeconomic status is substantially a result of the way they handled the statistics.

In August 1995, National Bureau of Economic Research economist Sanders Korenman and Harvard University sociologist Christopher Winship argued that measurement error was not properly handled by Herrnstein and Murray. Korenman and Winship concluded: "... there is evidence of substantial bias due to measurement error in their estimates of the effects of parents' socioeconomic status. In addition, Herrnstein and Murray's measure of parental socioeconomic status (SES) fails to capture the effects of important elements of family background (such as single-parent family structure at age 14). As a result, their analysis gives an exaggerated impression of the importance of IQ relative to parents' SES, and relative to family background more generally. Estimates based on a variety of methods, including analyses of siblings, suggest that parental family background is at least as important, and may be more important than IQ in determining socioeconomic success in adulthood."

In the book Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to The Bell Curve, a group of social scientists and statisticians analyzes the genetics-intelligence link, the concept of intelligence, the malleability of intelligence and the effects of education, the relationship between cognitive ability, wages and meritocracy, pathways to racial and ethnic inequalities in health, and the question of public policy. This work argues that much of the public response was polemic, and failed to analyze the details of the science and validity of the statistical arguments underlying the book's conclusions.

Use of Armed Forces Qualification Test

William J. Matthews writes that part of The Bell Curve's analysis is based on the Armed Forces Qualification Test (AFQT) "which is not an IQ test but designed to predict performance of certain criterion variables". The AFQT covers subjects such as trigonometry.

Heckman observed that the AFQT was designed only to predict success in military training schools and that most of these tests appear to be achievement tests rather than ability tests, measuring factual knowledge and not pure ability. He continues:

Ironically, the authors delete from their composite AFQT score a timed test of numerical operations because it is not highly correlated with the other tests. Yet it is well known that in the data they use, this subtest is the single best predictor of earnings of all the AFQT test components. The fact that many of the subtests are only weakly correlated with each other, and that the best predictor of earnings is only weakly correlated with their "g-loaded" score, only heightens doubts that a single-ability model is a satisfactory description of human intelligence. It also drives home the point that the "g-loading" so strongly emphasized by Murray and Herrnstein measures only agreement among tests—not predictive power for socioeconomic outcomes. By the same token, one could also argue that the authors have biased their empirical analysis against the conclusions they obtain by disregarding the test with the greatest predictive power.

Janet Currie and Duncan Thomas presented evidence suggesting AFQT scores are likely better markers for family background than "intelligence" in a 1999 study:

Herrnstein and Murray report that conditional on maternal "intelligence" (AFQT scores), child test scores are little affected by variations in socio-economic status. Using the same data, we demonstrate their finding is very fragile.

Cognitive sorting and the "cognitive elite" concept

The cognitive elite concept has been widely criticized. Frank Wilson refuted "as cultural superstition and social science pornography The Bell Curve's theories on the role of intelligence in the social stratification of postindustrial America." Nicholas Lemann described the notion of a cognitive elite as "a sociological cartoon with political uses, not a phenomenon to be accepted at face value."

Charles R. Tittle [de] and Thomas Rotolo found that the more the written, IQ-like, examinations are used as screening devices for occupational access, the stronger the relationship between IQ and income. Thus, rather than higher IQ leading to status attainment because it indicates skills needed in a modern society, IQ may reflect the same test-taking abilities used in artificial screening devices by which status groups protect their domains.

Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser write that Herrnstein and Murray provide scant evidence of growth in cognitive sorting. Using data from the General Social Survey, they tested each of these hypotheses using a short verbal ability test which was administered to about 12,500 American adults between 1974 and 1994; the results provided no support for any of the trend hypotheses advanced by Herrnstein and Murray. One chart in The Bell Curve purports to show that people with IQs above 120 have become "rapidly more concentrated" in high-IQ occupations since 1940. But Robert Hauser and his colleague Min-Hsiung Huang retested the data and came up with estimates that fell "well below those of Herrnstein and Murray." They add that the data, properly used, "do not tell us anything except that selected, highly educated occupation groups have grown rapidly since 1940."

In 1972, Noam Chomsky questioned Herrnstein's idea that society was developing towards a meritocracy. Chomsky criticized the assumptions that people only seek occupations based on material gain. He argued that Herrnstein would not want to become a baker or lumberjack even if he could earn more money that way. He also criticized the assumption that such a society would be fair with pay based on value of contributions. He argued that because there are already substantial inequalities, people will often be paid at levels that preserve such inequalities rather than commensurately with their contribution to society.

Race and intelligence

See also: History of the race and intelligence controversy

One part of the controversy concerned the parts of the book which dealt with racial group differences on IQ and the consequences of this. In chapter 13, the authors state: "It seems highly likely to us that both genes and the environment have something to do with racial differences." The introduction to the chapter, however, provides the caveat that "The debate about whether and how much genes and environment have to do with ethnic differences remains unresolved".

In an article praising the book, economist Thomas Sowell criticized some of its aspects, including some of its arguments about race and the malleability of IQ:

When European immigrant groups in the United States scored below the national average on mental tests, they scored lowest on the abstract parts of those tests. So did white mountaineer children in the United States tested back in the early 1930s ... Strangely, Herrnstein and Murray refer to "folklore" that "Jews and other immigrant groups were thought to be below average in intelligence." It was neither folklore nor anything as subjective as thoughts. It was based on hard data, as hard as any data in The Bell Curve. These groups repeatedly tested below average on the mental tests of the World War I era, both in the army and in civilian life. For Jews, it is clear that later tests showed radically different results—during an era when there was very little intermarriage to change the genetic makeup of American Jews.

In 1996, Stephen Jay Gould released a revised and expanded edition of his 1981 book The Mismeasure of Man, intended to more directly refute many of The Bell Curve's claims regarding race and intelligence, and argued that the evidence for heritability of IQ did not indicate a genetic origin to group differences in intelligence.

Psychologist David Marks has suggested that the ASVAB test used in the analyses of The Bell Curve correlates highly with measures of literacy, and argues that the ASVAB test in fact is not a measure of general intelligence but of literacy.

Melvin Konner, professor of anthropology and associate professor of psychiatry and neurology at Emory University, called Bell Curve a "deliberate assault on efforts to improve the school performance of African-Americans":

This book presented strong evidence that genes play a role in intelligence but linked it to the unsupported claim that genes explain the small but consistent black-white difference in IQ. The juxtaposition of good argument with a bad one seemed politically motivated, and persuasive refutations soon appeared. Actually, African-Americans have excelled in virtually every enriched environment they have been placed in, most of which they were previously barred from, and this in only the first decade or two of improved but still not equal opportunity. It is likely that the real curves for the two races will one day be superimposable on each other, but this may require decades of change and different environments for different people. Claims about genetic potential are meaningless except in light of this requirement.

The 2014 textbook Evolutionary Analysis by Herron and Freeman devoted an entire chapter to debunking what they termed the "Bell Curve fallacy", saying that "Murray and Herrnstein's argument amounts to little more than an appeal to personal incredulity" and that it is a mistake to think that heritability can tell us something about the causes of differences between population means. In reference to the comparison of African-American with European-American IQ scores, the text states that only a common garden experiment, in which the two groups are raised in an environment typically experienced by European-Americans, would allow one to see if the difference is genetic. This kind of experiment, routine with plants and animals, cannot be conducted with humans. Nor is it possible to approximate this design with adoptions into families of the different groups, because the children would be recognizable and possibly be treated differently. The text concludes: "There is no way to assess whether genetics has anything to do with the difference in IQ score between ethnic groups."

Rutledge M. Dennis suggests that through soundbites of works like Jensen's famous study on the achievement gap, and Herrnstein and Murray's book The Bell Curve, the media "paints a picture of Blacks and other people of color as collective biological illiterates—as not only intellectually unfit but evil and criminal as well", thus providing, he says "the logic and justification for those who would further disenfranchise and exclude racial and ethnic minorities".

Charles Lane pointed out that 17 of the researchers whose work is referenced by the book have also contributed to Mankind Quarterly, a journal of anthropology founded in 1960 in Edinburgh, which has been viewed as supporting the theory of the genetic superiority of white people. David Bartholomew reports Murray's response as part of the controversy over the Bell Curve. In his afterword to the 1996 Free Press edition of The Bell Curve, Murray responded that the book "draws its evidence from more than a thousand scholars" and among the researchers mentioned in Lane's list "are some of the most respected psychologists of our time and that almost all of the sources referred to as tainted are articles published in leading refereed journals".

The Bell Curve Wars: Race, Intelligence, and the Future of America is a collection of articles published in reaction to the book. Edited by Steven Fraser, the writers of these essays do not have a specific viewpoint concerning the content of The Bell Curve, but express their own critiques of various aspects of the book, including the research methods used, the alleged hidden biases in the research and the policies suggested as a result of the conclusions drawn by the authors. Fraser writes that "by scrutinizing the footnotes and bibliography in The Bell Curve, readers can more easily recognize the project for what it is: a chilly synthesis of the work of disreputable race theorists and eccentric eugenicists".

Allegations of racism

Since the book provided statistical data making the assertion that blacks were, on average, less intelligent than whites, some people have argued that The Bell Curve could be used by extremists to justify genocide and hate crimes. Much of the work referenced by The Bell Curve was funded by the Pioneer Fund, which aims to advance the scientific study of heredity and human differences, and which has been accused of promoting white supremacist views, particularly scientific racism. Murray criticized the characterization of the Pioneer Fund as a racist organization, arguing that it has as much relationship to its founder as "Henry Ford and today's Ford Foundation".

Evolutionary biologist Joseph L. Graves described The Bell Curve as an example of racist science, containing all the types of errors in the application of scientific method that have characterized the history of scientific racism:

  1. Claims that are not supported by the data given
  2. Errors in calculation that invariably support the hypothesis
  3. No mention of data that contradict the hypothesis
  4. No mention of theories and data that conflict with core assumptions
  5. Bold policy recommendations that are consistent with those advocated by racists.

Eric Siegel wrote on the Scientific American blog that the book "endorses prejudice by virtue of what it does not say. Nowhere does the book address why it investigates racial differences in IQ. By never spelling out a reason for reporting on these differences in the first place, the authors transmit an unspoken yet unequivocal conclusion: Race is a helpful indicator as to whether a person is likely to hold certain capabilities. Even if we assume the presented data trends are sound, the book leaves the reader on his or her own to deduce how to best put these insights to use. The net effect is to tacitly condone the prejudgment of individuals based on race." Similarly, Howard Gardner accused the authors of engaging in "scholarly brinkmanship", arguing that "Whether concerning an issue of science, policy, or rhetoric, the authors come dangerously close to embracing the most extreme positions, yet in the end shy away from doing so ... Scholarly brinkmanship encourages the reader to draw the strongest conclusions, while allowing the authors to disavow this intention."

Columnist Bob Herbert, writing for The New York Times, described the book as "a scabrous piece of racial pornography masquerading as serious scholarship". "Mr. Murray can protest all he wants", wrote Herbert; "his book is just a genteel way of calling somebody a nigger."

See also

References

  1. Bird, Kevin; Jackson, John P.; Winston, Andrew S. (2024). "Confronting Scientific Racism in Psychology: Lessons from Evolutionary Biology and Genetics". American Psychologist. 79 (4): 497–508. doi:10.1037/amp0001228. PMID 39037836. Recent articles claim that the folk categories of race are genetically meaningful divisions, and that evolved genetic differences among races and nations are important for explaining immutable differences in cognitive ability, educational attainment, crime, sexual behavior, and wealth; all claims that are opposed by a strong scientific consensus to the contrary. ... Despite the veneer of modern science, RHR psychologists' recent efforts merely repeat discredited racist ideas of a century ago. The issue is truly one of scientific standards; if psychology embraced the scientific practices of evolutionary biology and genetics, current forms of RHR would not be publishable in reputable scholarly journals.
  2. Turkheimer, Eric; Harden, Kathryn Paige; Nisbett, Richard E. (June 15, 2017). "There's still no good reason to believe black-white IQ differences are due to genes". Vox. Vox Media. Archived from the original on May 4, 2021. Retrieved 29 April 2021.
  3. Panofsky, Aaron; Dasgupta, Kushan; Iturriaga, Nicole (2021). "How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 175 (2): 387–398. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24150. PMC 9909835. PMID 32986847. S2CID 222163480.
  4. Rosenthal, S. J. (1995). The Pioneer Fund: PROD. The American Behavioral Scientist, 39(1), 44.
  5. ^ Devlin, Bernie; Fienberg, Stephen E.; Resnick, Daniel P.; Roeder, Kathryn (1997). Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to The Bell Curve. Springer Science & Business Media. ISBN 978-0-387-94986-4.
  6. Herrnstein, Richard J.; Murray, Charles (11 May 2010). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. Simon and Schuster. pp. 22–23. ISBN 978-1-4391-3491-7.
  7. Herrnstein & Murray (1994) pp. 263–264
  8. Herrnstein & Murray (1994) pp. 171, 158, 163, 174, 230, 180, 132, 194, 247–248, 194, 146, 264 respectively.
  9. p. 518.
  10. p. 526.
  11. pp. 548–549.
  12. ^ Lemann, Nicholas (1997-01-18). "The Bell Curve Flattened". Slate. ISSN 1091-2339. Retrieved 2016-09-10.
  13. ^ Gould, Stephen Jay (1994-11-28). "Curveball". The New Yorker. Retrieved 2016-09-10.
  14. "Racism Resurgent: How Media Let The Bell Curve's Pseudo-Science Define the Agenda on Race" by Jim Naureckas January/February 1995
  15. T. Dickens, William; L. Schultze, Charles; J. Kane, Thomas (1995-06-01). "Does The Bell Curve Ring True? A Closer Look at a Grim Portrait of American Society". Brookings Institution. Retrieved 2022-09-25.
  16. Arthur S. Goldberger and Charles F. Manski (1995) "Review Article: The Bell Curve by Herrnstein and Murray", Journal of Economic Literature, 36(2), June 1995, pp. 762–776. "HM and their publishers have done a disservice by circumventing peer review.... a process of scientific review is now under way. But, given the process to date, peer review of The Bell Curve is now an exercise in damage control ..."
  17. Gottfredson, Linda S. (1997). "Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial)" (PDF). Intelligence. 24: 13–23. doi:10.1016/s0160-2896(97)90011-8. ISSN 0160-2896.
  18. Gottfredson 1997, pp. 17–20
  19. ^ "Linda Gottfredson". Southern Poverty Law Center. Retrieved 2018-02-11.
  20. ^ Campbell 1996
  21. Graves & Johnson 1995, pp. 279–280
  22. Laosa 1996
  23. Harrington 1997, pp. 116–118
  24. Alderfer (2003)
  25. Armour-Thomas (2003)
  26. Neisser, Ulric; Boodoo, Gwyneth; Bouchard, Thomas J. Jr.; Boykin, A. Wade; Brody, Nathan; Ceci, Stephen J.; Halpern, Diane F.; Loehlin, John C.; Perloff, Robert; Sternberg, Robert J.; Urbina, Susana (1996). "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" (PDF). American Psychologist. 51 (2): 77–101. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77.
  27. Alderfer (2003)
  28. "A Review of The Bell Curve: Bad Science Makes for Bad Conclusions". David Boles, Blogs. 1998-03-23. Retrieved 2016-09-10.
  29. Miele, Frank (1995). "An Interview with the Author of The Bell Curve". Skeptic (U.S. magazine). Archived from the original on February 5, 2005. Retrieved November 26, 2019.
  30. ^ Heckman, James J. (1995). "Lessons from the Bell Curve". Journal of Political Economy. 103 (5): 1091–1120. doi:10.1086/262014. S2CID 153463195..
  31. August 1995 letter exchange in Commentary magazine
  32. Interview with James Heckman. Douglas Clement. June 2005. The Region.
  33. Rollback, Part II Noam Chomsky, 1995
  34. ^ Block, Ned (January 6, 1996). "How Heritability Misleads about Race". NYU.
  35. Segalowitz, Sidney J. (October 1999). "Why twin studies really don't tell us much about human heritability". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 22 (5): 904–905. doi:10.1017/s0140525x99442207. ISSN 0140-525X. S2CID 143865688.
  36. Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Princeton University Press, 1996.
  37. http://ssrn.com/abstract=225294 Korenman, Sanders and Winship, Christopher, "A Reanalysis of The Bell Curve" (August 1995). NBER Working Paper Series, Vol. w5230, 1995.
  38. William J. Matthews, Ph.D. (1998) A Review of the Bell Curve: Bad Science Makes for Bad Conclusions
  39. Cracked Bell James J. Heckman. March 1995. Reason
  40. "The Intergenerational Transmission of 'Intelligence' Down the Slippery Slopes of The Bell Curve". Industrial Relations: A Journal of Economy and Society, Vol. 38, No. 3, July 1999
  41. Wilson, Frank Harold (1995). "For Whom Does the Bell Toll?: Meritocracy, the Cognitive Elite, and the Continuing Significance of Race in Postindustrial America". The Journal of Negro Education. 64 (3): 253–266. doi:10.2307/2967207. JSTOR 2967207.
  42. Lemann, Nicholas (1997). "Is There a Cognitive Elite in America?". Intelligence, Genes, and Success. Springer New York. pp. 315–325. doi:10.1007/978-1-4612-0669-9_14. ISBN 978-0-387-94986-4.
  43. "IQ and Stratification: An Empirical Evaluation of Herrnstein and Murray's Social Change Argument". Charles R. Tittle, Thomas Rotolo Social Forces, Vol. 79, No. 1 (Sep. 2000), pp. 1–28
  44. Verbal Ability and Socioeconomic Success: A Trend Analysis Hauser R. M.; Huang M. H.
  45. Chomsky, Noam. 1972. ""Chomsky on IQ and inequality". I.Q. Tests: Building Blocks for the New Class System Archived 2011-05-11 at the Wayback Machine." Rampart: 24–30. pp. 26–28, 30.
  46. Sowell, Thomas (1995). "Ethnicity and IQ". The American Spectator. 28 (2).
  47. Flynn, J. R. (1999). "Evidence against Rushton: The Genetic Loading of the Wisc-R Subtests and the Causes of Between-Group IQ Differences". Personality and Individual Differences. 26 (2): 373–393. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(98)00149-4.
  48. Gould, Stephen Jay (1996). The mismeasure of man (Rev. and expanded ed.). New York: Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-31425-0.
  49. Marks, D. F. (2010). "IQ variations across time, race, and nationality: an artifact of differences in literacy skills". Psychological Reports. 106 (3): 643–664. doi:10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664. PMID 20712152. S2CID 12179547.
  50. Kaufman, Scott Barry (August 23, 2010). "The Flynn Effect and IQ Disparities Among Races, Ethnicities, and Nations: Are There Common Links?". Psychology Today. Retrieved May 4, 2020.
  51. Konnor, Melvin (2003). The Tangled Wing: Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit (2nd ed.). Henry Holt and Company. p. 428.
  52. Herron and Freeman, Jon and Scott (2014). Evolutionary Analysis. Boston: Pearson. pp. 360–363.
  53. Dennis, Rutledge M. (Summer 1995). "Social Darwinism, scientific racism, and the metaphysics of race" (PDF). Journal of Negro Education. 64 (3): 243–252. doi:10.2307/2967206. JSTOR 2967206.
  54. Lane, Charles (1994-12-01). "The Tainted Sources of 'The Bell Curve'". The New York Review of Books. ISSN 0028-7504. Retrieved 2016-09-10.
  55. Bartholomew, David J. (2004). Measuring Intelligence: Facts and Fallacies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 10. ISBN 978-0-521-54478-8.
  56. Herrnstein, Richard J.; Murray, Charles (1996). The Bell Curve. New York: Free Press. p. 564.
  57. Steven Fraser, ed. (1995). The Bell Curve Wars: race, intelligence, and the future of America. New York: Basic Books. ISBN 978-0-465-00693-9.
  58. Fraser, Steven, ed. (1995). The Bell Curve Wars: Race, Intelligence, and the Future of America. Basic Books. ISBN 978-0-465-00693-9. OCLC 782205959.
  59. Coyne, Jerry (August 2, 2006). "Ann Coulter and Charles Darwin. Coultergeist". TalkReason. Archived from the original on 2009-04-30. Retrieved May 4, 2020.
  60. Naureckas, Jim (January 1, 1995). "Racism Resurgent". FAIR. Retrieved May 4, 2020.
  61. "The Bell Curve and the Pioneer Fund" (transcript from ABC World News Tonight). November 22, 1994. Retrieved May 4, 2020 – via Hartford Web Publishing.
  62. Metcalf, Stephen (October 17, 2005). "Moral Courage: Is defending The Bell Curve an example of intellectual honesty?". Slate. Retrieved May 4, 2020.
  63. Rosenthal, Steven J. (1995). "The Pioneer Fund: Financier of Fascist Research". American Behavioral Scientist. 39 (1): 44–61. doi:10.1177/0002764295039001006. ISSN 0002-7642.
  64. Herrnstein & Murray (1994) p. 564.
  65. Graves, Joseph L. (2001). The Emperor's New Clothes. Rutgers University Press. p. 8. ISBN 978-0-8135-2847-2.
  66. Siegel, Eric (April 12, 2017). "The Real Problem with Charles Murray and 'The Bell Curve'". Scientific American. Retrieved May 4, 2020.
  67. Gardner, Howard (Winter 1995). "Cracking Open the IQ Box". The American Prospect.
  68. Herbert, Bob (1994-10-26). "In America; Throwing a Curve". The New York Times. Retrieved 2007-01-09.

Sources

Further reading

External links

Interviews with Murray

Responses to The Bell Curve

Categories: