Misplaced Pages

Concealed ovulation: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 09:23, 18 August 2011 editMiradre (talk | contribs)9,214 edits important ep topic← Previous edit Latest revision as of 13:56, 29 October 2024 edit undo2601:cf:8200:4330:9485:a892:e85a:3254 (talk) Edited for concisenessTags: Mobile edit Mobile web edit 
(208 intermediate revisions by 96 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Lack of visible changes in ovulating females}}
{{Inappropriate tone|date=April 2011}}
'''Concealed ovulation''' or '''hidden estrus''' is the lack of distinctive signaling that the adult female of a species is "]". These signals may include swelling and redness of the genitalia in ] and bonobos '']'', ] release in the feline family, etc. By comparison, the females of ]s and a few other species<!-- '''Concealed ovulation''' or '''hidden estrus''' is the lack of any perceptible change (e.g., a change in appearance or scent) when an adult female is fertile and near ]. Some examples of perceptible changes are ] in ]s and ]s, and ] release in the feline family. In contrast, the females of ]s and a few other species<ref>{{cite journal | author=Sandy J. Andelman | title = Evolution of Concealed Ovulation in Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) | journal = The American Naturalist | volume = 129 | pages = 785–799 |date=June 1987 | doi = 10.1086/284675 | issue=6| s2cid = 83522515 }}</ref> that undergo hidden estrus have few external signs of ], making it difficult for a mate to consciously deduce, by means of external signs only, whether or not a female is near ovulation.
] in a female ] indicates she is ovulating. Unlike other primates, female humans show no obvious outward signs of ovulation.]]
--><ref>{{cite journal | author=Sandy J. Andelman | title = Evolution of Concealed Ovulation in Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) | journal = The American Naturalist | volume = 129 | pages = 785–799 | month = June | year = 1987 | doi = 10.1086/284675}}</ref> have few external signs of ], making it difficult to tell, by means of external signs only, whether or not a female is near ].


== Concealed ovulation in women == == Human females ==


In humans, an adult woman's fertility peaks for a few days during each roughly monthly cycle. The frequency and length of fertility (the time when a woman can become pregnant) is highly variable between women, and can slightly change for each woman over the course of her lifespan. Humans are considered to have concealed ovulation because there is no outward physiological sign, either to a woman herself or to others, that ovulation, or biological fertility, is occurring. Knowledge of the fertility cycle, learned through experience or from educational sources, can allow a woman to estimate her own level of fertility at a given time (]). Whether other humans, potential reproductive partners in particular, can detect fertility in women through behavioral or invisible biological cues is highly debated. Scientists and laypersons are interested in this question because it has implications for human social behavior, and could theoretically offer biological explanations for some human sexual behavior. However, the science here is weak, due to a relatively small number of studies.
While women can be taught to recognize their own level of fertility (]), whether men can detect fertility in women is highly debated. Several small studies have found that fertile women (compared to women in infertile portions of the menstrual cycle or on hormonal contraception) appear more attractive.<ref name="Roberts et al 2004">{{cite journal | author=S.C. Roberts, J. Havlicek, J. Flegr, M. Hruskova, A.C. Little, B.C. Jones, D.I. Perrett and M. Petrie | title = Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle | journal = Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B (Suppl.) | volume = 271 | pages = S270–S272 | month = August | year = 2004 | doi = 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0174 | pmid=15503991 | pmc=1810066}}</ref><ref name="Miller et al 2007">{{cite journal | author = Geoffrey Miller, Joshua M. Tybur and Brent D. Jordan | title = Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap dancers: economic evidence for human estrous? | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 28 | issue = 6 | pages = 375–381 | month = June | year = 2007 | url = http://www.unm.edu/~gfmiller/cycle_effects_on_tips.pdf | accessdate = 2008-01-21 | doi = 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.06.002 |format=PDF}}</ref> It has also been suggested that a woman's voice may become more attractive to males during this time.<ref name="Pipitone and Gallup 2008">{{cite journal | last = Pipitone | first = R. | coauthors = G . Gallup Jr | title = Women's voice attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 29 | issue = 4 | pages = 268–274 | date = 2008-05-18 | doi = 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.02.001}}</ref> Two small studies of monogamous couples found that women initiated sex significantly more frequently when fertile, but male-initiated sex occurred at a constant rate in all phases of the menstrual cycle.<ref name="Bullivant et al 2004">{{cite journal | author= Susan B. Bullivant, Sarah A. Sellergren, Kathleen Stern, et al. | title= Women's sexual experience during the menstrual cycle: identification of the sexual phase by noninvasive measurement of luteinizing hormone | journal=Journal of Sex Research | month=February | year=2004 | volume=41 | issue=1 | pages=82–93 | url=http://www.findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_m2372/is_1_41/ai_n6032944 | pmid=15216427 | doi= 10.1080/00224490409552216}}</ref>


Several small studies have found that fertile women appear more attractive to men than women during infertile portions of her menstrual cycle, or women using ].<ref name="Roberts et al 2004">{{cite journal |author1=S. C. Roberts |author2=J. Havlicek |author3=J. Flegr |author4=M. Hruskova |author5=A. C. Little |author6=B. C. Jones |author7=D. I. Perrett |author8=M. Petrie | title = Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle | journal = ] | volume = 271 | pages = S270–S272 |date=August 2004 | doi = 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0174 | pmid=15503991 | pmc=1810066 | issue=Suppl 5}}</ref><ref name="Miller et al 2007">{{cite journal |author1=Geoffrey Miller |author2=Joshua M. Tybur |author3=Brent D. Jordan | title = Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap dancers: economic evidence for human estrous? | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 28 | issue = 6 | pages = 375–381 |date=June 2007 | url = http://www.unm.edu/~gfmiller/cycle_effects_on_tips.pdf | access-date = 2008-01-21 | doi = 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.06.002 |citeseerx=10.1.1.154.8176 }}</ref> It has also been suggested that a woman's voice may become more attractive to men during this time.<ref name="Pipitone and Gallup 2008">{{cite journal | last = Pipitone | first = R. |author2=G . Gallup Jr | title = Women's voice attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 29 | issue = 4 | pages = 268–274 | date = 2008-05-18 | doi = 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.02.001}}</ref> Two small studies of ] human couples found that women initiated sex significantly more frequently when fertile, but male-initiated sex occurred at a constant rate, without regard to the woman's phase of menstrual cycle.<ref name="Bullivant et al 2004">{{cite journal|author=Susan B. Bullivant |title=Women's sexual experience during the menstrual cycle: identification of the sexual phase by noninvasive measurement of luteinizing hormone |journal=Journal of Sex Research |date=February 2004 |volume=41 |issue=1 |pages=82–93 |url=https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490409552216 |archive-url=https://archive.today/20070728011531/http://www.findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_m2372/is_1_41/ai_n6032944 |url-status=dead |archive-date=2007-07-28 |pmid=15216427 |doi=10.1080/00224490409552216 |author2=Sarah A. Sellergren |author3=Kathleen Stern |display-authors=3 |last4=Spencer |first4=Natasha A. |last5=Jacob |first5=Suma |last6=Mennella |first6=Julie A. |last7=McClintock |first7=Martha K. |s2cid=40401379 }}</ref> It may be that a woman's awareness of men's courtship signals<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Ann Renninger | first1 = Lee | last2 = Wade | first2 = T. Joel | last3 = Grammer | first3 = Karl | year = 2004 | title = Getting that female glance: Patterns and consequences of male nonverbal behavior in courtship contexts | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 25 | issue = 6| pages = 416–431 | doi=10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.006}}</ref> increases during her highly fertile phase due to an enhanced olfactory awareness of chemicals specifically found in men's body odor.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Thornhill | first1 = Randy | last2 = Gangestad | first2 = Steven W. | year = 1999 | title = The Scent of Symmetry: A Human Sex Pheromone that Signals Fitness? | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 20 | issue = 3| pages = 175–201 | doi=10.1016/s1090-5138(99)00005-7}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Brooksbank | first1 = B. W. L. | year = 1962 | title = Urinary excretion of androst-16-en-3 alpha-ol in human male axillary sweat | doi = 10.1007/bf01938327 | journal = Experientia | volume = 30 | issue = 8| pages = 864–865 | pmid = 4416149 | s2cid = 44558005 }}</ref>
Analyses of data provided by the post-1998 U.S. Demographic and Health Surveys found no variation in the occurrence of coitus in the menstrual phases except during menses.<ref name="Brewis and Meyer 2005">{{Cite doi|10.1086/430016}}</ref> These findings of differences in female-initiated sex versus male-initiated sex are likely caused by the female’s awareness of her ovulation cycle (because of hormone changes) but the male’s inability to detect ovulation because of its being “hidden.”


Analyses of data provided by the post-1998 U.S. Demographic and Health Surveys found no variation in the occurrence of coitus in the menstrual phases (except during menstruation itself).<ref name="Brewis and Meyer 2005">{{Cite journal | last1 = Brewis | first1 = A. | last2 = Meyer | first2 = M. | doi = 10.1086/430016 | title = Demographic Evidence That Human Ovulation is Undetectable (at Least in Pair Bonds) | journal = Current Anthropology | volume = 46 | issue = 3 | pages = 465–471 | year = 2005 | s2cid = 30243603 }}</ref> This is contrary to other studies, which have found female sexual desire and ]s (EPCs) to increase during the midfollicular to ovulatory phases (that is, the highly fertile phase).<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Regan | first1 = P. C. | year = 1996 | title = Rhythms of desire: the association between menstrual cycle phases and female sexual desire | journal = Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality | volume = 5 | pages = 145–156 }}</ref> These findings of differences in woman-initiated versus man-initiated sex are likely caused by the woman's subconscious awareness of her ovulation cycle (because of hormonal changes causing her to feel increased sexual desire), contrasting with the man's inability to detect ovulation because of its being "hidden".{{Citation needed|date=May 2022}}
In 2008, researchers announced the discovery of hormones related to ovulation in human semen. They theorized that ], ], and ] may encourage ovulation in women exposed to semen. These hormones are not found in the semen of chimpanzees, suggesting that this phenomenon may be a male counter-strategy to concealed ovulation in human females. Other researchers are skeptical that the low levels of hormones found in semen could have any effect on ovulation.<ref name="Mothluk 2006">{{cite journal | last = Motluk | first = Alison | title = The secret life of semen | journal = | issue = 2563 | date = 2006-08-05 | url = http://www.eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2006-08/ns-tsl080206.php | accessdate = 2008-06-28}}</ref> One group of authors has theorized that concealed ovulation and menstruation were key factors in the development of ] in early human society.<ref name="Knight 1991">{{cite book | author = Chris Knight | title = Blood relations: menstruation and the origins of culture | publisher = Yale University Press | location = New Haven, Conn | year = 1991 | isbn = 0-300-04911-0}}</ref><ref name="Knight 1995">{{cite journal | last = Knight | first = Chris | coauthors = Camilla Power & Ian Watts | title = The Human Symbolic Revolution: A Darwinian Account | journal = Cambridge Archaeological Journal | volume = 5 | issue = 1 | pages = 75–114 | year = 1995 | url = http://www.radicalanthropologygroup.org/pub_knight_power_watts_big.pdf | accessdate = 2006-12-13 |format=PDF | doi = 10.1017/S0959774300001190}}</ref>
In 2008, researchers announced the discovery in human semen of hormones usually found in ovulating women. They theorized that ], ], and ] may encourage ovulation in women exposed to semen. These hormones are not found in the semen of chimpanzees, suggesting this phenomenon may be a human male counter-strategy to concealed ovulation in human females. Other researchers are skeptical that the low levels of hormones found in semen could have any effect on ovulation.<ref name="Mothluk 2006">{{cite journal | last = Motluk | first = Alison | title = The secret life of semen | journal = New Scientist | issue = 2563 | date = 2006-08-05 | url = http://www.eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2006-08/ns-tsl080206.php | access-date = 2008-06-28}}</ref> One group of authors has theorized that concealed ovulation and menstruation were key factors in the development of ] in early human society.<ref name="Knight 1991">{{cite book | author = Chris Knight | title = Blood relations: menstruation and the origins of culture | publisher = Yale University Press | location = New Haven, Conn | year = 1991 | isbn = 978-0-300-04911-4}}</ref><ref name="Knight 1995">{{cite journal | last = Knight | first = Chris | author2 = Camilla Power | author3 = Ian Watts | title = The Human Symbolic Revolution: A Darwinian Account | journal = Cambridge Archaeological Journal | volume = 5 | issue = 1 | pages = 75–114 | year = 1995 | url = http://www.radicalanthropologygroup.org/pub_knight_power_watts_big.pdf | access-date = 2006-12-13 | doi = 10.1017/S0959774300001190 | s2cid = 54701302 | archive-date = 2020-08-01 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20200801052656/http://radicalanthropologygroup.org/sites/default/files/pdf/pub_knight_power_watts_big.pdf | url-status = dead }}</ref>


=== Evolutionary hypotheses === ==Evolutionary hypotheses==


There are many questions concerning concealed ovulation, specifically why and when it occurred. ] retains traits that increase the fitness of the individual, meaning that the traits of those individuals who reproduce the most viable offspring will be selected for and maintained in the species. The following hypotheses thus try to examine the adaptive advantages for the evolution of concealed ovulation in human females. It could be that the lack of signaling in some species is a retained ancestral trait, not something that existed previously and later disappeared. If signaling is supposed to have existed and was lost, then it could have been merely due to reduced adaptive importance and lessened selection,<ref name="Burt 1992">{{cite journal | author=Burt, Austin | title = 'Concealed ovulation' and sexual signals in primates. | journal = Folia Primatologica | volume = 58 | pages = 1–6 | month = June | year = 1992 | doi = 10.1159/000156600}}</ref> or due to direct adaptive advantages for the concealment of ovulation. Yet another possibility, specifically for humans, is that while highly specific signaling of the ovulation is absent, the female form evolved to mimic permanent signaling of fertility.<ref name="Szalay and Costello 1991">{{cite journal | author=Frederick S. Szalay and Robert K.Costello | title = Evolution of permanent estrus displays in hominids | journal = Journal of Human Evolution | volume = 20 | pages = 439–464 | month = June | year = 1991 | doi = 10.1016/0047-2484(91)90019-R}}</ref> There are several hypotheses that aim toward answering these questions. It may be possible for elements from multiple hypotheses to be true.<ref name="Schroder 1993">{{Cite doi|10.1016/0162-3095(93)90026-E}}</ref> ] have advanced a number of different possible explanations for concealed ovulation.<ref name="Schroder 1993">{{Cite journal | last1 = Schoroder | first1 = I. | title = Concealed ovulation and clandestine copulation: A female contribution to human evolution | doi = 10.1016/0162-3095(93)90026-E | journal = Ethology and Sociobiology | volume = 14 | issue = 6 | pages = 381–389 | year = 1993 }}</ref> Some posit that the lack of signaling in some species is a trait retained from evolutionary ancestors, not something that existed previously and later disappeared. If signaling is supposed to have existed and was lost, then it could have been merely due to reduced adaptive importance and lessened selection,<ref name="Burt 1992">{{cite journal | author=Burt, Austin | title = 'Concealed ovulation' and sexual signals in primates | journal = Folia Primatologica | volume = 58 | issue = 1 | pages = 1–6 |date=June 1992 | doi = 10.1159/000156600| pmid = 1618432 }}</ref> or due to direct adaptive advantages for the concealment of ovulation. Yet another possibility (regarding humans specifically) is that while highly specific signaling of ovulation is absent, human female anatomy evolved to mimic permanent signaling of fertility.<ref name="Szalay and Costello 1991">{{cite journal |author1=Frederick S. Szalay |author2=Robert K.Costello | title = Evolution of permanent estrus displays in hominids | journal = Journal of Human Evolution | volume = 20 | pages = 439–464 |date=June 1991 | doi = 10.1016/0047-2484(91)90019-R | issue=6}}</ref>


====Paternal Investment Hypothesis==== ===Paternal investment hypothesis===


This hypothesis is strongly supported by many evolutionary biologists.<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> Several hypotheses regarding human evolution integrate the idea that human females increasingly required supplemental ] in their offspring. The shared reliance on this idea across several hypotheses concerning human evolution increases its significance in terms of this specific phenomenon. The ] hypothesis is strongly supported by many evolutionary biologists.<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> Several hypotheses regarding human evolution integrate the idea that women increasingly required supplemental paternal investment in their offspring. The shared reliance on this idea across several hypotheses concerning human evolution increases its significance in terms of this specific phenomenon.


This hypothesis suggests that human females concealed ovulation in order to obtain male aid in rearing offspring. Schroder<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> summarizes this hypothesis outlined in Alexander and Noonan’s 1979 paper.{{Citation needed|date=April 2011}} If human females no longer signaled the time of ovulation, males would be unable to detect the exact period in which they were fecund. This led to a change in their mating strategy; rather than seeking multiple female partners and mating with them hoping that they were fecund during that period, males instead chose to mate with a particular female multiple times throughout her menstrual cycle. A mating would be successful in resulting in conception when it occurred during ovulation, and thus, frequent matings, necessitated by the effects of concealed ovulation, would be most successful. This hypothesis suggests that women concealed ovulation to obtain men's aid in rearing offspring. Schröder<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> summarizes this hypothesis outlined in Alexander and Noonan's 1979 paper: if women no longer signaled the time of ovulation, men would be unable to detect the exact period in which they were fecund. This led to a change in men's mating strategy: rather than mating with multiple women in the hope that some of them, at least, were fecund during that period, men instead chose to mate with a particular woman repeatedly throughout her menstrual cycle. A mating would be successful in resulting in conception when it occurred during ovulation, and thus, frequent matings, necessitated by the effects of concealed ovulation, would be most evolutionarily successful. A similar ] was proposed by Lovejoy in 1981 that argued that concealed ovulation, reduced canines and ] evolved from a reproductive strategy where males provisioned food resources to his paired female and dependent offspring.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Lovejoy|first=C. Owen|date=1981-01-23|title=The Origin of Man|journal=Science|language=en|volume=211|issue=4480|pages=341–350|doi=10.1126/science.211.4480.341|issn=0036-8075|pmid=17748254|bibcode=1981Sci...211..341L}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Lovejoy|first=C. Owen|date=2009-10-02|title=Reexamining Human Origins in Light of Ardipithecus ramidus|journal=Science|language=en|volume=326|issue=5949|pages=74–74e8|doi=10.1126/science.1175834|issn=0036-8075|url=http://doc.rero.ch/record/211449/files/PAL_E4439.pdf|pmid=19810200|bibcode=2009Sci...326...74L|s2cid=42790876}}</ref>


Continuous female sexual receptivity suggests that human sexuality is not solely defined by reproduction; a large part of it revolves around conjugal love and communication between partners. Copulations between partners while the female is pregnant or in the infertile period of her menstrual cycle do not achieve the base purpose of sex – conception but do strengthen the bond between these partners. Therefore, the increased copulations because of concealed ovulation are thought to have played a role in fostering pair bonds in humans.<ref name="Benagiano and Mori 2009">{{Cite pmid|19281665}}</ref> ] suggests human sexuality is not solely defined by reproduction; a large part of it revolves around conjugal love and communication between partners. Copulations between partners while the woman is pregnant or in the infertile period of her menstrual cycle do not achieve conception, but do strengthen the bond between these partners. Therefore, the increased frequency of copulations due to concealed ovulation are thought to have played a role in fostering ]s in humans.<ref name="Benagiano and Mori 2009">{{Cite journal
| last1 = Benagiano | first1 = G.
| last2 = Mori | first2 = M.
| title = The origins of human sexuality: Procreation or recreation?
| journal = Reproductive Biomedicine Online
| volume = 18
| pages = 50–59
| year = 2009
| issue = Suppl 1
| pmid = 19281665
| doi=10.1016/s1472-6483(10)60116-2
}}</ref>


The pair bond would be very advantageous to the reproductive ] of both partners throughout the period of pregnancy, lactation, and rearing of offspring. Pregnancy and lactation require vast amounts of energy on the part of the female, necessitating a large amount of energy intake in the form of food. However, during these periods, the female’s foraging ability would be greatly hindered because of constraints placed upon her by the pregnancy itself or the amount of time tending to or minding the offspring. Supplemental male investment in the mother and her offspring is advantageous to all parties. While the male is supplementing the female’s limited foraging intakes, she can devote the necessary time to the care of her offspring. The offspring benefits from the supplemental investment, in the form of food and defense from the father, and receives the full attention and resources of the mother. Through this shared parental investment, both male and female would increase their offspring’s chances for survival, thereby increasing their reproductive fitness. This increased reproductive fitness is the key to natural selection favoring the establishment of pair bonds in humans and since pair bonds are thought to have been strengthened by concealed ovulation, this must have been under selective pressure. The pair bond would be very advantageous to the ] of both partners throughout the period of pregnancy, lactation, and rearing of offspring. Pregnancy, lactation and caring for post-lactation offspring require vast amounts of energy and time on the part of the woman. She must at first consume more food, then provide food to her offspring, while her ability to forage is reduced throughout. Supplemental male investment in the mother and her offspring is advantageous to all parties. While the man supplements the woman's limited gathered food, the woman is enabled to devote the necessary time and energy to the care of their offspring. The offspring benefits from the supplemental investment, in the form of food and defense from the father, and receives the full attention and resources of the mother. Through this shared parental investment, both man and woman would increase their offspring's chances for survival, thereby increasing their reproductive fitness. In this way, natural selection would favor the establishment of pair bonds in humans. To the extent that concealed ovulation strengthened pair bonding, selective pressure would favor concealed ovulation as well.


Another, more recent, hypothesis is that concealed ovulation is an adaptation in response to a promiscuous mating system, similar to that of our closest evolutionary relatives, ] and ]. The theory is that concealed ovulation evolved in women to lessen paternity certainty, which would both lessen the chances of infanticide (as a father is less likely to kill offspring that might be his), and potentially increase the number of men motivated to assist her in caring for her offspring (]). This is supported by the fact that all other mammals with concealed ovulation, such as dolphins and ]s, are promiscuous, and that the only other ape species that have multi-male communities, as humans do, are promiscuous. It is argued that evidence such as the ], showing that a man does not seem to be naturally geared towards sexual mate-guarding behavior (that is, preventing other males from having access to his sexual partner), supports the conclusion that sexual monogamy (though perhaps not ] and/or pair bonding) was rare in early modern humans.<ref name="Ryan">{{cite book|author=Christopher Ryan Ph.D|author2=Cacilda Jethá Ph.D|name-list-style=amp|title=Sex at Dawn|isbn =978-0-06-220794-4 |publisher=HarperCollins|year=2012}}</ref>
====Reduced Infanticide Hypothesis====
This hypothesis suggests that the adaptive advantage for females that had hidden estrous would be a reduction in the possibility of infanticide by males, as they would be unable to selectively kill their rivals’ offspring.<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> This hypothesis shows support in the recent studies on wild ] that display concealed ovulation and frequent matings with males outside their fertile ovulatory period.<ref name="Heisterman et al 2001">{{Cite doi|10.1098/rspb.2001.1833}}</ref> Heistermann et al. hypothesize that concealed ovulation is used by females to confuse paternity and thus reduce infanticide. He explains that since males determine paternity and thus decide on whether to kill the female’s child based on his previous matings with that female, a female’s promiscuous matings in conjunction with concealed ovulation would lead a male to believe that there is still a possibility for that child to be his own.


===Reduced infanticide hypothesis===
====Sex and Reward Hypothesis====
This hypothesis suggests the adaptive advantage for women who had hidden estrus would be a reduction in the possibility of ] by men, as they would be unable to reliably identify, and kill, their rivals' offspring.<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> This hypothesis is supported by recent studies of wild ], documenting concealed ovulation, and frequent matings with males outside their fertile ovulatory period.<ref name="Heisterman et al 2001">{{Cite journal | last1 = Hestermann | first1 = M. | last2 = Ziegler | first2 = T. | last3 = Van Schaik | first3 = C. P. | last4 = Launhardt | first4 = K. | last5 = Winkler | first5 = P. | last6 = Hodges | first6 = J. K. | doi = 10.1098/rspb.2001.1833 | title = Loss of oestrus, concealed ovulation and paternity confusion in free-ranging Hanuman langurs | journal = Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences | volume = 268 | issue = 1484 | pages = 2445–2451 | year = 2001 | pmid = 11747562| pmc = 1088898}}</ref> Heistermann et al. hypothesize that concealed ovulation is used by women to confuse paternity and thus reduce ]. He explains that as ovulation is always concealed in women, men can only determine paternity (and thus decide on whether to kill the woman's child) probabilistically, based on his previous mating frequency with her, and so he would be unable to escape the possibility that the child might be his own, even if he were aware of promiscuous matings on the woman's part.


===Sex and reward hypothesis===
Schroder<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> reviews Symons'{{Citation needed|date=April 2011}} and Hill's{{Citation needed|date=April 2011}} hypothesis that states that hunting males exchanged meat for sex with females. Females that continuously mimicked estrus may have benefited from more meat than those that did not. If this occurred with enough frequency, then a definite period of estrus would have been lost, and, with it, sexual signaling specific to ovulation would have disappeared.


Schröder<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> reviews a hypothesis by Symons and Hill, that after hunting, men exchanged meat for sex with women. Women who continuously mimicked estrus may have benefited from more meat than those that did not. If this occurred with enough frequency, then a definite period of estrus would have been lost, and with it sexual signaling specific to ovulation would have disappeared.
====Social-Bonding Hypothesis====


===Social-bonding hypothesis===
Schroder<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> presents the idea that there was a “gradual diminution of mid-cycle estrous and concomitant continuous sexual receptivity in human women” because it facilitated orderly social relationships throughout the menstrual cycle by eliminating the periodic intensification of male-male aggressiveness in competition for mates.<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> It has been said that the extended estrous period of the bonobo (reproductive-age females are in heat for 75% of their menstrual cycle) has a similar effect to the lack of a "heat" in human females. While concealed human ovulation may have evolved in this fashion of extending estrous until it was no longer a distinct period, as paralleled in the bonobo, this hypothesis for why concealed ovulation evolved has frequently been rejected. Schroder outlines the two objections to this hypothesis: (1) Natural selection would need to work at a level above the individual, which is difficult to prove. (2) Selection, because it acts on the individuals with the most reproductive success, would thus favor greater reproductive success over social integration at the expense of reproductive success.


Schröder<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> presents the idea of a "gradual diminution of mid-cycle estrus and concomitant continuous sexual receptivity in human women" because it facilitated orderly social relationships throughout the menstrual cycle by eliminating the periodic intensification of male–male aggressiveness in competition for mates.<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> The extended estrous period of the bonobo (reproductive-age females are in heat for 75% of their menstrual cycle) has been said to have a similar effect to the lack of a "heat" in women. While concealed human ovulation may have evolved in this fashion, extending estrus until it was no longer a distinct period, as paralleled in the bonobo, this theory of why concealed ovulation evolved has frequently been rejected. Schröder outlines the two objections to this hypothesis: (1) natural selection would need to work at a level above the individual, which is difficult to prove; and (2) selection, because it acts on the individuals with the most reproductive success, would thus favor greater reproductive success over social integration at the expense of reproductive success.
====Cuckoldry Hypothesis====


However, since 1993 when that was written, group selection models have seen a resurgence.<ref name="Koeslag, 1997">{{cite journal | last1 = Koeslag | first1 = J. H. | year = 1997 | title = Sex, the prisoner's dilemma game, and the evolutionary inevitability of cooperation | journal = J. Theor. Biol. | volume = 189 | issue = 1| pages = 53–61 | doi=10.1006/jtbi.1997.0496 | pmid=9398503| bibcode = 1997JThBi.189...53K }}</ref><ref name= "Koeslag, 2003">{{cite journal | last1 = Koeslag | first1 = J. H. | year = 2003 | title = Evolution of cooperation: cooperation defeats defection in the cornfield model | journal = J. Theor. Biol. | volume = 224 | issue = 3 | pages = 399–410 | doi = 10.1016/s0022-5193(03)00188-7 | pmid = 12941597 | bibcode = 2003JThBi.224..399K }}</ref><ref name="Wilson, D. S. 2008">{{cite journal | last1 = Wilson | first1 = D. S. | last2 = Wilson | first2 = E. O. | year = 2008 | title = Evolution 'for the good of the group'. | journal = American Scientist | volume = 96 | issue = 5| pages = 380–389 | doi=10.1511/2008.74.1}}</ref> (See ], ], and ].)
Benshoof and Thornhill{{Citation needed|date=April 2011}} hypothesized that estrous became hidden after monogamous relationships became the norm in Homo erectus.<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> Concealed ovulation allowed the female to mate with genetically superior males and thus gain the benefit of their genes for her offspring while still maintaining the pair bond with her lesser male partner. The bonded male would have little reason to doubt her fidelity because of the concealed ovulation and would have high, albeit unfounded, paternity confidence in his offspring. His confidence would encourage him to invest in the child even though it was not his own. Again, the idea of paternal investment being vital to the offspring’s survival, and thus vital to the male and female partner’s fitness is a central fixture of a hypothesis regarding concealed ovulation.


===Cuckoldry hypothesis===
====Concealed Ovulation as a Side Effect of Bipedalism====
Pawlowski<ref name="Pawlowski 1999">{{Cite doi|10.1086/200017}}</ref> presents the importance of ] to the mechanics and necessity of ovulation signaling. The more open savannah environment inhabited by early humans (as made available by bipedalism) brought greater danger from predators. This would have caused humans to live in more dense groups, and, in such a scenario, the long-distance sexual signaling provided by female genital swellings would have lost its function. Concealed ovulation is thus argued to be a loss of function evolutionary change rather than an adaptation. ] systems were also modified in humans with the move to the ] in order to conserve water. It is thought that female genital swellings would have incurred added cost because of ineffective evaporation of water from the area. Pawlowski continues by saying that the change to bipedalism in early hominins changed both the position of female genitals and the line of vision of males. Since males could no longer constantly see the female genitals, swelling of them during estrous as a mode of signaling would have become useless. Also, anogenital swelling at each ovulatory period may have interfered with the mechanics of bipedal locomotion, and selection may have favored females who were less hindered by this occurrence. This hypothesis ultimately concludes that bipedalism, which was strongly selected for, caused the physiological changes and a loss of function of sexual signaling through female genital swelling, leading to the concealed ovulation we now observe.


Schröder in his review writes that Benshoof and Thornhill hypothesized that estrus became hidden after monogamous relationships became the norm in '']''.<ref name="Schroder 1993" /> Concealed ovulation allowed the woman to mate secretly at times with a genetically superior man, and thus gain the benefit of his genes for her offspring, while still retaining the benefits of the pair bond with her usual sexual partner. Her usual sexual partner would have little reason to doubt her fidelity, because of the concealed ovulation, and would have high, albeit unfounded, paternity confidence in her offspring. His confidence would encourage him to invest his time and energy in assisting her to care for the child, even though it was not his own. Again, the idea of a man's investment being vital to the child's survival is a central fixture of a hypothesis regarding concealed ovulation, even as the evolutionary benefits accrue to the child, the woman, and her clandestine partner, and not to her regular sexual partner.
Pawlowski’s paper offers views that differ from the other hypotheses regarding concealed ovulation in that it pinpoints physiological changes in early humans as the cause of concealed ovulation rather than social or behavioral ones.<ref name="Pawlowski 1999" /> One of the strengths of this is derived from the other hypotheses’ weakness – it is difficult to track the evolution of a behavior as it leaves no verifiable evidence in the form of bone or DNA. However, the fact that the Hanuman langurs also display some concealed ovulation and that it is not directly caused by a physiological change to bipedalism may suggest that bipedalism was not, at least the sole, cause of concealed ovulation in humans. As stated earlier, it is possible for many elements of different hypotheses to be true regarding the selective pressures for concealed ovulation in humans.

==As a side effect of bipedalism==
Pawlowski<ref name="Pawlowski 1999">{{Cite journal | last1 = Pawłowski | first1 = B. | title = Loss of Oestrus and Concealed Ovulation in Human Evolution: The Case against the Sexual-Selection Hypothesis | doi = 10.1086/200017 | journal = Current Anthropology | volume = 40 | issue = 3 | pages = 257–276 | year = 1999 | s2cid = 85884654 }}</ref> presents the importance of ] to the mechanics and necessity of ovulation signaling. The more open savannah environment inhabited by early humans brought greater danger from predators. This would have caused humans to live in denser groups, and, in such a scenario, the long-distance sexual signaling provided by female genital swellings would have lost its function. Concealed ovulation is thus argued to be a loss of function evolutionary change rather than an adaptation. ] systems were also modified in humans with the move to the ]{{clarify|date=June 2015}} to conserve water. It is thought that female genital swellings would have incurred added cost because of ineffective evaporation of water from the area. Pawlowski continues by saying the change to bipedalism in early hominins changed both the position of female genitals and the line of vision of males. Since males could no longer constantly see the female genitals, swelling of them during estrus as a mode of signaling would have become useless. Also, anogenital swelling at each ovulatory period may have interfered with the mechanics of bipedal locomotion, and selection may have favored females who were less hindered by this occurrence. This hypothesis ultimately concludes that bipedalism, which was strongly selected for, caused the physiological changes and a loss of function of sexual signaling through female genital swelling, leading to the concealed ovulation we now observe.

Pawlowski's paper offers views that differ from the other hypotheses regarding concealed ovulation in that it pinpoints physiological changes in early humans as the cause of concealed ovulation rather than social or behavioral ones.<ref name="Pawlowski 1999" /> One of the strengths of this is derived from the other hypotheses' weaknesses – it is difficult to track the evolution of a behavior as it leaves no verifiable evidence in the form of bone or DNA. However, the fact that the ]s also display some concealed ovulation and that it is not directly caused by a physiological change to bipedalism may suggest bipedalism was not, at least, the sole cause of concealed ovulation in humans. As stated earlier, it is possible for many elements of different hypotheses to be true regarding the selective pressures for concealed ovulation in humans.


==See also== ==See also==
*]
*]
*] *]
*'']''
*]
*] *'']''
*]


==References== ==References==
Line 58: Line 72:
==Further reading== ==Further reading==


* {{cite journal | last = Marlowe | first = F.W. | title = Is human ovulation concealed? Evidence from conception beliefs in a hunter-gatherer society | journal = Archives of Sexual Behavior | volume = 33 | issue = 5 | pages = 427–432 | month = October | year = 2004 | url = http://www.fas.harvard.edu/~hbe-lab/acrobatfiles/is%20ovulation%20concealed.pdf | accessdate = 2007-10-11 | doi = 10.1023/B:ASEB.0000037423.84026.1f |format=PDF | pmid = 15305113}} * {{cite journal|last=Marlowe |first=F.W. |title=Is human ovulation concealed? Evidence from conception beliefs in a hunter-gatherer society |journal=Archives of Sexual Behavior |volume=33 |issue=5 |pages=427–432 |date=October 2004 |url=http://www.fas.harvard.edu/~hbe-lab/acrobatfiles/is%20ovulation%20concealed.pdf |access-date=2007-10-11 |doi=10.1023/B:ASEB.0000037423.84026.1f |pmid=15305113 |s2cid=14817144 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080821223420/http://www.fas.harvard.edu/~hbe-lab/acrobatfiles/is%20ovulation%20concealed.pdf |archive-date=2008-08-21 }}
* <ref>{{Cite journal|last=Burt|first=A.|date=1992|title='Concealed ovulation' and sexual signals in primates|url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/1618432/|journal=Folia Primatologica; International Journal of Primatology|volume=58|issue=1|pages=1–6|doi=10.1159/000156600|issn=0015-5713|pmid=1618432}}</ref>
* {{cite journal | last = Haselton | first = Martie G. | title = Ovulatory shifts in human female ornamentation: Near ovulation, women dress to impress | journal = Hormones and Behavior | volume = 51 | issue = 1 | pages = 40–45 | month = January | year = 2007 | url = http://www.debralieberman.com/downloads/courses/650/haselton_et_al_2007.pdf | accessdate = 2008-01-21 | doi = 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.07.007 |format=PDF | pmid = 17045994 | last2 = Mortezaie | first2 = M | last3 = Pillsworth | first3 = EG | last4 = Bleske-Rechek | first4 = A | last5 = Frederick | first5 = DA}}{{Dead link|date=October 2009}}

{{Evolutionary psychology}}


{{DEFAULTSORT:Concealed Ovulation}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Concealed Ovulation}}
Line 65: Line 81:
] ]
] ]

]

Latest revision as of 13:56, 29 October 2024

Lack of visible changes in ovulating females

Concealed ovulation or hidden estrus is the lack of any perceptible change (e.g., a change in appearance or scent) when an adult female is fertile and near ovulation. Some examples of perceptible changes are swelling and redness of the vulva in baboons and bonobos, and pheromone release in the feline family. In contrast, the females of humans and a few other species that undergo hidden estrus have few external signs of fecundity, making it difficult for a mate to consciously deduce, by means of external signs only, whether or not a female is near ovulation.

Sexual swelling in a female hamadryas baboon indicates she is ovulating. Unlike other primates, female humans show no obvious outward signs of ovulation.

Human females

In humans, an adult woman's fertility peaks for a few days during each roughly monthly cycle. The frequency and length of fertility (the time when a woman can become pregnant) is highly variable between women, and can slightly change for each woman over the course of her lifespan. Humans are considered to have concealed ovulation because there is no outward physiological sign, either to a woman herself or to others, that ovulation, or biological fertility, is occurring. Knowledge of the fertility cycle, learned through experience or from educational sources, can allow a woman to estimate her own level of fertility at a given time (fertility awareness). Whether other humans, potential reproductive partners in particular, can detect fertility in women through behavioral or invisible biological cues is highly debated. Scientists and laypersons are interested in this question because it has implications for human social behavior, and could theoretically offer biological explanations for some human sexual behavior. However, the science here is weak, due to a relatively small number of studies.

Several small studies have found that fertile women appear more attractive to men than women during infertile portions of her menstrual cycle, or women using hormonal contraception. It has also been suggested that a woman's voice may become more attractive to men during this time. Two small studies of monogamous human couples found that women initiated sex significantly more frequently when fertile, but male-initiated sex occurred at a constant rate, without regard to the woman's phase of menstrual cycle. It may be that a woman's awareness of men's courtship signals increases during her highly fertile phase due to an enhanced olfactory awareness of chemicals specifically found in men's body odor.

Analyses of data provided by the post-1998 U.S. Demographic and Health Surveys found no variation in the occurrence of coitus in the menstrual phases (except during menstruation itself). This is contrary to other studies, which have found female sexual desire and extra-pair copulations (EPCs) to increase during the midfollicular to ovulatory phases (that is, the highly fertile phase). These findings of differences in woman-initiated versus man-initiated sex are likely caused by the woman's subconscious awareness of her ovulation cycle (because of hormonal changes causing her to feel increased sexual desire), contrasting with the man's inability to detect ovulation because of its being "hidden".

In 2008, researchers announced the discovery in human semen of hormones usually found in ovulating women. They theorized that follicle stimulating hormone, luteinising hormone, and estradiol may encourage ovulation in women exposed to semen. These hormones are not found in the semen of chimpanzees, suggesting this phenomenon may be a human male counter-strategy to concealed ovulation in human females. Other researchers are skeptical that the low levels of hormones found in semen could have any effect on ovulation. One group of authors has theorized that concealed ovulation and menstruation were key factors in the development of symbolic culture in early human society.

Evolutionary hypotheses

Evolutionary psychologists have advanced a number of different possible explanations for concealed ovulation. Some posit that the lack of signaling in some species is a trait retained from evolutionary ancestors, not something that existed previously and later disappeared. If signaling is supposed to have existed and was lost, then it could have been merely due to reduced adaptive importance and lessened selection, or due to direct adaptive advantages for the concealment of ovulation. Yet another possibility (regarding humans specifically) is that while highly specific signaling of ovulation is absent, human female anatomy evolved to mimic permanent signaling of fertility.

Paternal investment hypothesis

The paternal investment hypothesis is strongly supported by many evolutionary biologists. Several hypotheses regarding human evolution integrate the idea that women increasingly required supplemental paternal investment in their offspring. The shared reliance on this idea across several hypotheses concerning human evolution increases its significance in terms of this specific phenomenon.

This hypothesis suggests that women concealed ovulation to obtain men's aid in rearing offspring. Schröder summarizes this hypothesis outlined in Alexander and Noonan's 1979 paper: if women no longer signaled the time of ovulation, men would be unable to detect the exact period in which they were fecund. This led to a change in men's mating strategy: rather than mating with multiple women in the hope that some of them, at least, were fecund during that period, men instead chose to mate with a particular woman repeatedly throughout her menstrual cycle. A mating would be successful in resulting in conception when it occurred during ovulation, and thus, frequent matings, necessitated by the effects of concealed ovulation, would be most evolutionarily successful. A similar hypothesis was proposed by Lovejoy in 1981 that argued that concealed ovulation, reduced canines and bipedalism evolved from a reproductive strategy where males provisioned food resources to his paired female and dependent offspring.

Continuous female sexual receptivity suggests human sexuality is not solely defined by reproduction; a large part of it revolves around conjugal love and communication between partners. Copulations between partners while the woman is pregnant or in the infertile period of her menstrual cycle do not achieve conception, but do strengthen the bond between these partners. Therefore, the increased frequency of copulations due to concealed ovulation are thought to have played a role in fostering pair bonds in humans.

The pair bond would be very advantageous to the reproductive fitness of both partners throughout the period of pregnancy, lactation, and rearing of offspring. Pregnancy, lactation and caring for post-lactation offspring require vast amounts of energy and time on the part of the woman. She must at first consume more food, then provide food to her offspring, while her ability to forage is reduced throughout. Supplemental male investment in the mother and her offspring is advantageous to all parties. While the man supplements the woman's limited gathered food, the woman is enabled to devote the necessary time and energy to the care of their offspring. The offspring benefits from the supplemental investment, in the form of food and defense from the father, and receives the full attention and resources of the mother. Through this shared parental investment, both man and woman would increase their offspring's chances for survival, thereby increasing their reproductive fitness. In this way, natural selection would favor the establishment of pair bonds in humans. To the extent that concealed ovulation strengthened pair bonding, selective pressure would favor concealed ovulation as well.

Another, more recent, hypothesis is that concealed ovulation is an adaptation in response to a promiscuous mating system, similar to that of our closest evolutionary relatives, bonobos and chimpanzees. The theory is that concealed ovulation evolved in women to lessen paternity certainty, which would both lessen the chances of infanticide (as a father is less likely to kill offspring that might be his), and potentially increase the number of men motivated to assist her in caring for her offspring (partible paternity). This is supported by the fact that all other mammals with concealed ovulation, such as dolphins and gray langurs, are promiscuous, and that the only other ape species that have multi-male communities, as humans do, are promiscuous. It is argued that evidence such as the Coolidge effect, showing that a man does not seem to be naturally geared towards sexual mate-guarding behavior (that is, preventing other males from having access to his sexual partner), supports the conclusion that sexual monogamy (though perhaps not social monogamy and/or pair bonding) was rare in early modern humans.

Reduced infanticide hypothesis

This hypothesis suggests the adaptive advantage for women who had hidden estrus would be a reduction in the possibility of infanticide by men, as they would be unable to reliably identify, and kill, their rivals' offspring. This hypothesis is supported by recent studies of wild hanuman langurs, documenting concealed ovulation, and frequent matings with males outside their fertile ovulatory period. Heistermann et al. hypothesize that concealed ovulation is used by women to confuse paternity and thus reduce infanticide in primates. He explains that as ovulation is always concealed in women, men can only determine paternity (and thus decide on whether to kill the woman's child) probabilistically, based on his previous mating frequency with her, and so he would be unable to escape the possibility that the child might be his own, even if he were aware of promiscuous matings on the woman's part.

Sex and reward hypothesis

Schröder reviews a hypothesis by Symons and Hill, that after hunting, men exchanged meat for sex with women. Women who continuously mimicked estrus may have benefited from more meat than those that did not. If this occurred with enough frequency, then a definite period of estrus would have been lost, and with it sexual signaling specific to ovulation would have disappeared.

Social-bonding hypothesis

Schröder presents the idea of a "gradual diminution of mid-cycle estrus and concomitant continuous sexual receptivity in human women" because it facilitated orderly social relationships throughout the menstrual cycle by eliminating the periodic intensification of male–male aggressiveness in competition for mates. The extended estrous period of the bonobo (reproductive-age females are in heat for 75% of their menstrual cycle) has been said to have a similar effect to the lack of a "heat" in women. While concealed human ovulation may have evolved in this fashion, extending estrus until it was no longer a distinct period, as paralleled in the bonobo, this theory of why concealed ovulation evolved has frequently been rejected. Schröder outlines the two objections to this hypothesis: (1) natural selection would need to work at a level above the individual, which is difficult to prove; and (2) selection, because it acts on the individuals with the most reproductive success, would thus favor greater reproductive success over social integration at the expense of reproductive success.

However, since 1993 when that was written, group selection models have seen a resurgence. (See group selection, reciprocal altruism, and kin selection.)

Cuckoldry hypothesis

Schröder in his review writes that Benshoof and Thornhill hypothesized that estrus became hidden after monogamous relationships became the norm in Homo erectus. Concealed ovulation allowed the woman to mate secretly at times with a genetically superior man, and thus gain the benefit of his genes for her offspring, while still retaining the benefits of the pair bond with her usual sexual partner. Her usual sexual partner would have little reason to doubt her fidelity, because of the concealed ovulation, and would have high, albeit unfounded, paternity confidence in her offspring. His confidence would encourage him to invest his time and energy in assisting her to care for the child, even though it was not his own. Again, the idea of a man's investment being vital to the child's survival is a central fixture of a hypothesis regarding concealed ovulation, even as the evolutionary benefits accrue to the child, the woman, and her clandestine partner, and not to her regular sexual partner.

As a side effect of bipedalism

Pawlowski presents the importance of bipedalism to the mechanics and necessity of ovulation signaling. The more open savannah environment inhabited by early humans brought greater danger from predators. This would have caused humans to live in denser groups, and, in such a scenario, the long-distance sexual signaling provided by female genital swellings would have lost its function. Concealed ovulation is thus argued to be a loss of function evolutionary change rather than an adaptation. Thermoregulatory systems were also modified in humans with the move to the savannah to conserve water. It is thought that female genital swellings would have incurred added cost because of ineffective evaporation of water from the area. Pawlowski continues by saying the change to bipedalism in early hominins changed both the position of female genitals and the line of vision of males. Since males could no longer constantly see the female genitals, swelling of them during estrus as a mode of signaling would have become useless. Also, anogenital swelling at each ovulatory period may have interfered with the mechanics of bipedal locomotion, and selection may have favored females who were less hindered by this occurrence. This hypothesis ultimately concludes that bipedalism, which was strongly selected for, caused the physiological changes and a loss of function of sexual signaling through female genital swelling, leading to the concealed ovulation we now observe.

Pawlowski's paper offers views that differ from the other hypotheses regarding concealed ovulation in that it pinpoints physiological changes in early humans as the cause of concealed ovulation rather than social or behavioral ones. One of the strengths of this is derived from the other hypotheses' weaknesses – it is difficult to track the evolution of a behavior as it leaves no verifiable evidence in the form of bone or DNA. However, the fact that the hanuman langurs also display some concealed ovulation and that it is not directly caused by a physiological change to bipedalism may suggest bipedalism was not, at least, the sole cause of concealed ovulation in humans. As stated earlier, it is possible for many elements of different hypotheses to be true regarding the selective pressures for concealed ovulation in humans.

See also

References

  1. Sandy J. Andelman (June 1987). "Evolution of Concealed Ovulation in Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops)". The American Naturalist. 129 (6): 785–799. doi:10.1086/284675. S2CID 83522515.
  2. S. C. Roberts; J. Havlicek; J. Flegr; M. Hruskova; A. C. Little; B. C. Jones; D. I. Perrett; M. Petrie (August 2004). "Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 271 (Suppl 5): S270–S272. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0174. PMC 1810066. PMID 15503991.
  3. Geoffrey Miller; Joshua M. Tybur; Brent D. Jordan (June 2007). "Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap dancers: economic evidence for human estrous?" (PDF). Evolution and Human Behavior. 28 (6): 375–381. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.154.8176. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.06.002. Retrieved 2008-01-21.
  4. Pipitone, R.; G . Gallup Jr (2008-05-18). "Women's voice attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle". Evolution and Human Behavior. 29 (4): 268–274. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.02.001.
  5. Susan B. Bullivant; Sarah A. Sellergren; Kathleen Stern; et al. (February 2004). "Women's sexual experience during the menstrual cycle: identification of the sexual phase by noninvasive measurement of luteinizing hormone". Journal of Sex Research. 41 (1): 82–93. doi:10.1080/00224490409552216. PMID 15216427. S2CID 40401379. Archived from the original on 2007-07-28.
  6. Ann Renninger, Lee; Wade, T. Joel; Grammer, Karl (2004). "Getting that female glance: Patterns and consequences of male nonverbal behavior in courtship contexts". Evolution and Human Behavior. 25 (6): 416–431. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.006.
  7. Thornhill, Randy; Gangestad, Steven W. (1999). "The Scent of Symmetry: A Human Sex Pheromone that Signals Fitness?". Evolution and Human Behavior. 20 (3): 175–201. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(99)00005-7.
  8. Brooksbank, B. W. L. (1962). "Urinary excretion of androst-16-en-3 alpha-ol in human male axillary sweat". Experientia. 30 (8): 864–865. doi:10.1007/bf01938327. PMID 4416149. S2CID 44558005.
  9. Brewis, A.; Meyer, M. (2005). "Demographic Evidence That Human Ovulation is Undetectable (at Least in Pair Bonds)". Current Anthropology. 46 (3): 465–471. doi:10.1086/430016. S2CID 30243603.
  10. Regan, P. C. (1996). "Rhythms of desire: the association between menstrual cycle phases and female sexual desire". Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality. 5: 145–156.
  11. Motluk, Alison (2006-08-05). "The secret life of semen". New Scientist (2563). Retrieved 2008-06-28.
  12. Chris Knight (1991). Blood relations: menstruation and the origins of culture. New Haven, Conn: Yale University Press. ISBN 978-0-300-04911-4.
  13. Knight, Chris; Camilla Power; Ian Watts (1995). "The Human Symbolic Revolution: A Darwinian Account" (PDF). Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 5 (1): 75–114. doi:10.1017/S0959774300001190. S2CID 54701302. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2020-08-01. Retrieved 2006-12-13.
  14. ^ Schoroder, I. (1993). "Concealed ovulation and clandestine copulation: A female contribution to human evolution". Ethology and Sociobiology. 14 (6): 381–389. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(93)90026-E.
  15. Burt, Austin (June 1992). "'Concealed ovulation' and sexual signals in primates". Folia Primatologica. 58 (1): 1–6. doi:10.1159/000156600. PMID 1618432.
  16. Frederick S. Szalay; Robert K.Costello (June 1991). "Evolution of permanent estrus displays in hominids". Journal of Human Evolution. 20 (6): 439–464. doi:10.1016/0047-2484(91)90019-R.
  17. Lovejoy, C. Owen (1981-01-23). "The Origin of Man". Science. 211 (4480): 341–350. Bibcode:1981Sci...211..341L. doi:10.1126/science.211.4480.341. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 17748254.
  18. Lovejoy, C. Owen (2009-10-02). "Reexamining Human Origins in Light of Ardipithecus ramidus" (PDF). Science. 326 (5949): 74–74e8. Bibcode:2009Sci...326...74L. doi:10.1126/science.1175834. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 19810200. S2CID 42790876.
  19. Benagiano, G.; Mori, M. (2009). "The origins of human sexuality: Procreation or recreation?". Reproductive Biomedicine Online. 18 (Suppl 1): 50–59. doi:10.1016/s1472-6483(10)60116-2. PMID 19281665.
  20. Christopher Ryan Ph.D & Cacilda Jethá Ph.D (2012). Sex at Dawn. HarperCollins. ISBN 978-0-06-220794-4.
  21. Hestermann, M.; Ziegler, T.; Van Schaik, C. P.; Launhardt, K.; Winkler, P.; Hodges, J. K. (2001). "Loss of oestrus, concealed ovulation and paternity confusion in free-ranging Hanuman langurs". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 268 (1484): 2445–2451. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1833. PMC 1088898. PMID 11747562.
  22. Koeslag, J. H. (1997). "Sex, the prisoner's dilemma game, and the evolutionary inevitability of cooperation". J. Theor. Biol. 189 (1): 53–61. Bibcode:1997JThBi.189...53K. doi:10.1006/jtbi.1997.0496. PMID 9398503.
  23. Koeslag, J. H. (2003). "Evolution of cooperation: cooperation defeats defection in the cornfield model". J. Theor. Biol. 224 (3): 399–410. Bibcode:2003JThBi.224..399K. doi:10.1016/s0022-5193(03)00188-7. PMID 12941597.
  24. Wilson, D. S.; Wilson, E. O. (2008). "Evolution 'for the good of the group'. ". American Scientist. 96 (5): 380–389. doi:10.1511/2008.74.1.
  25. ^ Pawłowski, B. (1999). "Loss of Oestrus and Concealed Ovulation in Human Evolution: The Case against the Sexual-Selection Hypothesis". Current Anthropology. 40 (3): 257–276. doi:10.1086/200017. S2CID 85884654.

Further reading

Evolutionary psychology
Evolutionary
processes
Areas
Cognition /
Emotion
Culture
Development
Human factors /
Mental health
Sex
Sex differences
Related subjects
Academic disciplines
Research topics
Theoretical positions
  1. Burt, A. (1992). "'Concealed ovulation' and sexual signals in primates". Folia Primatologica; International Journal of Primatology. 58 (1): 1–6. doi:10.1159/000156600. ISSN 0015-5713. PMID 1618432.
Categories: