Misplaced Pages

Anecdotal evidence: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 02:23, 3 February 2006 edit213.130.141.22 (talk) rv replacement with unwikified essay by known POV pusher← Previous edit Latest revision as of 19:33, 24 October 2024 edit undoRetimuko (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users7,289 edits Undid revision 1253186231 by Rabia Islam (talk)Tag: Undo 
(496 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|Evidence relying on personal testimony}}
'''Anecdotal evidence''' is a term used in medical, scientific and legal discourse to refer to evidence, typically in the form of isolated ]s, describing an event that occurred under ] conditions.
{{redirect|Anecdata|the web portal|Anecdata.org}}
{{Copy edit|date=August 2024}}
An '''anecdotal evidence''' (or '''anecdata'''<ref>{{Citation |title=anecdata, n. |date=2023-03-02 |work=Oxford English Dictionary |url=https://oed.com/dictionary/anecdata_n |access-date=2024-10-03 |edition=3 |publisher=Oxford University Press |language=en |doi=10.1093/oed/9365074374}}</ref>) is a piece of ] based on descriptions and reports of individual, personal experiences, or observations,<ref name=":0">{{Citation |last=Irwig |first=Les |title=The weakness of one |date=2008 |work=Smart Health Choices: Making Sense of Health Advice |url=https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK63643/ |access-date=2024-10-03 |publisher=Hammersmith Press |language=en |last2=Irwig |first2=Judy |last3=Trevena |first3=Lyndal |last4=Sweet |first4=Melissa}}</ref><ref name=":1">{{Cite book |url=https://archive.org/details/oxforddictionary0000unse_a2v4/mode/2up |title=Oxford dictionary of English |date=2010 |publisher=Oxford ; New York : Oxford University Press |others=Internet Archive |isbn=978-0-19-957112-3 |pages=58}}</ref> collected in a non-] manner.<ref>{{Citation |last=Porta |first=Miquel |title=Anecdotal Evidence |date=2016-07-21 |work=A Dictionary of Epidemiology |url=https://www.oxfordreference.com/display/10.1093/acref/9780199976720.001.0001/acref-9780199976720-e-60 |access-date=2024-10-03 |publisher=Oxford University Press |language=en |doi=10.1093/acref/9780199976720.001.0001/acref-9780199976720-e-60 |isbn=978-0-19-997672-0}}</ref>


The word ''anecdotal'' constitutes a variety of forms of evidence. This word refers to personal experiences, self-reported claims,<ref name=":1" /> or eyewitness accounts of others,<ref>{{Cite book |last=Cambridge University Press |url=https://archive.org/details/cambridgeacademi00camb/mode/2up |title=Cambridge academic content dictionary |date=2009 |publisher=New York : Cambridge University Press |others=Internet Archive |isbn=978-0-521-87143-3 |pages=31}}</ref> including those from fictional sources, making it a broad category that can lead to confusion due to its varied interpretations.
Anecdotal evidence may be true (for instance, the existence of the ] was originally anecdotal). However, it is viewed with caution, as personal testimony is subject to ]; and even if it is true, there is no way to know if a single ] represents the trend for the data or is an exception (an ]).


Anecdotal evidence can be true or false but is not usually subjected to the methodology of ], the ], or the rules of legal, historical, academic, or ], meaning that there are little or no safeguards against fabrication or inaccuracy.<ref name=":0" /> However, the use of anecdotal reports in ] or promotion of a product, service, or idea may be considered a ], which is highly regulated in some jurisdictions.<ref>{{Cite web |title=16 CFR Part 255 (Oct. 1, 2024) -- Guides Concerning Use of Endorsements and Testimonials in Advertising |url=https://www.ecfr.gov/on/2024-10-01/title-16/chapter-I/subchapter-B/part-255 |access-date=2024-10-03 |website=eCFR |language=en}}</ref>
It commonly arises in the form of the ] of ]: for instance, "My grandmother lived to 95, smoked constantly, and didn't die of lung cancer" may be taken as evidence that smoking does not cause lung cancer.


The persuasiveness of anecdotal evidence compared to that of statistical evidence has been a subject of debate; some studies have argued for the presence a generalized tendency to overvalue anecdotal evidence, whereas others have emphasized the types of argument as a prerequisite or rejected the conclusion altogether.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hoeken |first=Hans |last2=Hustinx |first2=Lettica |date= |title=When is Statistical Evidence Superior to Anecdotal Evidence in Supporting Probability Claims? The Role of Argument Type |url=https://academic.oup.com/hcr/article/35/4/491-510/4107468 |journal=Human Communication Research |language=en |publication-date=2009-10-01 |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=491–510 |doi=10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009.01360.x}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hornikx |first=J. M. A. |date=2007 |title=Is anecdotal evidence more persuasive than statistical evidence? A comment on classic cognitive psychological studies |url=https://repository.ubn.ru.nl/handle/2066/44329 |journal=164 |language=en}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hoeken |first=Hans |date=2001-11-01 |title=Anecdotal, Statistical, and Causal Evidence: Their Perceived and Actual Persuasiveness |url=https://link.springer.com/article/10.1023/A:1012075630523 |journal=Argumentation |language=en |volume=15 |issue=4 |pages=425–437 |doi=10.1023/A:1012075630523 |issn=1572-8374}}</ref><ref name="cognitive-research">{{cite journal |last1=Michal |first1=Audrey |date=2021 |title=When and why do people act on flawed science? Effects of anecdotes and prior beliefs on evidence-based decision-making |journal=Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications |volume=6 |issue=1 |page=28 |doi=10.1186/s41235-021-00293-2 |pmc=8023527 |pmid=33825055 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hoeken |first=H. |last2=Hustinx |first2=L. G. M. M. |date=2003 |title=The relative persuasiveness of anecdotal, statistical, causal, and expert evidence |url=https://repository.ubn.ru.nl/handle/2066/82921 |journal=502 |language=en}}</ref>
While not conclusive in itself, multiply-corroborated anecdotal evidence may provide the basis for more rigorous investigation. For example, anecdotal testimony commonly forms part of the evidence for discrimination lawsuits, and may be sufficient to initiate a ].


== Scientific context ==
==References==
{{See also|Scientific evidence}}
* Ministry of Economic Development, New Zealand
In science, definitions of anecdotal evidence include:
* Sprenger & Lang, Attorneys
* MSBN, June 22, 2004
* City of Phoenix study


* "casual observations/indications rather than rigorous or scientific analysis"<ref name="yourdictionary">{{cite web|url =https://www.yourdictionary.com/anecdotal|website = YourDictionary.com|title = anecdotal|access-date = 17 June 2019}}</ref>
== External Links ==
* "information passed along by word-of-mouth but not documented scientifically"<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.nechakowhitesturgeon.org/glossary|title=Nechako White Sturgeon Recovery Initiative - Glossary - NWSRI|website=www.nechakowhitesturgeon.org|access-date=2020-04-07}}</ref>
* from a course in ] at ].
*"evidence that comes from an individual experience. This may be the experience of a person with an illness or the experience of a practitioner based on one or more patients outside a formal research study."<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK63658/def-item/glossary.gl1-d4/|title=Anecdotal evidence - Smart Health Choices - NCBI Bookshelf|website=www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov|access-date=2020-04-07}}</ref>
* .
*"the report of an experience by one or more persons that is not objectively documented or an experience or outcome that occurred outside of a controlled environment"<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://theness.com/neurologicablog/index.php/no-love-for-anecdotal-evidence/|title=No Love for Anecdotal Evidence|date=2007-03-08|website=NeuroLogica Blog|access-date=2020-04-07}}</ref>
* , from the ]'s Dictionary.

Anecdotal evidence may be considered within the scope of ] as some anecdotal evidence can be both empirical and verifiable, e.g. in the use of ] in medicine. Other anecdotal evidence, however, does not qualify as ], because its nature prevents it from being investigated by the scientific method, for instance, in that of ] or in the case of intentionally fictional anecdotes. Where only one or a few anecdotes are presented, there is a chance that they may be unreliable due to ] or otherwise ] samples of typical cases.<ref name="weiten">{{cite book|page = 75 |title= Psychology: Themes and Variations|first=Wayne|last= Weiten|publisher = Wadsworth/Cengage Learning|date = 2010|isbn = 9780495601975}}</ref><ref>{{cite book|url = https://books.google.com/books?id=eNsVUGTMcDoC&pg=PA25|page= 25 |title= Research in Psychology: Methods and Design|first = C. James|last = Goodwin|publisher = John Wiley & Sons|date= 2009|isbn= 9780470522783}}</ref> Similarly, psychologists have found that due to ] people are more likely to remember notable or unusual examples rather than typical examples.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gibson |first1=Rhonda |last2=Zillman |first2=Dolf |year=1994 |title=Exaggerated Versus Representative Exemplification in News Reports: Perception of Issues and Personal Consequences |journal=Communication Research |volume=21 |issue=5 |pages=603–624 |doi=10.1177/009365094021005003 |s2cid=145050644 }}</ref> Thus, even when accurate, anecdotal evidence is not necessarily representative of a typical experience. Accurate determination of whether an anecdote is typical requires ] evidence.<ref>{{cite web |last1=Schwarcz |first1=Joe |author1-link=Joseph A. Schwarcz |last2=Barrett |first2=Stephen |author2-link=Stephen Barrett |url=http://www.quackwatch.org/01QuackeryRelatedTopics/science.html |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120920043202/http://www.quackwatch.org/01QuackeryRelatedTopics/science.html |archive-date=20 September 2012 |access-date=16 June 2022 |title=Some Notes on the Nature of Science |url-status=unfit}}{{cbignore}}</ref> Misuse of anecdotal evidence in the form of ] is an ]<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.iep.utm.edu/fallacy/#AnecdotalEvidence|title=Fallacies {{!}} Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy|website=www.iep.utm.edu|access-date=2020-04-07}}</ref> and is sometimes referred to as the "person who" fallacy ("I know a person who..."; "I know of a case where..." etc.) which places undue weight on experiences of close peers which may not be typical.

Anecdotal evidence can have varying degrees of formality. For instance, in medicine, published anecdotal evidence by a trained observer (a doctor) is called a ], and is subjected to formal ].<ref name="Jenicek">{{cite book |last= Jenicek |first= M. |title= Clinical Case Reporting in Evidence-Based Medicine |location= Oxford |publisher= Butterworth–Heinemann |year= 1999 |pages= 117 |isbn= 0-7506-4592-X }}</ref> Although such evidence is not seen as conclusive, researchers may sometimes regard it as an invitation to more rigorous scientific study of the phenomenon in question.<ref name="Vandenbroucke">{{cite journal |last= Vandenbroucke |first= J. P. |year= 2001 |title= In Defense of Case Reports and Case Series |journal= Annals of Internal Medicine |volume= 134 |issue= 4 |pages= 330–334 |pmid= 11182844 |doi= 10.7326/0003-4819-134-4-200102200-00017|s2cid= 867759 }}</ref> For instance, one study found that 35 of 47 anecdotal reports of drug side-effects were later sustained as "clearly correct."<ref name="Venning">{{cite journal |last= Venning |first= G. R. |title= Validity of anecdotal reports of suspected adverse drug reactions: the problem of false alarms |journal= Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) |year= 1982 |volume= 284 |issue= 6311 |pages= 249–52 |pmid= 0006799125 |doi= 10.1136/bmj.284.6311.249 |pmc= 1495801}}</ref>

Anecdotal evidence is considered the least certain type of scientific information.<ref name="isbn0-12-588560-1">{{cite book |author= Riffenburgh, R. H. |title= Statistics in Medicine |publisher= Academic Press |location= Boston |year= 1999 |pages= |isbn= 0-12-588560-1 |url= https://archive.org/details/statisticsinmedi00riff/page/196 }}</ref> Researchers may use anecdotal evidence for suggesting new ], but never as validating evidence.<ref>{{cite book | last1 = Lilienfeld | first1 = Scott O. | author-link1 = Scott Lilienfeld | last2 = Lynn | first2 = Steven Jay | last3 = Lohr | first3 = Jeffrey M. | chapter = Initial Thoughts, Reflections, and Considerations | title = Science and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology | chapter-url = https://books.google.com/books?id=q50gBQAAQBAJ&pg=PA9 | edition = 2 | location = New York | publisher = Guilford Publications | date = 2014 | page = 9 | isbn = 9781462517510 | quote = Testimonial and anecdotal evidence can be quite useful in the early stages of scientific investigation. Nevertheless, such evidence is almost always much more helpful in the context of discovery (i.e., hypothesis generation) than in the context of justification (i.e., hypothesis testing ).}}</ref><ref name="Mebius">{{cite journal |last= Mebius |first= A. |year= 2022 |title= Against 'instantaneous' expertise |journal= Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine |volume= 17 |issue= 11 |page= 11 |doi= 10.1186/s13010-022-00123-3 |pmid= 36127693 |pmc= 9490894 |s2cid= 252384889 |doi-access= free }}</ref>

If an anecdote illustrates a desired conclusion rather than a logical conclusion, it is considered a ] or ].<ref name="Thompson">Thompson B. {{webarchive |url= https://web.archive.org/web/20060420154531/http://www.cuyamaca.edu/bruce.thompson/Fallacies/anecdotal.asp |date= April 20, 2006 }}</ref>

In any case where some factor affects the probability of an outcome, rather than uniquely determining it, selected individual cases prove nothing; e.g. "my grandfather smoked two packs a day until he died at 90" and "my sister never smoked but died of lung cancer". Anecdotes often refer to the exception, rather than the rule: "Anecdotes are useless precisely because they may point to idiosyncratic responses."<ref>{{cite journal | first= Scott H. |last= Sicherer |title= Food allergy: When and how to perform oral food challenges |journal= Pediatric Allergy and Immunology |year= 1999 |volume= 10 |issue= 4 |pages= 226–234 |doi= 10.1034/j.1399-3038.1999.00040.x |pmid= 10678717 |s2cid= 1484234 }}</ref>

In medicine, anecdotal evidence is also subject to ]s.<ref name="Lee">{{Cite web|url=https://www.medicinenet.com/evaluating_medications_and_supplement_products/views.htm|title=Evaluating Treatment Products|website=MedicineNet}}<!-- old archived link was https://archive.today/20121209111018/http://www.medicinenet.com/script/main/art.asp?articlekey=55090&page=2 --></ref>

== Legal ==
In the legal sphere, anecdotal evidence, if it passes certain legal requirements and is admitted as ], is a common form of evidence used in a court of law. Often this form of anecdotal evidence is the only evidence presented at trial.<ref>{{cite web |title=The Judicial Learning Center | date=10 August 2012 |url=https://judiciallearningcenter.org/your-day-in-court/#:~:text=Often%20witness%20testimony%20may%20be,the%20actual%20event%20taking%20place.}}</ref> Scientific evidence in a court of law is called ], but this is much rarer. Anecdotal evidence, with a few safeguards, represents the bulk of evidence in court.

The legal rigors applied to testimony for it to be considered evidence is that it must be given ], that the person is only testifying to their own words and actions, and that someone intentionally lying under oath is subject to ]. However, these rigors do not make ] in a court of law equal to ] as there are far less legal rigors. Testimony about another person's experiences or words is called ] and is usually not admissible, though there are certain exceptions. However, any hearsay that is not objected to or thrown out by a judge is considered evidence for a jury. This means that trials contain quite a bit of anecdotal evidence, which is considered as relevant evidence by a jury. Eyewitness testimony (which is a form of anecdotal evidence) is considered the most compelling form of evidence by a jury.<ref>{{cite web |title=Benton, Ross, Bradshaw, Thomas, & Bradshaw, 2006 |url=https://www.psychologicalscience.org/uncategorized/myth-eyewitness-testimony-is-the-best-kind-of-evidence.html}}</ref>


==See also== ==See also==
{{wiktionary|anecdotal evidence}}
* ]
{{Div col}}
* ]
* {{annotated link|Anecdotal value}}
* ]
* {{annotated link|Argument from ignorance}}
* ]
* {{annotated link|Confirmation bias}}
* {{annotated link|Correlation does not imply causation}}
* {{annotated link|Empirical evidence}}
* {{annotated link|Eyewitness testimony}}
* {{annotated link|Fallacy}}
* {{annotated link|Faulty generalization}}
* {{annotated link|Hasty generalization}}
* {{annotated link|List of fallacies}}
* {{annotated link|Lived experience}}
* {{annotated link|Post hoc ergo propter hoc}}
* {{annotated link|Presumption of guilt}}
* {{annotated link|Scientific method}}

{{Div col end}}

==References==
{{reflist}}

{{Fallacies}}
{{Authority control}}


]
]
]
] ]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 19:33, 24 October 2024

Evidence relying on personal testimony "Anecdata" redirects here. For the web portal, see Anecdata.org.
This article may require copy editing for grammar, style, cohesion, tone, or spelling. You can assist by editing it. (August 2024) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

An anecdotal evidence (or anecdata) is a piece of evidence based on descriptions and reports of individual, personal experiences, or observations, collected in a non-systematic manner.

The word anecdotal constitutes a variety of forms of evidence. This word refers to personal experiences, self-reported claims, or eyewitness accounts of others, including those from fictional sources, making it a broad category that can lead to confusion due to its varied interpretations.

Anecdotal evidence can be true or false but is not usually subjected to the methodology of scholarly method, the scientific method, or the rules of legal, historical, academic, or intellectual rigor, meaning that there are little or no safeguards against fabrication or inaccuracy. However, the use of anecdotal reports in advertising or promotion of a product, service, or idea may be considered a testimonial, which is highly regulated in some jurisdictions.

The persuasiveness of anecdotal evidence compared to that of statistical evidence has been a subject of debate; some studies have argued for the presence a generalized tendency to overvalue anecdotal evidence, whereas others have emphasized the types of argument as a prerequisite or rejected the conclusion altogether.

Scientific context

See also: Scientific evidence

In science, definitions of anecdotal evidence include:

  • "casual observations/indications rather than rigorous or scientific analysis"
  • "information passed along by word-of-mouth but not documented scientifically"
  • "evidence that comes from an individual experience. This may be the experience of a person with an illness or the experience of a practitioner based on one or more patients outside a formal research study."
  • "the report of an experience by one or more persons that is not objectively documented or an experience or outcome that occurred outside of a controlled environment"

Anecdotal evidence may be considered within the scope of scientific method as some anecdotal evidence can be both empirical and verifiable, e.g. in the use of case studies in medicine. Other anecdotal evidence, however, does not qualify as scientific evidence, because its nature prevents it from being investigated by the scientific method, for instance, in that of folklore or in the case of intentionally fictional anecdotes. Where only one or a few anecdotes are presented, there is a chance that they may be unreliable due to cherry-picked or otherwise non-representative samples of typical cases. Similarly, psychologists have found that due to cognitive bias people are more likely to remember notable or unusual examples rather than typical examples. Thus, even when accurate, anecdotal evidence is not necessarily representative of a typical experience. Accurate determination of whether an anecdote is typical requires statistical evidence. Misuse of anecdotal evidence in the form of argument from anecdote is an informal fallacy and is sometimes referred to as the "person who" fallacy ("I know a person who..."; "I know of a case where..." etc.) which places undue weight on experiences of close peers which may not be typical.

Anecdotal evidence can have varying degrees of formality. For instance, in medicine, published anecdotal evidence by a trained observer (a doctor) is called a case report, and is subjected to formal peer review. Although such evidence is not seen as conclusive, researchers may sometimes regard it as an invitation to more rigorous scientific study of the phenomenon in question. For instance, one study found that 35 of 47 anecdotal reports of drug side-effects were later sustained as "clearly correct."

Anecdotal evidence is considered the least certain type of scientific information. Researchers may use anecdotal evidence for suggesting new hypotheses, but never as validating evidence.

If an anecdote illustrates a desired conclusion rather than a logical conclusion, it is considered a faulty or hasty generalization.

In any case where some factor affects the probability of an outcome, rather than uniquely determining it, selected individual cases prove nothing; e.g. "my grandfather smoked two packs a day until he died at 90" and "my sister never smoked but died of lung cancer". Anecdotes often refer to the exception, rather than the rule: "Anecdotes are useless precisely because they may point to idiosyncratic responses."

In medicine, anecdotal evidence is also subject to placebo effects.

Legal

In the legal sphere, anecdotal evidence, if it passes certain legal requirements and is admitted as testimony, is a common form of evidence used in a court of law. Often this form of anecdotal evidence is the only evidence presented at trial. Scientific evidence in a court of law is called physical evidence, but this is much rarer. Anecdotal evidence, with a few safeguards, represents the bulk of evidence in court.

The legal rigors applied to testimony for it to be considered evidence is that it must be given under oath, that the person is only testifying to their own words and actions, and that someone intentionally lying under oath is subject to perjury. However, these rigors do not make testimony in a court of law equal to scientific evidence as there are far less legal rigors. Testimony about another person's experiences or words is called hearsay and is usually not admissible, though there are certain exceptions. However, any hearsay that is not objected to or thrown out by a judge is considered evidence for a jury. This means that trials contain quite a bit of anecdotal evidence, which is considered as relevant evidence by a jury. Eyewitness testimony (which is a form of anecdotal evidence) is considered the most compelling form of evidence by a jury.

See also

References

  1. "anecdata, n.", Oxford English Dictionary (3 ed.), Oxford University Press, 2023-03-02, doi:10.1093/oed/9365074374, retrieved 2024-10-03
  2. ^ Irwig, Les; Irwig, Judy; Trevena, Lyndal; Sweet, Melissa (2008), "The weakness of one", Smart Health Choices: Making Sense of Health Advice, Hammersmith Press, retrieved 2024-10-03
  3. ^ Oxford dictionary of English. Internet Archive. Oxford ; New York : Oxford University Press. 2010. p. 58. ISBN 978-0-19-957112-3.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: others (link)
  4. Porta, Miquel (2016-07-21), "Anecdotal Evidence", A Dictionary of Epidemiology, Oxford University Press, doi:10.1093/acref/9780199976720.001.0001/acref-9780199976720-e-60, ISBN 978-0-19-997672-0, retrieved 2024-10-03
  5. Cambridge University Press (2009). Cambridge academic content dictionary. Internet Archive. New York : Cambridge University Press. p. 31. ISBN 978-0-521-87143-3.
  6. "16 CFR Part 255 (Oct. 1, 2024) -- Guides Concerning Use of Endorsements and Testimonials in Advertising". eCFR. Retrieved 2024-10-03.
  7. Hoeken, Hans; Hustinx, Lettica (2009-10-01). "When is Statistical Evidence Superior to Anecdotal Evidence in Supporting Probability Claims? The Role of Argument Type". Human Communication Research. 35 (4): 491–510. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009.01360.x.
  8. Hornikx, J. M. A. (2007). "Is anecdotal evidence more persuasive than statistical evidence? A comment on classic cognitive psychological studies". 164.
  9. Hoeken, Hans (2001-11-01). "Anecdotal, Statistical, and Causal Evidence: Their Perceived and Actual Persuasiveness". Argumentation. 15 (4): 425–437. doi:10.1023/A:1012075630523. ISSN 1572-8374.
  10. Michal, Audrey (2021). "When and why do people act on flawed science? Effects of anecdotes and prior beliefs on evidence-based decision-making". Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications. 6 (1): 28. doi:10.1186/s41235-021-00293-2. PMC 8023527. PMID 33825055.
  11. Hoeken, H.; Hustinx, L. G. M. M. (2003). "The relative persuasiveness of anecdotal, statistical, causal, and expert evidence". 502.
  12. "anecdotal". YourDictionary.com. Retrieved 17 June 2019.
  13. "Nechako White Sturgeon Recovery Initiative - Glossary - NWSRI". www.nechakowhitesturgeon.org. Retrieved 2020-04-07.
  14. "Anecdotal evidence - Smart Health Choices - NCBI Bookshelf". www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov. Retrieved 2020-04-07.
  15. "No Love for Anecdotal Evidence". NeuroLogica Blog. 2007-03-08. Retrieved 2020-04-07.
  16. Weiten, Wayne (2010). Psychology: Themes and Variations. Wadsworth/Cengage Learning. p. 75. ISBN 9780495601975.
  17. Goodwin, C. James (2009). Research in Psychology: Methods and Design. John Wiley & Sons. p. 25. ISBN 9780470522783.
  18. Gibson, Rhonda; Zillman, Dolf (1994). "Exaggerated Versus Representative Exemplification in News Reports: Perception of Issues and Personal Consequences". Communication Research. 21 (5): 603–624. doi:10.1177/009365094021005003. S2CID 145050644.
  19. Schwarcz, Joe; Barrett, Stephen. "Some Notes on the Nature of Science". Archived from the original on 20 September 2012. Retrieved 16 June 2022.
  20. "Fallacies | Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy". www.iep.utm.edu. Retrieved 2020-04-07.
  21. Jenicek, M. (1999). Clinical Case Reporting in Evidence-Based Medicine. Oxford: Butterworth–Heinemann. p. 117. ISBN 0-7506-4592-X.
  22. Vandenbroucke, J. P. (2001). "In Defense of Case Reports and Case Series". Annals of Internal Medicine. 134 (4): 330–334. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-134-4-200102200-00017. PMID 11182844. S2CID 867759.
  23. Venning, G. R. (1982). "Validity of anecdotal reports of suspected adverse drug reactions: the problem of false alarms". Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 284 (6311): 249–52. doi:10.1136/bmj.284.6311.249. PMC 1495801. PMID 0006799125.
  24. Riffenburgh, R. H. (1999). Statistics in Medicine. Boston: Academic Press. pp. 196. ISBN 0-12-588560-1.
  25. Lilienfeld, Scott O.; Lynn, Steven Jay; Lohr, Jeffrey M. (2014). "Initial Thoughts, Reflections, and Considerations". Science and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology (2 ed.). New York: Guilford Publications. p. 9. ISBN 9781462517510. Testimonial and anecdotal evidence can be quite useful in the early stages of scientific investigation. Nevertheless, such evidence is almost always much more helpful in the context of discovery (i.e., hypothesis generation) than in the context of justification (i.e., hypothesis testing ).
  26. Mebius, A. (2022). "Against 'instantaneous' expertise". Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine. 17 (11): 11. doi:10.1186/s13010-022-00123-3. PMC 9490894. PMID 36127693. S2CID 252384889.
  27. Thompson B. Fallacies. Archived April 20, 2006, at the Wayback Machine
  28. Sicherer, Scott H. (1999). "Food allergy: When and how to perform oral food challenges". Pediatric Allergy and Immunology. 10 (4): 226–234. doi:10.1034/j.1399-3038.1999.00040.x. PMID 10678717. S2CID 1484234.
  29. "Evaluating Treatment Products". MedicineNet.
  30. "The Judicial Learning Center". 10 August 2012.
  31. "Benton, Ross, Bradshaw, Thomas, & Bradshaw, 2006".
Common fallacies (list)
Formal
In propositional logic
In quantificational logic
Syllogistic fallacy
Informal
Equivocation
Question-begging
Correlative-based
Illicit transference
Secundum quid
Faulty generalization
Ambiguity
Questionable cause
Appeals
Consequences
Emotion
Genetic fallacy
Ad hominem
Other fallacies
of relevance
Arguments
Categories: