Misplaced Pages

Intelligence quotient: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 11:34, 26 March 2006 editHuangdi (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users7,920 edits Race and IQ← Previous edit Latest revision as of 16:10, 6 January 2025 edit undoAddi-bot (talk | contribs)14 editsm Puts links on first line. 
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Score from a test designed to assess intelligence}}
{{redirect|IQ}}
{{about|the history and applications of IQ|the classifications of IQ|IQ classification}}{{Redirect|IQ}}
] results, which causes a ] graph of IQ score frequency. Colors delineate one ].]]
{{Use British English|date=February 2023}}
An '''intelligence quotient''' or '''IQ''' is a score derived from a set of ]s developed to measure a person's ] ("]") in relation to their age group. An IQ test does not measure intelligence the way a ruler measures height (absolutely), but rather the way a ] measures speed (relatively); IQ is described as a "quotient" because, originally, it represented the ratio between a person's "mental age" and actual chronological age.
{{Use dmy dates|date=August 2022}}
{{Infobox diagnostic
| name = Intelligence quotient
| image = Raven Matrix.svg
| alt =
| caption = One kind of IQ test item, modelled after items in the ] test
| ICD10 = {{ICD10|Z|01|8}}
| ICD9 = {{ICD9proc|94.01}}
}}
{{Psychology sidebar|all}}
An '''intelligence quotient''' ('''IQ''') is a total score derived from a set of ]s or subtests designed to assess ].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Braaten |first1=Ellen B. |last2=Norman |first2=Dennis |title=Intelligence (IQ) Testing |journal=Pediatrics in Review |date=1 November 2006 |volume=27 |issue=11 |pages=403–408 |doi=10.1542/pir.27-11-403 |pmid=17079505 |issn=0191-9601 |url=https://pedsinreview.aappublications.org/content/27/11/403 |access-date=22 January 2020}}</ref> Originally, IQ was a score obtained by dividing a person's ] score, obtained by administering an intelligence test, by the person's chronological age, both expressed in terms of years and months. The resulting fraction (]) was multiplied by 100 to obtain the IQ score.<ref>{{Cite encyclopedia |title=Glossary of Important Assessment and Measurement Terms|publisher=National Council on Measurement in Education |date=2016 |location=Philadelphia, PA |url=http://www.ncme.org/ncme/NCME/Resource_Center/Glossary/NCME/Resource_Center/Glossary1.aspx?hkey=4bb87415-44dc-4088-9ed9-e8515326a061#anchorI |access-date=1 July 2017 |entry=intelligence quotient (IQ) |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170722194028/http://www.ncme.org/ncme/NCME/Resource_Center/Glossary/NCME/Resource_Center/Glossary1.aspx?hkey=4bb87415-44dc-4088-9ed9-e8515326a061#anchorI |archive-date=22 July 2017|url-status=dead}}</ref> For modern ], the ] is ] to a ] with mean 100 and ] 15.<ref name="Gottfredson2009pp31–32" /> This results in approximately two-thirds of the population scoring between IQ 85 and IQ 115 and about 2 percent each ] and ].<ref name="Neisser97">{{cite journal |last=Neisser |first=Ulrich |title=Rising Scores on Intelligence Tests |journal=American Scientist |volume=85 |issue=5 |pages=440–447 |year=1997 |url=http://www.americanscientist.org/issues/feature/rising-scores-on-intelligence-tests/ |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161104214157/http://www.americanscientist.org/issues/feature/rising-scores-on-intelligence-tests/99999 |archive-date=4 November 2016 |access-date=1 December 2017 |bibcode=1997AmSci..85..440N}}</ref><ref name="Hunt2011p5" />


Scores from intelligence tests are estimates of intelligence. Unlike, for example, distance and mass, a concrete measure of intelligence cannot be achieved given the abstract nature of the concept of "intelligence".<ref>{{cite book|last1=Haier|first1=Richard|title=The Neuroscience of Intelligence |publisher=Cambridge University Press|isbn=9781107461437|pages=18–19|date=28 December 2016}}</ref> IQ scores have been shown to be associated with such factors as ],<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Cusick |first1=Sarah E. |last2=Georgieff |first2=Michael K. |date=1 August 2017 |title=The Role of Nutrition in Brain Development: The Golden Opportunity of the 'First 1000 Days' |journal=The Journal of Pediatrics |volume=175 |pages=16–21 |doi=10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.05.013 |pmc=4981537 |pmid=27266965 | issn = 0022-3476}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Saloojee|first1=Haroon|last2=Pettifor|first2=John M|date=15 December 2001|title=Iron deficiency and impaired child development|journal=British Medical Journal|volume=323|issue=7326|pages=1377–1378|doi=10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377|issn=0959-8138|pmc=1121846|pmid=11744547}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Qian|first1=Ming|last2=Wang|first2=Dong|last3=Watkins|first3=William E.|last4=Gebski|first4=Val|last5=Yan|first5=Yu Qin|last6=Li|first6=Mu|last7=Chen|first7=Zu Pei|date=2005|title=The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China|url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15734706/|journal=Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition|volume=14|issue=1|pages=32–42|issn=0964-7058|pmid=15734706}}</ref> parental ],<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Poh|first1=Bee Koon|last2=Lee|first2=Shoo Thien|last3=Yeo|first3=Giin Shang|last4=Tang|first4=Kean Choon|last5=Noor Afifah|first5=Ab Rahim|last6=Siti Hanisa|first6=Awal|last7=Parikh|first7=Panam|last8=Wong|first8=Jyh Eiin|last9=Ng|first9=Alvin Lai Oon|last10=SEANUTS Study Group|date=13 June 2019|title=Low socioeconomic status and severe obesity are linked to poor cognitive performance in Malaysian children|journal=BMC Public Health|volume=19|issue=Suppl 4|pages=541|doi=10.1186/s12889-019-6856-4|issn=1471-2458|pmc=6565598|pmid=31196019 |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Galván|first1=Marcos|last2=Uauy|first2=Ricardo|last3=Corvalán|first3=Camila|last4=López-Rodríguez|first4=Guadalupe|last5=Kain|first5=Juliana|date=September 2013|title=Determinants of cognitive development of low SES children in Chile: a post-transitional country with rising childhood obesity rates|url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22915146/|journal=Maternal and Child Health Journal|volume=17|issue=7|pages=1243–1251|doi=10.1007/s10995-012-1121-9|issn=1573-6628|pmid=22915146|s2cid=19767926}}</ref> ] and ],<ref>{{cite journal |author1=Markus Jokela |author2=G. David Batty |author3=Ian J. Deary |author4=Catharine R. Gale |author5=Mika Kivimäki |year=2009 |title=Low Childhood IQ and Early Adult Mortality: The Role of Explanatory Factors in the 1958 British Birth Cohort |journal=Pediatrics |volume=124 |issue=3 |pages=e380 – e388 |doi=10.1542/peds.2009-0334 |pmid=19706576 |s2cid=25256969}}</ref>{{sfn|Deary|Batty|2007}} parental ],{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}} and ].<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Ronfani|first1=Luca|last2=Vecchi Brumatti|first2=Liza|last3=Mariuz|first3=Marika|last4=Tognin|first4=Veronica|date=2015|title=The Complex Interaction between Home Environment, Socioeconomic Status, Maternal IQ and Early Child Neurocognitive Development: A Multivariate Analysis of Data Collected in a Newborn Cohort Study|journal=PLOS ONE|volume=10|issue=5|pages=e0127052|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0127052|pmid=25996934|pmc=4440732|bibcode=2015PLoSO..1027052R|doi-access=free}}</ref> While the ] has been investigated for nearly a century, there is still debate about the significance of heritability estimates<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Johnson |first1=Wendy |last2=Turkheimer |first2=Eric |last3=Gottesman |first3=Irving I. |last4=Bouchard |first4=Thomas J. |title=Beyond Heritability |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |date=August 2009 |volume=18 |issue=4 |pages=217–220 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01639.x |pmid=20625474 |pmc=2899491}}</ref>{{sfn|Turkheimer|2008}} and the mechanisms of inheritance.<ref name=Devlin97>{{cite journal |pages=468–71 |issue=6641 |volume=388 |year=1997 |pmid=9242404 |journal=Nature |doi=10.1038/41319 |last1=Devlin |first1=B. |last2=Daniels |first2=Michael |last3=Roeder |first3=Kathryn |author3-link= Kathryn Roeder |title=The heritability of IQ |bibcode=1997Natur.388..468D |s2cid=4313884|doi-access=free }}</ref>
For people living in the prevailing conditions of the developed world, IQ is highly ], and by adulthood the influence of family environment on IQ is undetectable. IQ test scores are correlated with measures of brain structure and function, as well as performance on simple tasks that anyone can complete within a few seconds.

IQ scores are used for educational placement, ], and evaluating job applicants. In research contexts, they have been studied as predictors of ]<ref name="Schmidt98"/> and ].<ref name="Strenze2007"/> They are also used to study distributions of psychometric intelligence in populations and the ] between it and other variables. Raw scores on IQ tests for many populations have been rising at an average rate that scales to three IQ points per decade since the early 20th century, a phenomenon called the ]. Investigation of different patterns of increases in subtest scores can also inform current research on human intelligence.

Historically, many proponents of IQ testing have been ] who used ] to push now-debunked views of ] in order to justify ] and oppose ].<ref name=":11">{{Cite web |last=Winston |first=Andrew S. |date=29 May 2020 |title=Scientific Racism and North American Psychology |url=https://oxfordre.com/psychology/display/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.001.0001/acrefore-9780190236557-e-516 |website=Oxford Research Encyclopedias: Psychology |quote=The use of psychological concepts and data to promote ideas of an enduring racial hierarchy dates from the late 1800s and has continued to the present. The history of scientific racism in psychology is intertwined with broader debates, anxieties, and political issues in American society. With the rise of intelligence testing, joined with ideas of eugenic progress and dysgenic reproduction, psychological concepts and data came to play an important role in naturalizing racial inequality. Although racial comparisons were not the primary concern of most early mental testing, results were employed to justify beliefs regarding Black “educability” and the dangers of Southern and Eastern European immigration.}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book |last=Newitz |first=Annalee |author-link=Annalee Newitz |title=Stories Are Weapons: Psychological Warfare and the American Mind |date=June 4, 2024 |publisher=W. W. Norton & Company |chapter=Chapter 4}}</ref> Such views are now rejected by a strong consensus of mainstream science, though fringe figures continue to promote them in ] and popular culture.<ref name=":12">{{Cite journal |last1=Bird |first1=Kevin |last2=Jackson |first2=John P. |last3=Winston |first3=Andrew S. |date=2024 |title=Confronting Scientific Racism in Psychology: Lessons from Evolutionary Biology and Genetics |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Famp0001228 |journal=American Psychologist |volume=79 |issue=4 |pages=497–508 |doi=10.1037/amp0001228 |pmid=39037836 |quote=Recent articles claim that the folk categories of race are genetically meaningful divisions, and that evolved genetic differences among races and nations are important for explaining immutable differences in cognitive ability, educational attainment, crime, sexual behavior, and wealth; all claims that are opposed by a strong scientific consensus to the contrary.}}</ref><ref name=":13">{{Cite journal |last1=Panofsky |first1=Aaron |last2=Dasgupta |first2=Kushan |last3=Iturriaga |first3=Nicole |year=2021 |title=How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |volume=175 |issue=2 |pages=387–398 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.24150 |issn=0002-9483 |pmc=9909835 |pmid=32986847 |quote=he claims that genetics defines racial groups and makes them different, that IQ and cultural differences among racial groups are caused by genes, and that racial inequalities within and between nations are the inevitable outcome of long evolutionary processes are neither new nor supported by science (either old or new). |doi-access=free}}</ref>


IQ is correlated with academic success, job performance, socioeconomic advancement, and "social pathologies". It is taken by psychologists to be an excellent ] for intelligence, and possibly the best measurable definition of intellectual ability, but generally not taken to represent intelligence perfectly. Recent work has demonstrated links between IQ and health, longevity, and functional literacy.
==History== ==History==
{{see also|History of the race and intelligence controversy}}


===Precursors to IQ testing===
Early attempts of mental tests were those of ] (1863) and ] (1888). These tests were more physical tests than mental ones. Their importance was in developing the idea that one's IQ can be measured and is different from person to person. They also proposed normal distributions of mental tests results within a large population.
Historically, even before IQ tests were devised, there were attempts to classify people into ] categories by observing their behavior in daily life.<ref name="TermanOldClasses" /><ref name="WechslerOldClasses" /> Those other forms of behavioral observation are still important for validating classifications based primarily on IQ test scores. Both intelligence classification by observation of behavior outside the testing room and classification by IQ testing depend on the definition of "intelligence" used in a particular case and on the ] and error of estimation in the classification procedure.


The English statistician ] (1822–1911) made the first attempt at creating a standardized test for rating a person's intelligence. A pioneer of ] and the application of statistical methods to the study of human diversity and the study of inheritance of human traits, he believed that intelligence was largely a product of heredity (by which he did not mean ], although he did develop several ] theories of particulate inheritance).<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bulmer |first1=M |year=1999 |title=The development of Francis Galton's ideas on the mechanism of heredity |journal=Journal of the History of Biology |volume=32 |issue=3 |pages=263–292 | doi = 10.1023/a:1004608217247 |pmid=11624207 |s2cid=10451997}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Cowan |first1=R. S. |year=1972 |title=Francis Galton's contribution to genetics |journal=Journal of the History of Biology |volume=5 |issue=2 |pages=389–412 |doi=10.1007/bf00346665|pmid=11610126 |s2cid=30206332}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Burbridge |first1=D |year=2001 |title=Francis Galton on twins, heredity and social class |journal=British Journal for the History of Science |volume=34 |issue=3 |pages=323–340 |doi=10.1017/s0007087401004332|pmid=11700679}}</ref> He hypothesized that there should exist a correlation between intelligence and other observable traits such as ]es, muscle grip, and ].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Fancher |first1=R. E. |year=1983 |title=Biographical origins of Francis Galton's psychology |journal=Isis |volume=74 |issue=2 |pages=227–233 |doi=10.1086/353245|pmid=6347965|s2cid=40565053 }}</ref> He set up the first mental testing center in the world in 1882 and he published "Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development" in 1883, in which he set out his theories. After gathering data on a variety of physical variables, he was unable to show any such correlation, and he eventually abandoned this research.<ref name="Kaufman2009p21" /><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gillham |first1=Nicholas W. |title=Sir Francis Galton and the birth of eugenics |journal=Annual Review of Genetics |volume=35 |issue=1 |pages=83–101 |year=2001 |pmid=11700278 |doi=10.1146/annurev.genet.35.102401.090055}}</ref>
] and his colleague ] created the '''Binet-Simon scale''' in ], which used testing to identify students who could benefit from extra help in school. Their assumption was that lower scores indicated the need for more teaching, not an inability to learn. This interpretation is still held by some modern experts.
], co-developer of the ]]]
French psychologist ] and psychiatrist ], had more success in 1905, when they published the ], which focused on verbal abilities.<ref name=":15" /> It was intended to identify "mental retardation" in school children,<ref name=Kaufman2009/> but in specific contradistinction to claims made by psychiatrists that these children were "sick" (not "slow") and should therefore be removed from school and cared for in asylums.<ref name=":15">{{cite journal |last1=Nicolas |first1=S. |last2=Andrieu |first2=B. |last3=Croizet |first3=J.-C. |last4=Sanitioso |first4=R. B. |last5=Burman |first5=J. T. |year=2013 |title=Sick? Or slow? On the origins of intelligence as a psychological object |journal=Intelligence |volume=41 |issue=5 |pages=699–711 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2013.08.006 |doi-access=free}} (This is an ] article, made freely available by ].)</ref> The score on the Binet–Simon scale would reveal the child's ]. For example, a six-year-old child who passed all the tasks usually passed by six-year-olds—but nothing beyond—would have a mental age that matched his chronological age, 6.0. (Fancher, 1985). Binet and Simon thought that intelligence was multifaceted, but came under the control of practical judgment.


In Binet and Simon's view, there were limitations with the scale and they stressed what they saw as the remarkable diversity of intelligence and the subsequent need to study it using qualitative, as opposed to quantitative, measures (White, 2000). American psychologist ] published a translation of it in 1910. American psychologist ] at ] revised the Binet–Simon scale, which resulted in the ] (1916). It became the most popular test in the United States for decades.<ref name=Kaufman2009/>{{sfn|Terman et al.|1915}}<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Wallin |first1=J. E. W. |title=The new clinical psychology and the psycho-clinicist |journal=Journal of Educational Psychology |volume=2 |issue=3 |pages=121–32 |year=1911 |doi=10.1037/h0075544 |url=https://zenodo.org/record/1429171}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Richardson |first1=John T. E. |title=Howard Andrew Knox and the origins of performance testing on Ellis Island, 1912-1916 |journal=History of Psychology |volume=6 |issue=2 |pages=143–70 |year=2003 |pmid=12822554 |doi=10.1037/1093-4510.6.2.143}}</ref>
Notably, Binet himself made ''no claim'' that his test properly measured intelligence. He stated in his paper ''New Methods for the Diagnosis of the Intellectual Level of Subnormals'' that
:"This scale properly speaking does not permit the measure of the intelligence, because intellectual qualities are not superposable, and therefore cannot be measured as linear surfaces are measured, but are on the contrary, a classification, a hierarchy among diverse intelligences; and for the necessities of practice this classification is equivalent to a measure."


The abbreviation "IQ" was coined by the ] ] for the ] term {{lang|de|Intelligenzquotient}}, his term for a scoring method for ] tests at ] he advocated in a 1912 book.{{sfn|Stern|1914|pp=70–84 (1914 English translation); pp. 48–58 (1912 original German edition)}}
In ], ] proposed three categories for the "feeble-minded" based on IQ scores: ] (IQ of 51&ndash;70), ] (IQ of 26&ndash;50), and ] (IQ of 0&ndash;25). This taxonomy was the standard of intelligence research for decades.


===General factor (''g'')===
In ], ] psychologist ] released the "Stanford Revision of the Binet-Simon Scale", generally known as the ''']'''. This became the most commonly administered test for many decades. The term "intelligence quotient," in which each student's score was the quotient of his or her tested mental age with his or her actual age, was adopted by Terman from a ] proposal by German psychologist ]. This led to refined testing developed by ] for ] recruits.
{{main|g factor (psychometrics)|l1=''g'' factor}}
The many different kinds of IQ tests include a wide variety of item content. Some test items are visual, while many are verbal. Test items vary from being based on abstract-reasoning problems to concentrating on arithmetic, vocabulary, or general knowledge.


The British psychologist ] in 1904 made the first formal ] of ]s between the tests. He observed that children's school grades across seemingly unrelated school subjects were positively correlated, and reasoned that these correlations reflected the influence of an underlying general mental ability that entered into performance on all kinds of mental tests. He suggested that all mental performance could be conceptualized in terms of a single general ability factor and a large number of narrow task-specific ability factors. Spearman named it ''g'' for "general factor" and labeled the specific factors or abilities for specific tasks ''s''.{{sfn|Deary|2001|pp=6–12}} In any collection of test items that make up an IQ test, the score that best measures ''g'' is the composite score that has the highest correlations with all the item scores. Typically, the "''g''-loaded" composite score of an IQ test battery appears to involve a common strength in abstract reasoning across the test's item content.{{Citation needed|date=June 2020}}
Today, the most commonly administered IQ test is the ''']''' test, originally developed by ] in 1974. The WISC-III test comprises ten types of problems, categorized by difficulty and by skill type (verbal and performance scales). A revised version, the WISC-IV, was released in 2003 and is used regularly in assessments. However, the interpretation of various combinations of subscales is still being researched. Another notable type of IQ test is the Bailey Scale of Infant Development, regarded as the 'best' means of testing cognitive development in infants.


===United States military selection in World War I===
Today, informal online IQ tests are popular, but they are at best rough approximations. The tests are not expert certified and notable limitations include a small number of questions and a lack of the time limit.
During World War I, the Army needed a way to evaluate and assign recruits to appropriate tasks. This led to the development of several mental tests by ], who worked with major hereditarians of American psychometrics—including Terman, Goddard—to write the test.<ref name=Gould>{{harvnb|Gould|1996}}</ref> The testing generated controversy and much public debate in the United States. Nonverbal or "performance" tests were developed for those who could not speak English or were suspected of malingering.<ref name=Kaufman2009/> Based on Goddard's translation of the Binet–Simon test, the tests had an impact in screening men for officer training:


<blockquote>...the tests did have a strong impact in some areas, particularly in screening men for officer training. At the start of the war, the army and national guard maintained nine thousand officers. By the end, two hundred thousand officers presided, and two- thirds of them had started their careers in training camps where the tests were applied. In some camps, no man scoring below C could be considered for officer training.<ref name=Gould/></blockquote>
==IQ score distribution==
{| border="2" cellpadding="4" cellspacing="0" style="margin: 1em 1em 1em 0; border: 1px #aaa solid; border-collapse: collapse; font-size: 95%;" ALIGN="right"
|+ '''IQ score distribution'''
|- bgcolor=#ccccff
!IQ Range !! Percentile Range
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
|Below 30 || 0 &ndash; 0.0002
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
|30 to 50 || 0.0002 &ndash; 0.04
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
|50 to 60 || 0.04 &ndash; 0.4
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
|60 to 74 || 0.4 &ndash; 4
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
|74 to 89 || 4 &ndash; 23
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
|89 to 100 || 23 &ndash; 50
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
|100 to 111 || 50 &ndash; 77
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
|111 to 120 || 77 &ndash; 91
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
|120 to 125 || 91 &ndash; 95
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
|125 to 132 || 95 &ndash; 98
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
|132 to 137 || 98 &ndash; 99.3
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
|137 to 150 || 99.3 &ndash; 99.96
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
|above 150 || 99.96 &ndash; 100
|}


In total 1.75 million men were tested, making the results the first mass-produced written tests of intelligence, though considered dubious and non-usable, for reasons including high variability of test implementation throughout different camps and questions testing for familiarity with American culture rather than intelligence.<ref name=Gould/> After the war, positive publicity promoted by army psychologists helped to make psychology a respected field.<ref>{{cite book |first1=Carrie H. |last1=Kennedy |first2=Jeffrey A. |last2=McNeil |editor1-first=Carrie H. |editor1-last=Kennedy |editor2-first=Eric |editor2-last=Zillmer |year=2006 |chapter=A history of military psychology |chapter-url= https://books.google.com/books?id=rytCzdXGgXkC&pg=PA1 |title=Military Psychology: Clinical and Operational Applications |pages=1–17 |location= New York |publisher=Guilford Press |isbn=978-1-57230-724-7}}</ref> Subsequently, there was an increase in jobs and funding in psychology in the United States.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Katzell |first1=Raymond A. |last2=Austin |first2=James T. |year=1992 |title=From then to now: The development of industrial-organizational psychology in the United States |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=77 |issue=6 |pages=803–35 |doi=10.1037/0021-9010.77.6.803}}</ref> Group intelligence tests were developed and became widely used in schools and industry.<ref name="Kevles, D. J. 1968">{{cite journal |last1=Kevles |first1=D. J. |title=Testing the Army's Intelligence: Psychologists and the Military in World War I |journal=The Journal of American History |volume=55 |issue=3 |pages=565–81 |year=1968 |doi=10.2307/1891014 |jstor=1891014}}</ref>
IQ scores are ''normalized''. It is not possible to infer an IQ from a test without specifying a sample population, as scores are interpreted so the mean IQ will be 100. The ] (&sigma;) is usually 15, but 16 for the Stanford-Binet IQ test, and 24 for the ] test. IQs are always reported as ]s; it is considered impossible for the tests to measure intelligence with such a degree of ] as would make further fineness meaningful. Tests are designed so that the distribution of IQ scores is ]; that is, it follows a ]; however, it is disputed that this bell curve still holds at the extremes (see: ]). A difference has been documented between the IQ score distributions of ] and ] test subjects; the distribution in left-handed people tends to cluster at the two extremes of the IQ scale.<!--ref-->


The results of these tests, which at the time reaffirmed contemporary racism and nationalism, are considered controversial and dubious, having rested on certain contested assumptions: that intelligence was heritable, innate, and could be relegated to a single number, the tests were enacted systematically, and test questions actually tested for innate intelligence rather than subsuming environmental factors.<ref name=Gould/> The tests also allowed for the bolstering of ] in the context of increased immigration, which may have influenced the passing of the ].<ref name=Gould/>
(The following numbers apply to IQ scales standard deviation σ = 15.) Roughly 68% of the population has an IQ between 85 and 115. The "normal" range, or range between &minus;2 and +2 standard deviations from the mean, is between 70 and 130, and contains about 95% of the population. An accurate score below 70 may indicate ], and a score above 130 may indicate ]. Retardation may result from normal variation or from a genetic or developmental malady; analogously, some otherwise normal people are very short, and others have ]. Giftedness appears to be normal variation; ] often have astonishing cognitive powers but below-average IQs.
It has been observed that scores outside the range 55 to 145 must be cautiously interpreted because there are smaller numbers of respondents with which to make comparisons in those ranges. Moreover, at such extreme values, the normal distribution is a less accurate estimate of the true IQ distribution.


] argued for a model of intelligence that included seven unrelated factors (verbal comprehension, word fluency, number facility, spatial visualization, associative memory, perceptual speed, reasoning, and induction). While not widely used, Thurstone's model influenced later theories.<ref name=Kaufman2009/>
In actuality there is a higher percentage of the population measured at 3 or more standard deviation levels on the test than the probabilities of the normal distribution would predict (], ''Genetic Studies of Genius'', v.1, 1925). Some IQ scoring procedures may attempt to integrate such clusters of statistical outliers into the curve by adjusting the scores so that they better represent actual probabilities (according to Silverman) and in these cases, scores around 145 and above may actually have been notably higher, were they not so adjusted.


] produced the first version of his test in 1939. It gradually became more popular and overtook the Stanford–Binet in the 1960s. It has been revised several times, as is common for IQ tests, to incorporate new research. One explanation is that psychologists and educators wanted more information than the single score from the Binet. Wechsler's ten or more subtests provided this. Another is that the Stanford–Binet test reflected mostly verbal abilities, while the Wechsler test also reflected nonverbal abilities. The Stanford–Binet has also been revised several times and is now similar to the Wechsler in several aspects, but the Wechsler continues to be the most popular test in the United States.<ref name=Kaufman2009/>
Most IQ tests in the ] tend to use a SD-15 or SD-16 scale, meaning that one standard deviation corresponds to ±16 points on the IQ scale. However, European IQ tests tend to use a SD-24 or SD-25 scale, resulting in discrepancies. Therefore, an IQ of 130 (+2 standard deviations) in the U.S. might correspond to an IQ of 148-150 in Europe. Due to these differences, percentiles are more accurate measurements than IQ numbers.


===IQ testing and the eugenics movement in the United States===
==IQ and General Intelligence Factor''==
], a set of beliefs and practices aimed at improving the ] quality of the ] by excluding people and groups judged to be inferior and promoting those judged to be superior,<ref name="Spektorowski">{{cite book|last1=Spektorowski|first1=Alberto|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=zdkdAAAAQBAJ&q=Historically,+the+term+has+referred+to+everything+from+prenatal+care+for+mothers+to+forced+sterilization+and+euthanasia&pg=PA24|title=Politics of Eugenics: Productionism, Population, and National Welfare|last2=Ireni-Saban|first2=Liza|date=2013|publisher=Routledge|isbn=978-0-203-74023-1|location=London|page=24|quote=As an applied science, thus, the practice of eugenics referred to everything from prenatal care for mothers to forced sterilization and euthanasia. Galton divided the practice of eugenics into two types—positive and negative—both aimed at improving the human race through selective breeding.|access-date=16 January 2017}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|date=26 September 2010|title=Eugenics|url=http://ghr.nlm.nih.gov/glossary=eugenics|work=Unified Medical Language System (Psychological Index Terms)|publisher=National Library of Medicine}}</ref><ref name="Galton1904">{{cite journal|author=Galton, Francis|author-link=Francis Galton|date=July 1904|title=Eugenics: Its Definition, Scope, and Aims|url=http://www.mugu.com/galton/essays/1900-1911/galton-1904-am-journ-soc-eugenics-scope-aims.htm|journal=The American Journal of Sociology|volume=X|issue=1|pages=82, 1st paragraph|bibcode=1904Natur..70...82.|doi=10.1038/070082a0|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20071103082723/http://galton.org/essays/1900-1911/galton-1904-am-journ-soc-eugenics-scope-aims.htm|archive-date=3 November 2007|access-date=27 December 2010|quote=Eugenics is the science which deals with all influences that improve the inborn qualities of a race; also with those that develop them to the utmost advantage.|doi-access=free}}</ref> played a significant role in the history and culture of the ] during the ], from the late 19th century until US involvement in ].<ref name="SusanCurrell">{{cite book|author1=Susan Currell|author2=]|title=Popular Eugenics: National Efficiency and American Mass Culture in the 1930s|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=WRL6MbBO024C&pg=PA86|year=2006|publisher=Ohio University Press|isbn=978-0-8214-1691-4|pages=2–3}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.princeton.edu/~tleonard/papers/retrospectives.pdf|title=Eugenics and Economics in the Progressive Era|language=en}}</ref>
{{main|General intelligence factor}}


The ] was rooted in the ] ideas of the British Scientist ]. In 1883, Galton first used the word eugenics to describe the biological improvement of human genes and the concept of being "well-born".<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://exhibits.hsl.virginia.edu/eugenics/2-origins/|title=Origins of Eugenics: From Sir Francis Galton to Virginia's Racial Integrity Act of 1924|website=University of Virginia: Historical Collections at the Claude Moore Health Sciences Library|access-date=25 October 2019}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Norrgard|first=K.|date=2008|title=Human testing, the eugenics movement, and IRBs|journal=Nature Education|volume=1|pages=170}}</ref> He believed that differences in a person's ability were acquired primarily through genetics and that eugenics could be implemented through ] in order for the human race to improve in its overall quality, therefore allowing for humans to direct their own evolution.<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://galton.org/books/hereditary-genius/text/pdf/galton-1869-genius-v3.pdf|title=Hereditary Genius|last=Galton|first=Francis|date=1869|page=64|access-date=1 October 2019}}</ref>
Modern IQ tests produce scores for different areas (e.g., language fluency, three-dimensional thinking, etc.), with the summary score calculated from subtest scores. Individual subtest scores tend to ] with one another, even when seemingly disparate in content. Analyses of an individual's scores on the subtests of a single IQ test or the scores from a variety of different IQ tests (e.g., ], ], ] and others) will reveal that they all measure a single common factor and various factors that are specific to each test. This kind of ] has led to the theory that underlying these disparate cognitive tasks is a single factor, termed the ] (or ''g''), that corresponds with the common-sense concept of intelligence. In the normal population, ''g'' and IQ are roughly 90% correlated and are often used interchangeably.


] was a eugenicist. In 1908, he published his own version, ''The Binet and Simon Test of Intellectual Capacity'', and cordially promoted the test. He quickly extended the use of the scale to the public schools (1913), to immigration (], 1914) and to a court of law (1914).<ref name=":3">{{Cite web|url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/2009/01/assessment.aspx|title=The birth of American intelligence testing|access-date=11 November 2017}}</ref>
==Genetics vs environment==


Unlike Galton, who promoted eugenics through selective breeding for positive traits, Goddard went with the US eugenics movement to eliminate "undesirable" traits.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.nature.com/scitable/forums/genetics-generation/america-s-hidden-history-the-eugenics-movement-123919444|title=America's Hidden History: The Eugenics Movement {{!}} Learn Science at Scitable|website=www.nature.com|language=en|access-date=11 November 2017}}</ref> Goddard used the term "]" to refer to people who did not perform well on the test. He argued that "feeble-mindedness" was caused by heredity, and thus feeble-minded people should be prevented from giving birth, either by institutional isolation or sterilization surgeries.<ref name=":3" /> At first, sterilization targeted the disabled, but was later extended to poor people. Goddard's intelligence test was endorsed by the eugenicists to push for laws for forced sterilization. Different states adopted the sterilization laws at different paces. These laws, whose constitutionality was upheld by the Supreme Court in their 1927 ruling ], forced over 60,000 people to go through sterilization in the United States.<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.eugenicsarchive.org/html/eugenics/essay8text.html|title=Social Origins of Eugenics|website=www.eugenicsarchive.org|access-date=11 November 2017}}</ref>
The role of genes and environment (nature vs. nurture) in determining IQ is reviewed in Plomin ''et al.'' (2001, 2003). The degree to which genetic variation contributes to observed variation in a trait is measured by a statistic called ]. Heritability scores range from 0 to 1, and can be interpreted as the percentage of variation (e.g. in IQ) that is due to variation in genes. ] and adoption studies are commonly used to determine the heritability of a trait. Until recently heritability was mostly studied in children. These studies yield an estimate of heritability of 0.5; that is, half of the variation in IQ among the children studied was due to variation in their genes. The remaining half was thus due to environmental variation and measurement error. A heritability of 0.5 implies that IQ is "substantially" heritable. Studies with adults show that they have a higher heritability of IQ than children do and that heritability could be as high as 0.8, though it is probably not this high. The ]'s 1995 task force on "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" concluded that within the White population the heritability of IQ is “around .75” (p. 85).


California's sterilization program was so effective that the Nazis turned to the government for advice on how to prevent the birth of the "unfit".<ref name="HARNE">{{Cite web|url=http://hnn.us/article/1796 |title=The Horrifying American Roots of Nazi Eugenics |website=hnn.us |date=September 2003 |access-date=11 November 2017}}</ref> While the US eugenics movement lost much of its momentum in the 1940s in view of the horrors of Nazi Germany, advocates of eugenics (including Nazi geneticist ]) continued to work and promote their ideas in the United States.<ref name="HARNE" /> In later decades, some eugenic principles have made a resurgence as a voluntary means of selective reproduction, with some calling them "]".<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Vizcarrondo |first1=Felipe E. |title=Human Enhancement: The New Eugenics |journal=The Linacre Quarterly |date=August 2014 |volume=81 |issue=3 |pages=239–243 |doi=10.1179/2050854914Y.0000000021 |pmid=25249705 |pmc=4135459}}</ref> As it becomes possible to test for and correlate genes with IQ (and its proxies),<ref>{{cite web |last1=Regalado |first1=Antonio |title=Eugenics 2.0: We're at the Dawn of Choosing Embryos by Health, Height, and More |url=https://www.technologyreview.com/s/609204/eugenics-20-were-at-the-dawn-of-choosing-embryos-by-health-height-and-more/ |website=Technology Review |access-date=20 November 2019}}</ref> ethicists and embryonic ] companies are attempting to understand the ways in which the technology can be ethically deployed.<ref>{{cite web |last1=LeMieux |first1=Julianna |title=Polygenic Risk Scores and Genomic Prediction: Q&A with Stephen Hsu |url=https://www.genengnews.com/insights/polygenic-risk-scores-and-genomic-prediction-qa-with-stephen-hsu/ |website=Genetic Engineering and Biotechnology News |access-date=20 November 2019 |date=1 April 2019}}</ref>
Considerable research has focused on biological correlates of ''g''; see ] and the section on brain size below. For example, general intelligence and ] brain volume measurements are correlated, and the effect is primarily determined by genetic factors.


===Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory===
===Environment===
{{main|Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory}}
] defined ] and authored the ] IQ test.]]
] (1941) proposed two types of cognitive abilities in a revision of Spearman's concept of general intelligence. ] (Gf) was hypothesized as the ability to solve novel problems by using reasoning, and ] (Gc) was hypothesized as a knowledge-based ability that was very dependent on education and experience. In addition, fluid intelligence was hypothesized to decline with age, while crystallized intelligence was largely resistant to the effects of aging. The theory was almost forgotten, but was revived by his student ] (1966) who later argued Gf and Gc were only two among several factors, and who eventually identified nine or ten broad abilities. The theory continued to be called Gf-Gc theory.<ref name=Kaufman2009/>


] (1993), after a comprehensive reanalysis of earlier data, proposed the ], which is a hierarchical model with three levels. The bottom stratum consists of narrow abilities that are highly specialized (e.g., induction, spelling ability). The second stratum consists of broad abilities. Carroll identified eight second-stratum abilities. Carroll accepted Spearman's concept of general intelligence, for the most part, as a representation of the uppermost, third stratum.<ref name=Lubinski2004>{{cite journal |last1=Lubinski |first1=David |title=Introduction to the Special Section on Cognitive Abilities: 100 Years After Spearman's (1904) "'General Intelligence,' Objectively Determined and Measured" |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=2004 |volume=86 |issue=1 |pages=96–111 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.96 |pmid=14717630 |s2cid=6024297}}</ref>{{sfn|Carroll|1993|p={{page needed|date=October 2020}}}}
Environmental factors play a large role in determining IQ in situations where environmental conditions are variable. <!-- this is almost a tautology. Can we say "... in third world countries" to make it concrete? --> Proper childhood ] appears critical for ]; ] can lower IQ. Other research indicates environmental factors such as prenatal exposure to ]s, duration of ], and ] deficiency can affect IQ. However, in the developed world, none of these effects are sufficiently pronounced to be important. <!-- would rather say "... explain variance..." or something like that, but maybe it's too technical -->


In 1999, a merging of the Gf-Gc theory of Cattell and Horn with Carroll's Three-Stratum theory has led to the Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory (CHC Theory), with ''g'' as the top of the hierarchy, ten broad abilities below, and further subdivided into seventy narrow abilities on the third stratum. CHC Theory has greatly influenced many of the current broad IQ tests.<ref name=Kaufman2009/>
In the developed world, there is some environmental effect on the IQ of children, accounting for up to a quarter of the variance. However, by adulthood, this correlation disappears, so that the cognitive ability of adults living in the prevailing conditions of the developed world is highly heritable.


Modern tests do not necessarily measure all of these broad abilities. For example, ''quantitative knowledge'' and ''reading and writing ability'' may be seen as measures of school achievement and not IQ.<ref name=Kaufman2009/> ''Decision speed'' may be difficult to measure without special equipment. ''g'' was earlier often subdivided into only Gf and Gc, which were thought to correspond to the nonverbal or performance subtests and verbal subtests in earlier versions of the popular Wechsler IQ test. More recent research has shown the situation to be more complex.<ref name="Kaufman2009" /> Modern comprehensive IQ tests do not stop at reporting a single IQ score. Although they still give an overall score, they now also give scores for many of these more restricted abilities, identifying particular strengths and weaknesses of an individual.<ref name="Kaufman2009" />
Nearly all ] traits show that, contrary to expectations, environmental effects actually cause adoptive siblings raised in the same family to be as different as children raised in different families (Harris, 1998; Plomin & Daniels, 1987). Put another way, shared environmental variation for personality is zero, and all environmental effects are nonshared. Intelligence is actually an exception to this rule, at least among children. The IQs of adoptive siblings, who share no genetic relation but do share a common family environment, are correlated at .32. Despite attempts to isolate them, the factors that cause adoptive siblings to be similar have not been identified, though it could be related to parents choosing the type of children they will adopt. However, as explained below, shared family effects on IQ disappear after adolescence.


===Other theories===
Active genotype-environment correlation, also called the "nature of nurture", is observed for IQ. This phenomenon is measured similarly to heritability; but instead of measuring variation in IQ due to genes, variation in environment due to genes is determined. One study found that 40% of variation in measures of home environment are accounted for by genetic variation. This suggests that the way human beings craft their environment is due in part to genetic influences.
An alternative to standard IQ tests, meant to test the ] of children, originated in the writings of psychologist ] (1896–1934) during his last two years of his life.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Mindes|first1=Gayle|title=Assessing Young Children |date=2003 |publisher= Merrill/Prentice Hall |isbn=9780130929082 |page=158 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=x41LAAAAYAAJ}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Haywood|first1=H. Carl |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=xQekS_oqGzoC|title=Dynamic Assessment in Practice: Clinical and Educational Applications |last2=Lidz |first2=Carol S.|date=2006|publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=9781139462075 |page=1}}</ref> According to Vygotsky, the maximum level of complexity and difficulty of problems that a child is capable to solve under some guidance indicates their level of potential development. The difference between this level of potential and the lower level of unassisted performance indicates the child's zone of proximal development.<ref>{{cite book |url=http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1934/problem-age.htm |last=Vygotsky |first=L.S. |year=1934 |chapter=The Problem of Age |title=The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky, Volume 5 |publication-date=1998 |pages=187–205}}</ref> Combination of the two indexes{{--}}the level of actual and the zone of the proximal development{{--}}according to Vygotsky, provides a significantly more informative indicator of psychological development than the assessment of the level of actual development alone.<ref>{{cite book |last=Chaiklin |first=S. |year=2003 |chapter=The Zone of Proximal Development in Vygotsky's analysis of learning and instruction |editor-last1=Kozulin |editor-first1=A. |editor-last2=Gindis |editor-first2=B. |editor-last3=Ageyev |editor-first3=V. |editor-last4=Miller |editor-first4=S. |title=Vygotsky's educational theory and practice in cultural context |pages=39–64 |location=Cambridge |publisher=Cambridge University Press}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last=Zaretskii |first=V.K. |title=The Zone of Proximal Development What Vygotsky Did Not Have Time to Write |journal=] |volume=47 |issue=6 |date=November–December 2009 |pages=70–93|doi=10.2753/RPO1061-0405470604 |s2cid=146894219 }}</ref> His ideas on the zone of development were later developed in a number of psychological and educational theories and practices, most notably under the banner of ], which seeks to measure developmental potential<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Sternberg|first1=R.S. |last2=Grigorenko|first2=E.L.|year=2001|title=All testing is dynamic testing|journal=Issues in Education |volume=7 |issue=2|pages=137–170}}</ref><ref>Sternberg, R.J. & Grigorenko, E.L. (2002). Dynamic testing: The nature and measurement of learning potential. Cambridge: University of Cambridge</ref>{{sfn|Haywood|Lidz |2006|p={{page needed|date=October 2020}}}} (for instance, in the work of ] and his associates,<ref>Feuerstein, R., Feuerstein, S., Falik, L & Rand, Y. (1979; 2002). Dynamic assessments of cognitive modifiability. ICELP Press, Jerusalem: Israel</ref> who has ] for its putative assumption or acceptance of "fixed and immutable" characteristics of intelligence or cognitive functioning). Dynamic assessment has been further elaborated in the work of ], and ] and in theories of ] authored by ] and ].<ref>{{cite book |contributor=Dodge, Kenneth A. |contribution=Foreword |pages=xiii–xv |last1=Haywood |first1=H. Carl |last2=Lidz |first2=Carol S. |title=Dynamic Assessment in Practice: Clinical And Educational Applications |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2006}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last=Kozulin |first=A. |year=2014 |chapter=Dynamic assessment in search of its identity |editor-last1=Yasnitsky |editor-first1=A. |editor-last2=van der Veer |editor-first2=R. |editor-last3=Ferrari |editor-first3=M. |title=The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology |publisher=Cambridge University Press |pages=126–147}}</ref>


]'s ] (1967) model of intelligence used three dimensions, which, when combined, yielded a total of 120 types of intelligence. It was popular in the 1970s and early 1980s, but faded owing to both practical problems and ] criticisms.<ref name="Kaufman2009" />
===Development===


]'s earlier work on neuropsychological processes led to the PASS theory (1997). It argued that only looking at one general factor was inadequate for researchers and clinicians who worked with learning disabilities, attention disorders, intellectual disability, and interventions for such disabilities. The PASS model covers four kinds of processes (planning process, attention/arousal process, simultaneous processing, and successive processing). The planning processes involve decision making, problem solving, and performing activities and require goal setting and self-monitoring.
It is reasonable to expect that genetic influences on traits like IQ should become less important as we gain experiences with age. Surprisingly, the opposite occurs. Heritability measures in infancy are as low as 20%, around 40% in middle childhood, and as high as 80% in adulthood.


The attention/arousal process involves selectively attending to a particular stimulus, ignoring distractions, and maintaining vigilance. Simultaneous processing involves the integration of stimuli into a group and requires the observation of relationships. Successive processing involves the integration of stimuli into serial order. The planning and attention/arousal components comes from structures located in the frontal lobe, and the simultaneous and successive processes come from structures located in the posterior region of the cortex.<ref name=Das1975>{{Cite journal |author1=Das, J.P. |author2=Kirby, J. |author3=Jarman, R.F. |year=1975 |title=Simultaneous and successive synthesis: An alternative model for cognitive abilities |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=82 |pages=87–103 |doi=10.1037/h0076163}}</ref><ref name=Das2002>{{Cite journal |author=Das, J.P. |year=2000 |title=A better look at intelligence |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |volume=11 |pages=28–33 |doi=10.1111/1467-8721.00162|s2cid=146129242}}</ref><ref name= Naglieri1990>{{Cite journal |author1=Naglieri, J.A. |author2=Das, J.P. |year=2002 |title=Planning, attention, simultaneous, and successive cognitive processes as a model for assessment |journal=School Psychology Review |volume=19 |issue=4 |pages=423–442|doi=10.1080/02796015.1990.12087349 }}</ref> It has influenced some recent IQ tests, and been seen as a complement to the Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory described above.<ref name=Kaufman2009/>
Shared family effects also seem to disappear by adulthood. Adoption studies show that, after adolescence, adopted siblings are no more similar in IQ than strangers (IQ correlation near zero), while full siblings show an IQ correlation of 0.6. Twin studies reinforce this pattern: ] raised separately are highly similar in IQ (0.86), more so than ] raised together (0.6) and much more than adopted siblings (~0.0).


==Current tests==
Most of the IQ studies described above were conducted in developed countries, such as the ], ], and ]. Also, a few studies have been conducted in Moscow, East Germany, and India, and those studies produce similar results. Any such investigation is limited to describing the genetic and environmental variation found within the populations studied. This is a caveat of any heritability study.
]
There are a variety of individually administered IQ tests in use in the English-speaking world.<ref name="Urbina2011Table2.1" /><ref name="FlanaganHarrison2012chs8-16" /><ref>{{Citation |last1=Stanek |first1=Kevin C. |title=Taxonomies and Compendia of Cognitive Ability and Personality Constructs and Measures Relevant to Industrial, Work and Organizational Psychology |date=2018 |url=http://sk.sagepub.com/reference/the-sage-handbook-of-industrial-work-and-org-psychology-vol1/i3345.xml |work=The SAGE Handbook of Industrial, Work and Organizational Psychology: Personnel Psychology and Employee Performance |pages=366–407 |access-date=2024-01-08 |place=1 Oliver's Yard, 55 City Road London EC1Y 1SP |publisher=SAGE Publications Ltd |doi=10.4135/9781473914940.n14 |isbn=978-1-4462-0721-5 |last2=Ones |first2=Deniz S.}}</ref> The most commonly used individual IQ test series is the ] (WAIS) for adults and the ] (WISC) for school-age test-takers. Other commonly used individual IQ tests (some of which do not label their standard scores as "IQ" scores) include the current versions of the ], ], the ], the ], and the ].


There are various other IQ tests, including:
===Mental retardation===
# ] (RPM)
# ] (CFIT)
# ] (RIAS)
# ]<ref>{{Cite web|title = Primary Mental Abilities Test {{!}} psychological test|url = https://www.britannica.com/topic/Primary-Mental-Abilities-Test|website = Encyclopædia Britannica|access-date = 26 November 2015}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|title = Defining and Measuring Psychological Attributes|url = http://homepages.rpi.edu/~verwyc/TESTOH2.htm|website = homepages.rpi.edu|access-date = 26 November 2015|archive-date = 15 October 2018 |archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20181015132922/http://homepages.rpi.edu/~verwyc/TESTOH2.htm|url-status = dead}}</ref>
# ] (KBIT)<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Test Review: Review of Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition. Bloomington, MN: Pearson, Inc|journal = Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment|date = 1 April 2010|issn = 0734-2829|pages = 167–174|volume = 28|issue = 2|doi = 10.1177/0734282909348217|first1 = Sherry K.|last1 = Bain|first2 = Kathryn E.|last2 = Jaspers|s2cid = 143961429}}</ref>
# ]
# ] (CAS)
# ] (NNAT)
#] (WRIT)


IQ scales are ].<ref>{{cite book|title=Psychology: An Introduction |last=Mussen |first=Paul Henry |year=1973 |publisher=Heath |location=Lexington, MA |isbn=978-0-669-61382-7 |page= |quote=The I.Q. is essentially a rank; there are no true "units" of intellectual ability. |url=https://archive.org/details/psychologyintrod00muss/page/363}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |title=The WISC-III Companion: A Guide to Interpretation and Educational Intervention |last=Truch |first=Steve |year=1993 |publisher=Pro-Ed |location=Austin, TX |isbn=978-0-89079-585-9 |page=35 |quote=An IQ score is not an equal-interval score, as is evident in Table A.4 in the WISC-III manual.}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |title=Measuring Intelligence: Facts and Fallacies |url=https://archive.org/details/measuringintelli00bart |url-access=limited |last=Bartholomew |first=David J. |author-link=D.J. Bartholomew |year=2004 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge |isbn=978-0-521-54478-8 |quote=When we come to quantities like IQ or g, as we are presently able to measure them, we shall see later that we have an even lower level of measurement—an ordinal level. This means that the numbers we assign to individuals can only be used to rank them—the number tells us where the individual comes in the rank order and nothing else. |page=}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998|pp=}} "In the jargon of psychological measurement theory, IQ is an ordinal scale, where we are simply rank-ordering people. ... It is not even appropriate to claim that the 10-point difference between IQ scores of 110 and 100 is the same as the 10-point difference between IQs of 160 and 150"</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last=Stevens |first=S. S. |author-link=Stanley Smith Stevens |title=On the Theory of Scales of Measurement |journal=] |volume=103 |issue=2684 |pages=677–680 |year=1946 |pmid=17750512 |doi=10.1126/science.103.2684.677 |bibcode=1946Sci...103..677S |s2cid=4667599}}</ref> The ] of the norming ] is usually (rank order) ] to a ] with mean 100 and ] 15.<ref name="Gottfredson2009pp31–32" /> While one ] is 15 points, and two SDs are 30 points, and so on, this does not imply that mental ability is linearly related to IQ, such that IQ 50 would mean half the cognitive ability of IQ 100. In particular, IQ points are not percentage points.
About 75&ndash;80 percent of ] is familial (runs in families), and 20&ndash;25 percent is due to organic problems, such as chromosomal abnormalities or brain damage. Mild to severe mental retardation is a symptom of several hundred single-gene disorders and many chromosomal abnormalities, including small deletions. Based on twin studies, moderate to severe mental retardation does not appear to be familial, but mild mental retardation does. That is, the relatives of the moderate to severely mentally retarded have normal ranges of IQs, whereas the families of the mildly mentally retarded have IQs skewing lower.


==Reliability and validity==
IQ score ranges (from DSM-IV):
===Reliability===
* mild mental retardation: IQ 50&ndash;55 to 70; children require mild support; formally called "Educable Mentally Retarded".
{|class="wikitable sortable" style="font-size:small; float:right; text-align:center; margin:0 0 0.5em 1em" summary="Sortable table showing actual I.Q. scores of twelve students on three different I.Q. tests, with students identified by pseudonyms in cited data source."
* moderate retardation: IQ 35&ndash;40 to 50&ndash;55; children require moderate supervision and assistance; formally called "Trainable Mentally Retarded".
|+ IQ scores can differ to some degree for the same person on different IQ tests, so a person does not always belong to the same IQ score range each time the person is tested. (IQ score table data and pupil pseudonyms adapted from description of KABC-II norming study cited in {{harvp|Kaufman|2009}}.<ref name="Kaufman2009Fig5.1" /><ref name="KaufmanSB2013Fig3.1" />)
* severe mental retardation: IQ 20&ndash;25 to 35&ndash;40; can be taught basic life skills and simple tasks with supervision.
|-
* profound mental retardation: IQ below 20&ndash;25; usually caused by a neurological condition; require constant care.
! class="unsortable" |Pupil!!KABC-II!!WISC-III!!WJ-III
|-
|A||90||95||111
|-
|B||125||110||105
|-
|C||100||93||101
|-
|D||116||127||118
|-
|E||93||105||93
|-
|F||106||105||105
|-
|G||95||100||90
|-
|H||112||113||103
|-
|I||104||96||97
|-
|J||101||99||86
|-
|K||81||78||75
|-
|L||116||124||102
|}


Psychometricians generally regard IQ tests as having high ].{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}<ref name="Mackintosh2011p169">{{Harvnb |Mackintosh|2011|page=169}} "after the age of 8–10, IQ scores remain relatively stable: the correlation between IQ scores from age 8 to 18 and IQ at age 40 is over 0.70."</ref> Reliability represents the measurement consistency of a test.<ref name="Weiten">{{cite book|vauthors= Weiten W|title=Psychology: Themes and Variations |publisher=]|year=2016|page=281|isbn=978-1305856127 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ALkaCgAAQBAJ&pg=PT331}}</ref> A reliable test produces similar scores upon repetition.<ref name="Weiten"/> On aggregate, IQ tests exhibit high reliability, although test-takers may have varying scores when taking the same test on differing occasions, and may have varying scores when taking different IQ tests at the same age. Like all statistical quantities, any particular estimate of IQ has an associated standard error that measures uncertainty about the estimate. For modern tests, the confidence interval can be approximately 10 points and reported ] can be as low as about three points.<ref>{{cite web |title=WISC-V Interpretive Report Sample |website=Pearson |url=https://images.pearsonclinical.com/images/assets/wisc-v/WISC-VInterpretiveReportSample-1.pdf |access-date=29 September 2020 |pages=18 |archive-date=22 April 2022 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220422190842/https://images.pearsonclinical.com/images/assets/wisc-v/WISC-VInterpretiveReportSample-1.pdf |url-status=dead }}</ref> Reported standard error may be an underestimate, as it does not account for all sources of error.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Kaufman |first1=Alan S. |last2=Raiford |first2=Susan Engi |last3=Coalson |first3=Diane L. |year=2016 |title=Intelligent testing with the WISC-V |publisher=Wiley |location=Hoboken, NJ |isbn=978-1-118-58923-6 |pages=683–702 |quote=Reliability estimates in Table 4.1 and standard errors of measurement in Table 4.4 should be considered best-case estimates because they do not consider other major sources of error, such as transient error, administration error, or scoring error (Hanna, Bradley, & Holen, 1981), which influence test scores in clinical assessments. Another factor that must be considered is the extent to which subtest scores reflect portions of true score variance due to a hierarchical general intelligence factor and variance due to specific group factors because these sources of true score variance are conflated.}}</ref>
The rate of mental retardation is higher among males than females, and higher among blacks than whites, according to a 1991 U.S. ] (CDC) study.


Outside influences such as low motivation or high anxiety can occasionally lower a person's IQ test score.<ref name="Weiten" /> For individuals with very low scores, the 95% confidence interval may be greater than 40 points, potentially complicating the accuracy of diagnoses of intellectual disability.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Whitaker |first1=Simon |title=Error in the estimation of intellectual ability in the low range using the WISC-IV and WAIS-III |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |date=April 2010 |volume=48 |issue=5 |pages=517–521 |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222824571 |access-date=22 January 2020 |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.017}}</ref> By the same token, high IQ scores are also significantly less reliable than those near to the population median.<ref>{{harvnb|Lohman|Foley Nicpon|2012|p={{page needed|date=October 2020}}}}. "The concerns associated with SEMs are actually substantially worse for scores at the extremes of the distribution, especially when scores approach the maximum possible on a test ... when students answer most of the items correctly. In these cases, errors of measurement for scale scores will increase substantially at the extremes of the distribution. Commonly the SEM is from two to four times larger for very high scores than for scores near the mean (Lord, 1980)."</ref> Reports of IQ scores much higher than 160 are considered dubious.<ref>{{harvnb|Urbina|2011|p=20}} " is just one of the reasons to be suspicious of reported IQ scores much higher than 160"</ref>
By race, the overall rate was 16.6 per 1000 for blacks and 6.8 per 1000 for whites. Rates of mental retardation for black males, the group with the highest rates, were 1.7 times higher than black females, 2.4 times higher than white males, and 3.1 times higher than white females. <!--The following statement isn't clear or notable in its present form: Mild mental retardation is almost never diagnosed until a person enters elementary school, which critics claim lends strong support to the notion that IQ tests are racially biased.-->


===Validity as a measure of intelligence===
Individuals with IQs below 70 have been exempted from the death penalty in the U.S. since 2002.
Reliability and validity are very different concepts. While reliability reflects reproducibility, validity refers to whether the test measures what it purports to measure.<ref name="Weiten" /> While IQ tests are generally considered to measure some forms of intelligence, they may fail to serve as an accurate measure of broader definitions of ] inclusive of, for example, ] and ]. For this reason, psychologist Wayne Weiten argues that their ] must be carefully qualified, and not be overstated.<ref name="Weiten" /> According to Weiten, "IQ tests are valid measures of the kind of intelligence necessary to do well in academic work. But if the purpose is to assess intelligence in a broader sense, the validity of IQ tests is questionable."<ref name="Weiten" />


Some scientists have disputed the value of IQ as a measure of intelligence altogether. In '']'' (1981, expanded edition 1996), ] ] compared IQ testing with the now-discredited practice of determining intelligence via ], arguing that both are based on the fallacy of ], "our tendency to convert abstract concepts into entities".<ref name="TMoMp24">{{harvnb|Gould|1981|p=24}}. {{harvnb|Gould|1996|p=}}.</ref> Gould's argument sparked a great deal of debate,<ref name="Kaplan et al">{{cite journal|last1=Kaplan|first1=Jonathan Michael |last2=Pigliucci|first2=Massimo|last3=Banta|first3=Joshua Alexander|year=2015|title=Gould on Morton, Redux: What can the debate reveal about the limits of data? |journal=Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences |url=http://philpapers.org/archive/KAPGOM.pdf |volume=30|pages=1–10}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Weisberg|first1=Michael|last2=Paul|first2=Diane B.|date=19 April 2016 |title=Morton, Gould, and Bias: A Comment on "The Mismeasure of Science" |journal=PLOS Biology |volume=14 |issue=4 |at=e1002444 |doi=10.1371/journal.pbio.1002444 |issn=1544-9173 |pmc=4836680 |pmid=27092558 |doi-access=free }}</ref> and the book is listed as one of '']''{{'}}s "25 Greatest Science Books of All Time".<ref>{{cite magazine |date=7 December 2006 |url=https://www.discovermagazine.com/the-sciences/25-greatest-science-books-of-all-time |title=25 Greatest Science Books of All Time |magazine=Discover}}</ref>
===IQ, education, and income===


Along these same lines, critics such as ] do not dispute the capacity of IQ test scores to predict some kinds of achievement, but argue that basing a concept of intelligence on IQ test scores alone neglects other important aspects of mental ability.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}<ref>] (14 September 2007). . '']''.</ref> ], another significant critic of IQ as the main measure of human cognitive abilities, argued that reducing the concept of intelligence to the measure of ''g'' does not fully account for the different skills and knowledge types that produce success in human society.<ref>Sternberg, Robert J., and Richard K. Wagner. "The g-ocentric view of intelligence and job performance is wrong." Current directions in psychological science (1993): 1–5.</ref>
Tambs ''et al.'' (1989) found that occupational status, educational attainment, and IQ are individually heritable; and further found that "genetic variance influencing educational attainment &hellip; contributed approximately one-fourth of the genetic variance for occupational status and nearly half the genetic variance for IQ". In a sample of US siblings, Rowe ''et al.'' (1997) report that the inequality in education and income was predominantly due to genes, with shared environmental factors playing a subordinate role.


Despite these objections, clinical psychologists generally regard IQ scores as having sufficient ] for many clinical purposes.{{Specify |reason=Quick summary of which clinical purposes it's used for, and ideally some of the limitations. Is this sentence better suited for the reliability section?|date=October 2020}}<ref name="Kaufman2009"/>{{sfn|Anastasi|Urbina|1997|pp=326–327}}
===Regression===
The heritability of IQ determines the extent to which the IQ of children will be similar to the IQ of parents. Because the heritability of IQ is less than 100%, the IQ of children tends to "regress" towards the mean IQ of the population. That is, high IQ parents tend to have children who are less bright than their parents, whereas low IQ parents tend to have children who are brighter than their parents. The effect can be quantified by the equation <math>\hat y = \bar x + h^2 \left ( \frac{\mbox{mom} + \mbox{dad}}{2} - \bar x \right)</math> where:
* <math>\hat y</math> is the predicted average IQ of Mom and Dad's children
* <math>\bar x</math> is the mean IQ of the population that Mom and Dad come from
* <math>h^2</math> is the heritability of IQ
Thus, if the heritability of IQ is 50%, a couple with an average IQ of 120 may have children that average around an IQ of 110, assuming that both parents come from a population with a median IQ of 100.


===Test bias or differential item functioning===
==IQ and the Brain==
Differential item functioning (DIF), sometimes referred to as measurement bias, is a phenomenon when participants from different groups (e.g. gender, race, disability) with the same ] give different answers to specific questions on the same IQ test.<ref>Embretson, S. E., Reise, S. P. (2000).''Item Response Theory for Psychologists''. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.</ref> DIF analysis measures such specific items on a test alongside measuring participants' latent abilities on other similar questions. A consistent different group response to a specific question among similar types of questions can indicate an effect of DIF. It does not count as differential item functioning if both groups have an equally valid chance of giving different responses to the same questions. Such bias can be a result of culture, educational level and other factors that are independent of group traits. DIF is only considered if test-takers from different groups ''with the same underlying ] ability level'' have a different chance of giving specific responses.<ref name=":1">{{cite journal |last1=Zumbo|first1=B.D. |year=2007|title=Three generations of differential item functioning (DIF) analyses: Considering where it has been, where it is now, and where it is going |journal=Language Assessment Quarterly |volume=4 |issue=2 |pages=223–233|doi=10.1080/15434300701375832|s2cid=17426415}}</ref> Such questions are usually removed in order to make the test equally fair for both groups. Common techniques for analyzing DIF are ] (IRT) based methods, Mantel-Haenszel, and ].<ref name=":1" />
{{main|Neuroscience and intelligence}}
===Brain size and IQ===
Modern studies using ] imaging have shown that brain size correlates with IQ by a factor of approximately .40 among adults (McDaniel, 2005). The correlation between brain size and IQ seems to hold for comparisons between and within families (Gignac et al. 2003; Jensen 1994; Jensen & Johnson 1994). However, one study found no familial correlation (Schoenemann et al. 2000). A ] (Thompson ''et al.'', 2001) showed that frontal ] volume was correlated with ''g'' and highly heritable. A related study has reported that the correlation between brain size (reported to have a ] of 0.85) and ''g'' is 0.4, and that correlation is mediated entirely by genetic factors (Posthuma et al 2002).


A 2005 study found that "differential validity in prediction suggests that the ] test may contain cultural influences that reduce the validity of the WAIS-R as a measure of cognitive ability for Mexican American students,"<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Verney|first1=SP|last2=Granholm |first2=E|last3=Marshall|first3=SP|last4=Malcarne|first4=VL|last5=Saccuzzo|first5=DP|year=2005 |title=Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment: Information Processing and Psychophysiological Approaches |journal=Assessment|volume=12|issue=3|pages=303–19|doi=10.1177/1073191105276674|pmid=16123251 |s2cid=31024437}}</ref> indicating a weaker positive correlation relative to sampled white students. Other recent studies have questioned the culture-fairness of IQ tests when used in South Africa.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Shuttleworth-Edwards|first1=Ann|last2=Kemp|first2=Ryan|last3=Rust |first3=Annegret |last4=Muirhead|first4=Joanne|last5=Hartman|first5=Nigel|last6=Radloff|first6=Sarah|year=2004|title=Cross-cultural Effects on IQ Test Performance: AReview and Preliminary Normative Indications on WAIS-III Test Performance |journal=Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology|volume=26|issue=7|pages=903–20 |doi=10.1080/13803390490510824 |pmid=15742541 |s2cid=16060622}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Cronshaw |first1=Steven F. |last2=Hamilton|first2=Leah K.|last3=Onyura|first3=Betty R. |last4=Winston |first4=Andrew S. |year=2006|title=Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons (2004)|journal=International Journal of Selection and Assessment |volume=14|issue=3|pages=278–87|doi=10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00346.x |s2cid=91179275}}</ref> Standard intelligence tests, such as the Stanford–Binet, are often inappropriate for ] children; the alternative of using developmental or adaptive skills measures are relatively poor measures of intelligence in autistic children, and may have resulted in incorrect claims that a majority of autistic children are of low intelligence.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Edelson|first1=M. G. |year=2006|title=Are the Majority of Children With Autism Mentally Retarded?: A Systematic Evaluation of the Data|journal=Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities |volume=21|issue=2|pages=66–83 |doi=10.1177/10883576060210020301|s2cid=145809356}}</ref>
===Brain areas associated with IQ===
Many different sources of information have converged on the view that the frontal lobes are critical for fluid intelligence. Patients with damage to the frontal lobe are impaired on fluid intelligence tests (Duncan et al 1995). The volume of frontal grey (Thompson et al 2001) and white matter (Schoenemann et al 2005) have also been associated with intelligence. In addition, recent neuroimaging studies have limited this association to the lateral prefrontal cortex. Duncan and colleagues (2000) showed using Positron Emission Tomography that problem-solving tasks that correlated more highly with IQ also activate the lateral prefrontal cortex. More recently, Gray and colleagues (2003) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to show that those individuals that were more adept at resisting distraction on a demanding working memory task had both a higher IQ and increased prefrontal activity. For a review of this topic, see Gray and Thompson (2004).


==The Flynn effect== ===Flynn effect===
{{main|Flynn effect}} {{Main|Flynn effect}}


Since the early 20th century, raw scores on IQ tests have increased in most parts of the world.<ref name="Neisser1998">{{Cite book|title=The Rising Curve: Long-Term Gains in IQ and Related Measures |editor-last=Neisser |editor-first=Ulric |display-authors=8 |author1=Ulric Neisser |author2=James R. Flynn |author3=Carmi Schooler |author4=Patricia M. Greenfield |author5=Wendy M. Williams |author6=Marian Sigman |author7=Shannon E. Whaley |author8=Reynaldo Martorell |author9=Richard Lynn |author10=Robert M. Hauser |author11=David W. Grissmer |author12=Stephanie Williamson |author13=Sheila Nataraj Kirby |author14=Mark Berends |author15=Stephen J. Ceci |author16=Tina B. Rosenblum |author17=Matthew Kumpf |author18=Min-Hsiung Huang |author19=Irwin D. Waldman |author20=Samuel H. Preston |author21=John C. Loehlin |year=1998 |publisher=American Psychological Association |location=Washington, DC |isbn=978-1-55798-503-3 |series=APA Science Volume Series |url=https://archive.org/details/risingcurvelongt00neis}}{{Page needed|date=January 2011}}</ref>{{sfn|Mackintosh|1998|p={{Page needed|date=January 2011}}}}{{sfn|Flynn|2009|p={{Page needed|date=January 2011}}}} When a new version of an IQ test is normed, the standard scoring is set so performance at the population median results in a score of IQ 100. The phenomenon of rising raw score performance means if test-takers are scored by a constant standard scoring rule, IQ test scores have been rising at an average rate of around three IQ points per decade. This phenomenon was named the Flynn effect in the book '']'' after ], the author who did the most to bring this phenomenon to the attention of psychologists.<ref name="Flynn1984">{{cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=James R. |title=The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 1932 to 1978. |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=95 |issue=1 |pages=29–51 |year=1984 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.29 |s2cid=51999517}}</ref><ref name="Flynn1987">{{cite journal |last1=Flynn |pages=171–91 |first1=James R. |issue=2 |volume=101 |year=1987 |title=Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171 |journal=Psychological Bulletin}}</ref>
Worldwide, IQ scores appear to be slowly rising, a trend known as the Flynn effect. However, tests are only renormalized occasionally to obtain mean scores of 100, for example WISC-R (1974), WISC-III (1991) and WISC-IV (2003). Hence it is difficult to compare IQ scores measured years apart.


Researchers have been exploring the issue of whether the Flynn effect is equally strong on performance of all kinds of IQ test items, whether the effect may have ended in some developed nations, whether there are social subgroup differences in the effect, and what possible causes of the effect might be.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Zhou |first1=Xiaobin |last2=Grégoire |first2=Jacques |last3=Zhu |first3=Jianjin |title=WAIS-IV Clinical Use and Interpretation: Scientist-Practitioner Perspectives |editor1-last=Weiss |editor1-first=Lawrence G. |editor2-last=Saklofske |editor2-first=Donald H. |editor3-last=Coalson |editor3-first=Diane |editor4-last=Raiford |editor4-first=Susan |chapter=The Flynn Effect and the Wechsler Scales |year=2010 |publisher=Academic Press |location=Amsterdam |series=Practical Resources for the Mental Health Professional |isbn=978-0-12-375035-8}}{{Page needed|date=January 2011}}</ref> A 2011 textbook, ''IQ and Human Intelligence'', by ], noted the Flynn effect demolishes the fears that IQ would be decreased. He also asks whether it represents a real increase in intelligence beyond IQ scores.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|pp=25–27}} A 2011 psychology textbook, lead authored by Harvard Psychologist Professor ], noted that humans' inherited intelligence could be ] while acquired intelligence goes up.<ref>{{cite book |first1=Daniel L. |last1=Schacter |first2=Daniel T. |last2=Gilbert |first3=Daniel M. |last3=Wegner |title=Psychology|date=2011 |publisher=Palgrave Macmillan |location=Basingstoke |isbn=978-0230579835 |page=384}}</ref>
==IQ correlations==


Research has suggested that the Flynn effect has slowed or reversed course in some Western countries beginning in the late 20th century. The phenomenon has been termed the ''negative Flynn effect''.<ref name=":4">{{Cite journal|last1=Bratsberg|first1=Bernt|last2=Rogeberg|first2=Ole|date=26 June 2018|title=Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|volume=115 |issue=26|pages=6674–6678|doi=10.1073/pnas.1718793115|pmid=29891660|pmc=6042097|bibcode=2018PNAS..115.6674B |doi-access=free}}</ref> A study of Norwegian military conscripts' test records found that IQ scores have been falling for generations born after the year 1975, and that the underlying cause of both initial increasing and subsequent falling trends appears to be environmental rather than genetic.<ref name=":4" />
===Race and IQ===
{{main|Race and intelligence}}
<!--Please read the race and intelligence article thoroughly before contributing to this section-->


===Age===
While the distributions of IQ scores among different racial-ethnic groups overlap considerably, groups differ in where their members cluster along the IQ scale. Some groups (e.g. East Asians and Jews) cluster higher than whites, while other groups (e.g. Blacks and Hispanics) cluster lower than whites. Similar clustering occurs with related variables, such as school achievement, reaction time, and brain size. Many hypotheses have been proposed to explain racial-ethnic group differences in IQ. Test bias nor simple differences in socioeconomic status partly explain the IQ differences. The remaining focus of the scientific debate is whether group differences are entirely caused by environmental factors or whether they also reflect a genetic component. The findings of this field are often thought to conflict with fundamental social philosophies, and have thus engendered a large controversy.
] is largely credited with the idea that IQ heritability rises with age.<ref>{{Cite journal |date=May 1987 |title=Ronald S. Wilson (1933–1986) |url=https://link.springer.com/10.1007/BF01065501 |journal=Behavior Genetics |language=en |volume=17 |issue=3 |pages=211–217 |doi=10.1007/BF01065501 |issn=0001-8244}}</ref> Researchers building on this phenomenon dubbed it "The Wilson Effect," named after the behavioral geneticist.<ref name=":14">{{Cite journal |last=Bouchard |first=Thomas J. |date=October 2013 |title=The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S1832427413000546/type/journal_article |journal=Twin Research and Human Genetics |language=en |volume=16 |issue=5 |pages=923–930 |doi=10.1017/thg.2013.54 |issn=1832-4274}}</ref> A paper by ], examining twin and adoption studies, including twins "reared apart," finds that IQ "reaches an asymptote at about 0.80 at 18–20 years of age and continuing at that level well into adulthood. In the aggregate, the studies also confirm that shared environmental influence decreases across age, approximating about 0.10 at 18–20 years of age and continuing at that level into adulthood."<ref name=":14" /> IQ can change to some degree over the course of childhood.{{sfn|Kaufman|2009|pp=–222}} In one ], the mean IQ scores of tests at ages 17 and 18 were correlated at ] with the mean scores of tests at ages five, six, and seven and at {{nowrap|1=''r'' = 0.96}}{{Explain|date=October 2020|reason=Please provide context to r correlation values. Are 0.86 and 0.96 good? How do they compare with correlation at older ages?}} with the mean scores of tests at ages 11, 12, and 13.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}


The current consensus is that ] generally declines with age after early adulthood, while ] remains intact.{{sfn|Kaufman|2009|loc="Chapter 8"|p={{Page needed|date=January 2011}}}} However, the exact peak age of fluid intelligence or crystallized intelligence remains elusive. Cross-sectional studies usually show that especially fluid intelligence peaks at a relatively young age (often in the early adulthood) while longitudinal data mostly show that intelligence is stable until mid-adulthood or later. Subsequently, intelligence seems to decline slowly.<ref name="DesjardinsWarnke2012">{{cite journal |last1=Desjardins |first1=Richard |last2=Warnke |first2=Arne Jonas |year=2012 |title=Ageing and Skills |url=http://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/education/ageing-and-skills_5k9csvw87ckh-en |journal=OECD Education Working Papers |doi=10.1787/5k9csvw87ckh-en |doi-access=free |hdl-access=free |hdl=10419/57089}}</ref>
===Religiosity and IQ===
{{main|Religiosity and intelligence}}


For decades, practitioners' handbooks and textbooks on IQ testing have reported IQ declines with age after the beginning of adulthood. However, later researchers pointed out this phenomenon is related to the ] and is in part a ] effect rather than a true aging effect. A variety of studies of IQ and aging have been conducted since the norming of the first Wechsler Intelligence Scale drew attention to IQ differences in different age groups of adults. Both cohort effects (the birth year of the test-takers) and practice effects (test-takers taking the same form of IQ test more than once) must be controlled to gain accurate data.{{Inconsistent|date=October 2020|reason=Resolve the distinction between IQ (which, by definition, is age-normalized) and intelligence (which IQ attempts to measure) in this section.}} It is unclear whether any lifestyle intervention can preserve fluid intelligence into older ages.{{sfn|Kaufman|2009|loc="Chapter 8"|p={{Page needed|date=January 2011}}}}
Several studies show an inverse correlation between IQ and degree of religious belief, while other research indicates a positive correlation between intelligence and religiosity.


===Health and IQ=== ==Genetics and environment==
] and ] factors play a role in determining IQ. Their relative importance has been the subject of much research and debate.<ref>{{citation|title=Continuity of Genetic and Environmental Influences on Cognition across the Life Span: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Twin and Adoption Studies|first1=Elliot M|last1=Tucker-Drob|first2=Daniel A|last2=Briley|journal=Psychological Bulletin|year=2014|volume=140|issue=4|pages=949–979|doi=10.1037/a0035893|pmc=4069230|pmid=24611582}}</ref>


===Heritability===
Persons with a higher IQ have generally lower adult morbidity and mortality. This may be because they better avoid injury and take better care of their own health. It also decreases the risk of ], severe ], and ]. On the other hand, it increases the risk of ] .
{{See also|Heritability of IQ|Environment and intelligence}}


The general figure for the ] of IQ, according to an ] report, is 0.45 for children, and rises to around 0.75 for late adolescents and adults.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}} Heritability measures for ] in infancy are as low as 0.2, around 0.4 in middle childhood, and as high as 0.9 in adulthood.<ref name="BouchardWilson2013">{{cite journal |last1=Bouchard |first1=Thomas J. |title=The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age |journal=Twin Research and Human Genetics |date=7 August 2013 |volume=16 |issue=5 |pages=923–930 |doi=10.1017/thg.2013.54 |pmid=23919982 |s2cid=13747480|doi-access=free }}</ref><ref name="PanizzonVuoksimaa2014">{{cite journal |last1=Panizzon |first1=Matthew S. |last2=Vuoksimaa |first2=Eero |last3=Spoon |first3=Kelly M. |last4=Jacobson |first4=Kristen C. |last5=Lyons |first5=Michael J. |last6=Franz |first6=Carol E. |last7=Xian |first7=Hong |last8=Vasilopoulos |first8=Terrie |last9=Kremen |first9=William S. |date=March 2014 |title=Genetic and environmental influences on general cognitive ability: Is ''g'' a valid latent construct? |journal=Intelligence |volume=43 |pages=65–76 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.008 |pmc=4002017 |pmid=24791031}}</ref> One proposed explanation is that people with different genes tend to reinforce the effects of those genes, for example by seeking out different environments.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}<ref>{{cite journal |display-authors=8 |last1=Huguet |first1=Guillaume |last2=Schramm |first2=Catherine |last3=Douard |first3=Elise |last4=Jiang |first4=Lai |last5=Labbe |first5=Aurélie |last6=Tihy |first6=Frédérique |last7=Mathonnet |first7=Géraldine |last8=Nizard |first8=Sonia |last9=Lemyre |first9=Emmanuelle |last10=Mathieu |first10=Alexandre |last11=Poline |first11=Jean-Baptiste |last12=Loth |first12=Eva |last13=Toro |first13=Roberto |last14=Schumann |first14=Gunter |last15=Conrod |first15=Patricia |last16=Pausova |first16=Zdenka |last17=Greenwood |first17=Celia |last18=Paus |first18=Tomas |last19=Bourgeron |first19=Thomas |last20=Jacquemont |first20=Sébastien |author21=IMAGEN Consortium |title=Measuring and Estimating the Effect Sizes of Copy Number Variants on General Intelligence in Community-Based Samples |journal=JAMA Psychiatry |date=May 2018 |volume=75 |issue=5 |pages=447–457 |doi=10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0039 |pmid=29562078 |pmc=5875373}}</ref>
Research in Scotland has shown that a 15-point lower IQ meant people had a fifth less chance of seeing their 76th birthday, while those with a 30-point disadvantage were 37% less likely than those with a higher IQ to live that long .


===Economic development and IQ=== ===Shared family environment===
Family members have aspects of environments in common (for example, characteristics of the home). This shared family environment accounts for 0.25–0.35 of the variation in IQ in childhood. By late adolescence, it is quite low (zero in some studies). The effect for several other psychological traits is similar. These studies have not looked at the effects of extreme environments, such as in abusive families.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bouchard |first1=TJ Jr. |title=Genetic and environmental influences on adult intelligence and special mental abilities |journal=Human Biology; an International Record of Research |volume=70 |issue=2 |pages=257–79 |year=1998 |pmid=9549239}}</ref><ref name="Plomin0103">{{cite journal |last1=Plomin |first1=R |last2=Asbury |first2=K |last3=Dunn |first3=J |title=Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later |journal=Canadian Journal of Psychiatry |volume=46 |issue=3 |pages=225–33 |year=2001 |pmid=11320676|doi=10.1177/070674370104600302 |doi-access=free}}</ref>{{sfn|Harris|2009|p={{page needed|date=October 2020}}}}
A controversial book '']'', claims to show that the wealth of a nation can in large part be explained by the average IQ score. This claim has been both disputed and supported in peer-reviewed papers. The data used has also been questioned.


===Non-shared family environment and environment outside the family===
==Practical validity==
Although parents treat their children differently, such differential treatment explains only a small amount of nonshared environmental influence. One suggestion is that children react differently to the same environment because of different genes. More likely influences may be the impact of peers and other experiences outside the family.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}<ref name="Plomin0103"/>
]


===Individual genes===
Evidence for the practical validity of IQ comes from examining the ] between IQ scores and life outcomes.
A very large proportion of the over 17,000 human genes are thought to have an effect on the development and functionality of the brain.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Pietropaolo |first1=S. |last2=Crusio |first2=W. E. |author-link2=Wim Crusio |doi=10.1002/wcs.135 |pmid=26302082 |title=Genes and cognition |journal=] |volume=2 |issue=3 |pages=345–352 |year=2010}}</ref> While a number of individual genes have been reported to be associated with IQ, none have a strong effect. Deary and colleagues (2009) reported that no finding of a strong single gene effect on IQ has been replicated.{{sfn|Deary|Johnson|Houlihan|2009}} Recent findings of gene associations with normally varying intellectual differences in adults and children continue to show weak effects for any one gene.<ref name="Davies2011">{{cite journal |vauthors=Davies G, Tenesa A, Payton A, Yang J, Harris SE, Liewald D, Ke X, Le Hellard S |year=2011 |title=Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic |journal=Mol Psychiatry |volume=16 |issue=10 |pages=996–1005 |doi=10.1038/mp.2011.85 |pmid=21826061 |pmc=3182557 |display-authors=etal}}</ref><ref name="Benyamin2013">{{cite journal |display-authors=8 |vauthors= Benyamin B, Pourcain B, Davis OS, Davies G, Hansell NK, Brion MJ, Kirkpatrick RM, Cents RA, Franic S, Miller MB, Haworth CM, Meaburn E, Price TS, Evans DM, Timpson N, Kemp J, Ring S, McArdle W, Medland SE, Yang J, Harris SE, Liewald DC, Scheet P, Xiao X, Hudziak JJ, de Geus C, ((Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2)), Jaddoe VW, Starr JM, Verhulst FC, Pennell C, Tiemeier H, Iacono WG, Palmer LJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Boomsma DI, Posthuma D, McGue M, Wright MJ, Smith GD, Deary IJ, Plomin R, Visscher PM |year=2013 |title= Childhood intelligence is heritable, highly polygenic and associated with FNBP1L |journal=Mol Psychiatry |volume=19| issue=2| pages=253–258| doi=10.1038/mp.2012.184 |pmid=23358156 |pmc=3935975}}</ref>
{| border="2" cellpadding="4" cellspacing="0" style="margin: 1em 1em 1em 0; border: 1px #aaa solid; border-collapse: collapse; font-size: 95%;"
|- bgcolor=#ccccff
|+ '''Economic and social correlates of IQ'''
! Factors || Correlation
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| School grades and IQ || 0.5
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| Total years of education and IQ || 0.55
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| IQ and parental socioeconomic status || 0.33
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| Job performance and IQ || 0.54
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| Negative social outcomes and IQ || &minus;0.2
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| IQs of identical twins || 0.86
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| IQs of husband and wife || 0.4
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| ''Heights'' of parent and child || 0.47
|}
{| border="2" cellpadding="4" cellspacing="0" style="margin: 1em 1em 1em 0; border: 1px #aaa solid; border-collapse: collapse; font-size: 95%;"
|- bgcolor=#ccccff
|+ '''Economic and social correlates of IQ'''
! IQ || <75 || 75&ndash;90 || 90&ndash;110 || 110&ndash;125 || >125
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| US population distribution || 5 || 20 || 50 || 20 || 5
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| Married by age 30 || 72 || 81 || 81 || 72 || 67
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| Out of labor force more than 1 month out of year (men) || 22 || 19 || 15 || 14 || 10
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| Unemployed more than 1 month out of year (men) || 12 || 10 || 7 || 7 || 2
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| Divorced in 5 years || 21 || 22 || 23 || 15 || 9
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| % of children w/ IQ in bottom decile (mothers) || 39 || 17 || 6 || 7 || < 1
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| Had an ] baby (mothers) || 32 || 17 || 8 || 4 || 2
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| Lives in poverty || 30 || 16 || 6 || 3 || 2
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| Ever incarcerated (men) || 7 || 7 || 3 || 1 || < 1
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| Chronic welfare recipient (mothers) || 31 || 17 || 8 || 2 || < 1
|- bgcolor=E9E8FF
| High school dropout || 55 || 35 || 6 || 0.4 || < 0.4
|- bgcolor=#DFE0FF
| colspan="6" | Values are the percentage of each IQ sub-population fitting each descriptor. Compiled by Gottfredson (1997) from Herrnstein & Murray (1994) pp. 171, 158, 163, 174, 230, 180, 132, 194, 247&ndash;248, 194, 146 respectively.
|}


A 2017 ] conducted on approximately 78,000 subjects identified 52 genes associated with intelligence.<ref>Sniekers S, Stringer S, Watanabe K, Jansen PR, Coleman JRI, Krapohl E, Taskesen E, Hammerschlag AR, Okbay A, Zabaneh D, Amin N, Breen G, Cesarini D, Chabris CF, Iacono WG, Ikram MA, Johannesson M, Koellinger P, Lee JJ, Magnusson PKE, McGue M, Miller MB, Ollier WER, Payton A, Pendleton N, Plomin R, Rietveld CA, Tiemeier H, van Duijn CM, Posthuma D. Genome-wide association meta-analysis of 78,308 individuals identifies new loci and genes influencing human intelligence. Nat Genet. 2017 Jul;49(7):1107-1112. doi: 10.1038/ng.3869. Epub 2017 May 22. Erratum in: Nat Genet. 2017 Sep 27;49(10 ):1558. PMID 28530673; PMCID: PMC5665562.</ref> ] is reported to be the single gene most associated with both adult and child intelligence.<ref>Benyamin B, Pourcain B, Davis OS, Davies G, Hansell NK, Brion MJ, Kirkpatrick RM, Cents RA, Franić S, Miller MB, Haworth CM, Meaburn E, Price TS, Evans DM, Timpson N, Kemp J, Ring S, McArdle W, Medland SE, Yang J, Harris SE, Liewald DC, Scheet P, Xiao X, Hudziak JJ, de Geus EJ; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2 (WTCCC2); Jaddoe VW, Starr JM, Verhulst FC, Pennell C, Tiemeier H, Iacono WG, Palmer LJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Boomsma DI, Posthuma D, McGue M, Wright MJ, Davey Smith G, Deary IJ, Plomin R, Visscher PM. Childhood intelligence is heritable, highly polygenic and associated with FNBP1L. Mol Psychiatry. 2014 Feb;19(2):253-8. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.184. Epub 2013 Jan 29. PMID 23358156; PMCID: PMC3935975.</ref>
Research shows that intelligence plays an important role in many valued life outcomes. In addition to academic success, intelligence correlates with job performance (see below), socioeconomic advancement (e.g., level of education, occupation, and income), and "social pathology" (e.g., adult criminality, poverty, unemployment, dependence on welfare, children outside of marriage). Recent work has demonstrated links between intelligence and health, longevity, and functional literacy. Correlations between '']'' and life outcomes are pervasive, though IQ and ] do not correlate. IQ and ''g'' correlate highly with school performance and job performance, less so with occupational prestige, moderately with income, and to a small degree with law-abidingness.


===Gene-environment interaction===
General intelligence (in the literature typically called "cognitive ability") is the best predictor of job performance by the standard measure, validity. ''Validity'' is the correlation between score (in this case cognitive ability, as measured, typically, by a paper-and-pencil test) and outcome (in this case job performance, as measured by a range of factors including supervisor ratings, promotions, training success, and tenure), and ranges between &minus;1.0 (the score is perfectly wrong in predicting outcome) and 1.0 (the score perfectly predicts the outcome). See ]. The validity of cognitive ability for job performance tends to increase with job complexity and varies across different studies, ranging from 0.2 for unskilled jobs to 0.8 for the most complex jobs.
] reported an interaction of genetic effects with ], such that the heritability was high in high-SES families, but much lower in low-SES families.<ref name="Rowe">{{cite journal |last1=Rowe |first1=D. C. |last2=Jacobson |first2=K. C. |year=1999 |title=Genetic and environmental influences on vocabulary IQ: parental education level as moderator |journal=Child Development |volume=70 |issue=5 |pages=1151–62 |doi=10.1111/1467-8624.00084|pmid=10546338 |s2cid=10959764}}</ref> In the US, this has been replicated in infants,<ref name="Tucker-Drob2011">{{cite journal |last1=Tucker-Drob |first1=E. M. |last2=Rhemtulla |first2=M. |last3= Harden |first3=K. P. |last4=Turkheimer |first4=E. |last5=Fask |first5=D. |year=2011 |title=Emergence of a Gene x Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Infant Mental Ability Between 10 Months and 2 Years |journal=Psychological Science |volume=22 |issue=1| pages=125–33 |doi=10.1177/0956797610392926 |pmid=21169524 |pmc=3532898}}</ref> children,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Turkheimer |first1=E. |last2=Haley |first2=A. |last3=Waldron |first3=M. |last4=D'Onofrio |first4=B. |last5=Gottesman |first5=I. I. |year=2003 |title=Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children |journal=Psychological Science |volume=14 |issue=6 |pages=623–628 |doi=10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x |pmid=14629696 |s2cid=11265284}}</ref> adolescents,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Harden |first1=K. P. |last2=Turkheimer |first2=E. |last3=Loehlin |first3=J. C. |year=2005 |title=Genotype environment interaction in adolescents' cognitive ability |doi=10.1007/s10519-005-7287-9 |journal=Behavior Genetics |volume=35 |issue=6| page=804 |s2cid=189842802}}</ref> and adults.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bates |first1=Timothy C. |last2=Lewis |first2=Gary J. |last3=Weiss |first3=Alexander |date=3 September 2013 |title=Childhood Socioeconomic Status Amplifies Genetic Effects on Adult Intelligence |journal=Psychological Science |volume=24 |issue=10 |pages=2111–2116 |doi=10.1177/0956797613488394 |pmid=24002887 |s2cid=1873699 |url=https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/10602446/GxSES_Bates_Lewis_Weiss_2013_Psychological_Science.pdf|hdl=20.500.11820/52797d10-f0d4-49de-83e2-a9cc3493703d |hdl-access=free }}</ref> Outside the US, studies show no link between heritability and SES.<ref name=":0">{{cite journal |last1=Tucker-Drob |first1=Elliot M. |last2=Bates |first2=Timothy C. |title=Large Cross-National Differences in Gene × Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Intelligence |journal=Psychological Science |date=15 December 2015 |volume=27 |issue=2 |pages=138–149 |doi=10.1177/0956797615612727 |pmid=26671911 |pmc=4749462}}</ref> Some effects may even reverse sign outside the US.<ref name=":0" /><ref name="Hanscombe2012">{{cite journal |last1=Hanscombe |first1=K. B. |last2=Trzaskowski |first2=M. |last3=Haworth |first3=C. M. |last4=Davis |first4=O. S. |last5=Dale |first5=P. S. |last6=Plomin |first6=R. |year=2012 |title=Socioeconomic Status (SES) and Children's Intelligence (IQ): In a UK-Representative Sample SES Moderates the Environmental, Not Genetic, Effect on IQ. |journal=PLOS ONE |volume=7 |issue=2 |page=e30320 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0030320 |pmid=22312423 |pmc=3270016 |bibcode=2012PLoSO...730320H|doi-access=free }}</ref>


Dickens and Flynn (2001) have argued that genes for high IQ initiate an environment-shaping ], with genetic effects causing bright children to seek out more stimulating environments that then further increase their IQ. In Dickens' model, environment effects are modeled as decaying over time. In this model, the Flynn effect can be explained by an increase in environmental stimulation independent of it being sought out by individuals. The authors suggest that programs aiming to increase IQ would be most likely to produce long-term IQ gains if they enduringly raised children's drive to seek out cognitively demanding experiences.<ref name="DickensFlynn2001">{{cite journal |last1=Dickens |first1=William T. |last2=Flynn |first2=James R. |title=Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. |journal=Psychological Review |volume=108 |issue=2 |pages=346–69 |year=2001 |pmid=11381833 |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346 |url=http://www.apa.org/journals/features/rev1082346.pdf |citeseerx=10.1.1.139.2436}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|first1=William T. |last1=Dickens |first2=James R. |last2=Flynn |title=The IQ Paradox: Still Resolved |journal=Psychological Review |volume=109 |issue=4 |year=2002 |doi=10.1037/0033-295x.109.4.764 |pages=764–771 |url=http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20020205.pdf |url-status=dead |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20070319031706/http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20020205.pdf |archive-date=19 March 2007}}</ref>
A large ] (Hunter and Hunter, 1984) which pooled validity results across many studies encompassing thousands of workers (32,124 for cognitive ability), reports that the validity of cognitive ability for entry-level jobs is 0.54, larger than any other measure including job tryout (0.44), experience (0.18), interview (0.14), age (&minus;0.01), education (0.10), and biographical inventory (0.37).


==Interventions==
Because higher test validity allows more accurate prediction of job performance, companies have a strong incentive to use cognitive ability tests to select and promote employees. IQ thus has high practical validity in economic terms. The ] of using one measure over another is proportional to the difference in their validities, all else equal. This is one economic reason why companies use job interviews (validity 0.14) rather than randomly selecting employees (validity 0.0).
In general, educational interventions, as those described below, have shown short-term effects on IQ, but long-term follow-up is often missing. For example, in the US, very large intervention programs such as the ] have not produced lasting gains in IQ scores. Even when students improve their scores on standardized tests, they do not always improve their cognitive abilities, such as memory, attention and speed.<ref>{{Cite news |last=Bidwell|first=Allie|date=13 December 2013|title=Study: High Standardized Test Scores Don't Translate to Better Cognition|work=U.S. News & World Report |url=https://www.usnews.com/news/articles/2013/12/13/study-high-standardized-test-scores-dont-translate-to-better-cognition |url-status=dead |access-date=1 July 2017 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131214082855/https://www.usnews.com/news/articles/2013/12/13/study%2Dhigh%2Dstandardized%2Dtest%2Dscores%2Ddont%2Dtranslate%2Dto%2Dbetter%2Dcognition |archive-date=14 December 2013}}</ref> More intensive, but much smaller projects, such as the ], have reported lasting effects, often on socioeconomic status variables, rather than IQ.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}


Recent studies have shown that training in using one's ] may increase IQ. A study on young adults published in April 2008 by a team from the Universities of Michigan and Bern supports the possibility of the transfer of fluid intelligence from specifically designed ].<ref name="ReferenceA">{{cite journal |last1=Jaeggi |first1=S. M. |last2=Buschkuehl |first2=M. |last3=Jonides |first3=J. |last4=Perrig |first4=W. J. |title=From the Cover: Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |volume=105 |issue=19 |pages=6829–33 |year=2008 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0801268105 |pmc=2383929 |pmid=18443283|bibcode=2008PNAS..105.6829J|doi-access=free }}</ref> Further research will be needed to determine nature, extent and duration of the proposed transfer. Among other questions, it remains to be seen whether the results extend to other kinds of fluid intelligence tests than the matrix test used in the study, and if so, whether, after training, fluid intelligence measures retain their correlation with educational and occupational achievement or if the value of fluid intelligence for predicting performance on other tasks changes. It is also unclear whether the training is durable for extended periods of time.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Sternberg |first1=R. J. |title=Increasing fluid intelligence is possible after all |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |volume=105 |issue=19 |pages=6791–2 |year=2008 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0803396105 |pmc=2383939 |pmid=18474863|bibcode=2008PNAS..105.6791S|doi-access=free }}</ref>
However, legal barriers, most prominently the 1971 United States Supreme Court decision '']'', have prevented American employers from directly using cognitive ability tests to select employees, despite the tests' high validity. This is largely based on that cognitive ability scores in selection adversely affects some minority groups, due to that different groups have different mean scores on tests of cognitive ability. However, cognitive ability tests are still used in some organizations. The U.S. military uses the ] (AFQT), as higher scores correlate with significant increases in effectiveness of both individual soldiers and units, and ] is known for using non-illegal tests that correlate with IQ tests as part of the interview process, weighing the results even more than experience in many cases.


==Music==
Some researchers have echoed the popular claim that "in economic terms it appears that the IQ score measures something with decreasing marginal value. It is important to have enough of it, but having lots and lots does not buy you that much." (Detterman and Daniel, 1989)
{{further|Mozart effect}}
Musical training in childhood correlates with higher than average IQ.<ref name="glenn">{{cite journal |last1=Glenn Schellenberg |first1=E. |title=Music Lessons Enhance IQ |journal=Psychological Science |volume=15 |issue=8 |pages=511–514 |year=2004 |pmid=15270994 |doi=10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00711.x|doi-access=free |citeseerx=10.1.1.152.4349}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Glenn Schellenberg |first1=E. |title=Long-term positive associations between music lessons and IQ |journal=Journal of Educational Psychology |volume=98 |issue=2 |pages=457–468 |year=2006 |doi=10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.457 |citeseerx= 10.1.1.397.5160}}</ref> However, a study of 10,500 twins found no effects on IQ, suggesting that the correlation was caused by genetic confounders.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Mosing |first1=Miriam A. |last2=Madison |first2=Guy |last3=Pedersen |first3=Nancy L. |last4=Ullén |first4=Fredrik |title=Investigating cognitive transfer within the framework of music practice: genetic pleiotropy rather than causality |journal=Developmental Science |volume=19 |issue=3 |pages=504–512 |date=1 May 2015 |doi=10.1111/desc.12306 |pmid=25939545}}</ref> A meta-analysis concluded that "Music training does not reliably enhance children and young adolescents' cognitive or academic skills, and that previous positive findings were probably due to confounding variables."<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Sala|first1=Giovanni |last2= Gobet|first2=Fernand|date=1 February 2017|title=When the music's over. Does music skill transfer to children's and young adolescents' cognitive and academic skills? A meta-analysis|journal=Educational Research Review |volume=20|pages=55–67|doi=10.1016/j.edurev.2016.11.005|issn=1747-938X|doi-access=free}}</ref>


It is popularly thought that listening to classical music raises IQ. However, multiple attempted replications (e.g.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Stough |first1=C. |last2=Kerkin |first2=B. |last3=Bates |first3=T. C. |last4=Mangan |first4=G. |year=1994 |title=Music and spatial IQ |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=17 |issue=5 |page=695 |doi=10.1016/0191-8869(94)90145-7 |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222240707 |access-date=3 October 2020}}</ref>) have shown that this is at best a short-term effect (lasting no longer than 10 to 15 minutes), and is not related to IQ-increase.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Chabris |first1=C. F. |year=1999 |title=Prelude or requiem for the 'Mozart effect'? |doi=10.1038/23608 |journal=Nature |volume=400 |issue=6747 |pages=826–827 |pmid=10476958 |bibcode=1999Natur.400..826C |s2cid=898161}}</ref>
However, some studies suggest IQ continues to confer large benefits even at very high levels. Ability and performance for jobs are linearly related, such that at all IQ levels, an increase in IQ translates into a concomitant increase in performance (Coward and Sackett, 1990). In an analysis of hundreds of siblings, it was found that IQ has a substantial effect on income independently of family background (Murray, 1998).


==Brain anatomy==
Other studies question the real-world importance of whatever is measured with IQ tests, especially for differences in accumulated ] and general ] in a nation. IQ correlates highly with school performance but the correlations decrease the closer one gets to real-world outcomes, like with job performance, and still lower with income. It explains less than one sixth of the income variance . Even for school grades, other factors explain most the variance. Regarding economic inequality, one study found that if we could magically give everyone identical IQs, we would still see 90 to 95 percent of the inequality we see today. . Another recent study (2002) found that wealth, race, and schooling are important to the inheritance of economic status, but IQ is not a major contributor and the genetic transmission of IQ is even less important . Some argue that IQ scores are used as an excuse for not trying to reduce poverty or otherwise improve living standards for all. Claimed low intelligence has historically been used to justify the ] and unequal treatment of women (but note that many studies find identical average IQs among men and women; see ]). In contrast, others claim that the refusal of high-IQ elites to take IQ seriously as a cause of inequality is itself immoral.
{{Main|Neuroscience and intelligence}}


Several neurophysiological factors have been correlated with intelligence in humans, including the ratio of brain weight to body weight and the size, shape, and activity level of different parts of the brain. Specific features that may affect IQ include the size and shape of the frontal lobes, the amount of blood and chemical activity in the frontal lobes, the total amount of gray matter in the brain, the overall thickness of the cortex, and the glucose metabolic rate.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Deary | first1 = I.J. | last2 = Penke | first2 = L. | last3 = Johnson | first3 = W. | year = 2010 | title = The neuroscience of human intelligence differences | url = http://www.larspenke.eu/pdfs/Deary_Penke_Johnson_2010_-_Neuroscience_of_intelligence_review.pdf | journal = Nature Reviews Neuroscience | volume = 11 | issue = 3| pages = 201–211 | doi=10.1038/nrn2793 | pmid=20145623| s2cid = 5136934| hdl = 20.500.11820/9b11fac3-47d0-424c-9d1c-fe6f9ff2ecac | hdl-access = free }}</ref>
== Public policy ==
{{main|Intelligence and public policy}}


==Health==
Because public policy is often intended to influence the same outcomes (for example to improve education, fight poverty and crime, promote fairness in employment, and counter racial discrimination), policy decisions frequently interact with intelligence measures. In some cases, modern public policy references intelligence measures or even aims to alter cognitive development directly.
{{Main|Impact of health on intelligence|Cognitive epidemiology}}
Health is important in understanding differences in IQ test scores and other measures of cognitive ability. Several factors can lead to significant cognitive impairment, particularly if they occur during pregnancy and childhood when the brain is growing and the blood–brain barrier is less effective. Such impairment may sometimes be permanent, or sometimes be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Buss |first1=Claudia |last2=Entringer |first2=Sonja |last3=Swanson |first3=James M. |last4=D. Wadhwa |first4=Pathik |date=2012 |title=The Role of Stress in Brain Development: The Gestational Environment's Long-Term Effects on the Brain |journal=Cerebrum |volume=4 |page=4 |pmid=23447790 |pmc=3574809 }}</ref>


Since about 2010, researchers such as Eppig, Hassel, and MacKenzie have found a very close and consistent link between IQ scores and infectious diseases, especially in the infant and preschool populations and the mothers of these children.<ref name=":7">Eppig. Christopher. Scientific American."Why is average IQ higher in some places?" 2011.</ref> They have postulated that fighting infectious diseases strains the child's metabolism and prevents full brain development. Hassel postulated that it is by far the most important factor in determining population IQ. However, they also found that subsequent factors such as good nutrition and regular quality schooling can offset early negative effects to some extent.
While broad consensus exists that intelligence measures neither dictate nor preclude any particular social policy, controversy surrounds many other aspects of this interaction. Central issues concern whether intelligence measures should be considered in policy decisions, the role of policy in influencing or accounting for group differences in measured intelligence, and the success of policies in light of individual and group intelligence differences. The importance and sensitivity of the policies at issue have produced an often-emotional ongoing debate spanning scholarly inquiry and the popular media from the national to the local level.


Developed nations have implemented several health policies regarding nutrients and toxins known to influence cognitive function. These include laws requiring fortification of certain food products<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Olson |first1=Rebecca |last2=Gavin-Smith |first2=Breda |last3=Ferraboschi |first3=Chiara |last4=Kraemer |first4=Klaus |date=April 2021 |title=Food Fortification: The Advantages, Disadvantages and Lessons from Sight and Life Programs |journal=Nutrients |volume=13 |issue=4 |pages=1118 |doi=10.3390/nu13041118 |pmid=33805305 |pmc=8066912 |doi-access=free }}</ref> and laws establishing safe levels of pollutants (e.g. ], ], and organochlorides). Improvements in nutrition, and in public policy in general, have been implicated in IQ increases.<ref>{{Cite news |last=Nisen |first=Max |date=July 22, 2013 |title=How Adding Iodine To Salt Resulted In A Decade's Worth Of IQ Gains For The United States |work=Business Insider |url=https://www.businessinsider.com/iodization-effect-on-iq-2013-7#ixzz2ZuFjmyY7 |access-date=December 16, 2023}}</ref>
===Use of IQ in the United States legal system===


] is a field of research that examines the associations between intelligence test scores and health. Researchers in the field argue that intelligence measured at an early age is an important predictor of later health and mortality differences.{{sfn|Deary|Batty|2007}}
The Supreme Court of the United States has also validated the use of IQ results during the sentencing phase of some criminal proceedings. The Supreme Court case of ''Atkins v. Virginia'', decided ] ], held that executions of mentally retarded criminals are "cruel and unusual punishments" prohibited by the Eighth Amendment. In ''Atkins'' the court stated that


==Social correlations==
:"&hellip;t appears that even among those States that regularly execute offenders and that have no prohibition with regard to the mentally retarded, only five have executed offenders possessing a known IQ less than 70 since we decided ''Penry''. The practice, therefore, has become truly unusual, and it is fair to say that a national consensus has developed against it."


===School performance===
In overturning the Virginia Supreme Court's holding, the ''Atkins'' opinion stated that petitioner's IQ result of 59 was a factor making the imposition of capital punishment a violation of his eighth amendment rights. In the opinion's notes the court provided some of the facts relied upon when reaching their decision
The ]'s report ''Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns'' states that wherever it has been studied, children with high scores on tests of intelligence tend to learn more of what is taught in school than their lower-scoring peers. The correlation between IQ scores and grades is about .50. This means that the explained variance is 25%. Achieving good grades depends on many factors other than IQ, such as "persistence, interest in school, and willingness to study" (p.&nbsp;81).{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}


It has been found that the correlation of IQ scores with school performance depends on the IQ measurement used. For undergraduate students, the Verbal IQ as measured by WAIS-R has been found to correlate significantly (0.53) with the grade point average (GPA) of the last 60 hours (credits). In contrast, Performance IQ correlation with the same GPA was only 0.22 in the same study.<ref>{{cite book |last=Kamphaus |first=Randy W. |title=Clinical assessment of child and adolescent intelligence |publisher=Springer |year=2005 | isbn=978-0-387-26299-4}}</ref>
:"At the sentencing phase, Dr. Nelson testified: "Atkins' full scale IQ is 59. Compared to the population at large, that means less than one percentile&hellip;. Mental retardation is a relatively rare thing. It's about one percent of the population." App. 274. According to Dr. Nelson, Atkins' IQ score "would automatically qualify for Social Security disability income." Id., at 280. Dr. Nelson also indicated that of the over 40 capital defendants that he had evaluated, Atkins was only the second individual who met the criteria for mental retardation. Id., at 310. He testified that, in his opinion, Atkins' limited intellect had been a consistent feature throughout his life, and that his IQ score of 59 is not an "aberration, malingered result, or invalid test score." Id., at 308."


Some measures of educational aptitude correlate highly with IQ tests{{snd}}for instance, {{harvp|Frey|Detterman|2004}} reported a correlation of 0.82 between ''g'' (]) and ] scores;{{sfn|Frey|Detterman|2004}} another research found a correlation of 0.81 between ''g'' and ] scores, with the explained variance ranging "from 58.6% in Mathematics and 48% in English to 18.1% in Art and Design".{{sfn|Deary|Strand|Smith|Fernandes|2007}}
==Validity and ''g''-loading of specific tests==


===Job performance===
While IQ is sometimes treated as an end unto itself, scholarly work on IQ focuses to a large extent on IQ's ], that is, the degree to which IQ predicts outcomes such as job performance, social pathologies, or academic achievement. Different IQ tests differ in their validity for various outcomes.
According to Schmidt and Hunter, "for hiring employees without previous experience in the job the most valid predictor of future performance is general mental ability."<ref name="Schmidt98">{{cite journal |last1=Schmidt |first1=Frank L. |last2=Hunter |first2=John E. |title=The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=124 |issue=2 |pages=262–74 |year=1998 |citeseerx=10.1.1.172.1733 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262 |s2cid=16429503 |url=http://www.moityca.com.br/pdfs/SchmidteHunter1998.pdf |access-date=25 October 2017 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20140602034440/http://www.moityca.com.br/pdfs/SchmidteHunter1998.pdf |archive-date=2 June 2014 |url-status=dead}}</ref> The validity of IQ as a predictor of ] is above zero for all work studied to date, but varies with the type of job and across different studies, ranging from 0.2 to 0.6.<ref name="Hunter84">{{cite journal |last1=Hunter |first1=John E. |last2=Hunter |first2=Ronda F. |title=Validity and utility of alternative predictors of job performance |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=96 |issue=1 |pages=72–98 |year=1984 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.96.1.72 |s2cid=26858912}}</ref> The correlations were higher when the unreliability of measurement methods was controlled for.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}} While IQ is more strongly correlated with reasoning and less so with motor function,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Warner |first1=Molly |last2=Ernst |first2=John |last3=Townes |first3=Brenda |last4=Peel |first4=John |last5=Preston |first5=Michael |title=Relationships Between IQ and Neuropsychological Measures in Neuropsychiatric Populations: Within-Laboratory and Cross-Cultural Replications Using WAIS and WAIS-R |journal=Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology |volume=9 |issue=5 |pages=545–62 |year=1987 |pmid=3667899 |doi=10.1080/01688638708410768}}</ref> IQ-test scores predict performance ratings in all occupations.<ref name="Schmidt98"/>


That said, for highly qualified activities (research, management) low IQ scores are more likely to be a barrier to adequate performance, whereas for minimally-skilled activities, athletic strength (manual strength, speed, stamina, and coordination) is more likely to influence performance.<ref name="Schmidt98"/> The prevailing view among academics is that it is largely through the quicker acquisition of job-relevant knowledge that higher IQ mediates job performance. This view has been challenged by Byington & Felps (2010), who argued that "the current applications of IQ-reflective tests allow individuals with high IQ scores to receive greater access to developmental resources, enabling them to acquire additional capabilities over time, and ultimately perform their jobs better."<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Byington |first1=Eliza |last2=Felps |first2=Will |title=Why do IQ scores predict job performance? |journal=Research in Organizational Behavior |volume=30 |pages=175–202 |doi=10.1016/j.riob.2010.08.003 |year=2010}}</ref>
Tests also differ in their ]-loading, which is the degree to which the test score reflects general mental ability rather than a specific skill or "group factor" such as verbal ability, spatial visualization, or mathematical reasoning). ''g''-loading and validity are related in the sense that most IQ tests derive their validity mostly or entirely from the degree to which they measure ''g'' (Jensen 1998).


Newer studies find that the effects of IQ on job performance have been greatly overestimated. The current estimates of the correlation between job performance and IQ are about 0.23 correcting for unreliability and range restriction.<ref>{{cite journal |url=https://ink.library.smu.edu.sg/lkcsb_research/7188/ |title=Revisiting the design of selection systems in light of new findings regarding the validity of widely used predictors |last1=Sackett |first1=Paul R. |last2=Zhang |first2=Charlene |last3=Berry |first3=Christopher M. |last4=Lievens |first4=Filip |year=2023 |journal=Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice |volume=16 |issue=3 |access-date=24 April 2023}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|doi=10.1037/apl0000994 |title=Revisiting meta-analytic estimates of validity in personnel selection: Addressing systematic overcorrection for restriction of range |year=2022 |last1=Sackett |first1=Paul R. |last2=Zhang |first2=Charlene |last3=Berry |first3=Christopher M. |last4=Lievens |first4=Filip |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=107 |issue=11 |pages=2040–2068 |pmid=34968080 |s2cid=245594032 |url=https://ink.library.smu.edu.sg/lkcsb_research/6894 }}</ref>
==Social construct==


In establishing a causal direction to the link between IQ and work performance, longitudinal studies by Watkins and others suggest that IQ exerts a causal influence on future academic achievement, whereas academic achievement does not substantially influence future IQ scores.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Watkins |first1=M |last2=Lei |first2=P |last3=Canivez |first3=G |title=Psychometric intelligence and achievement: A cross-lagged panel analysis |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=1 |pages=59–68 |year=2007 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.04.005|citeseerx=10.1.1.397.3155}}</ref> Treena Eileen Rohde and Lee Anne Thompson write that general cognitive ability, but not specific ability scores, predict academic achievement, with the exception that processing speed and spatial ability predict performance on the SAT math beyond the effect of general cognitive ability.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Rohde |first1=T |last2=Thompson |first2=L |title=Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=1 |pages=83–92 |year=2007 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.05.004}}</ref>
Some maintain that IQ is a ] invented by the privileged classes, used to maintain their privilege. Others maintain that intelligence, measured by IQ or ''g'', reflects a real ability, is a useful tool in performing life tasks and has a biological reality.


However, large-scale longitudinal studies indicate an increase in IQ translates into an increase in performance at all levels of IQ: i.e. ability and job performance are ] linked at all IQ levels.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Coward |first1=W. Mark |last2=Sackett |first2=Paul R. |title=Linearity of ability-performance relationships: A reconfirmation |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=75 |issue=3 |pages=297–300 |year=1990 |doi=10.1037/0021-9010.75.3.297}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Robertson |first1=Kimberley Ferriman |last2=Smeets |first2=Stijn |last3=Lubinski |first3=David |last4=Benbow |first4=Camilla P. |title=Beyond the Threshold Hypothesis |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |date=14 December 2010 |volume=19 |issue=6 |pages=346–351 |doi=10.1177/0963721410391442 |s2cid=46218795 |url=https://my.vanderbilt.edu/smpy/files/2013/02/Ferriman_20101.pdf}}</ref>
The social-construct and real-ability interpretations for IQ differences can be distinguished because they make opposite predictions about what would happen if people were given equal opportunities. The social explanation predicts that equal treatment will eliminate differences, while the real-ability explanation predicts that equal treatment will accentuate differences. Evidence for both outcomes exists. Achievement gaps persist in socioeconomically advantaged, integrated, liberal, suburban school districts in the United States (see Noguera, 2001). Test-score gaps tend to be larger at higher socioeconomic levels (Gottfredson, 2003). Some studies have reported a narrowing of score gaps over time.


===Income===
The reduction of intelligence to a single score seems extreme and wrong to many people. Opponents argue that it is much more useful to know a person's strengths and weaknesses than to know their IQ score. Such opponents often cite the example of two people with the same overall IQ score but very different ability profiles. As measured by IQ tests, most people have highly balanced ability profiles, with differences in subscores being greater among the more intelligent. However, this assumes the ability of IQ tests to comprehensively gauge the wide variety of human intellectual abilities.
<!-- This whole section needs to be checked against the sources for missing information and undue weight -->
<!--
{| class="wikitable"
|-
|+ '''Relation between IQ and earnings in the U.S.'''
! IQ || <75 || 75–90 || 90–110 || 110–125 || >125
|-
| Age 18 || 2,000 || 5,000 || 8,000 || 8,000 || 21,000
|-
| Age 26 || 3,000 || 10,000 || 16,000 || 20,000 || 42,000
|-
| Age 32 || 5,000 || 12,400 || 20,000 || 27,000 || 48,000
|-
| colspan="6" | Values are the average earnings (1993 US Dollars) of each IQ sub-population.<ref> {{cite journal | last1 = Murray | first1 = C | year = 1997 | title = IQ and economic success | journal = Public Interest | volume = 128 | pages = 21–35}}</ref>
|}
-->


It has been suggested that "in economic terms it appears that the IQ score measures something with decreasing marginal value" and it "is important to have enough of it, but having lots and lots does not buy you that much".{{sfn |Detterman|Daniel|1989}}<ref>
The creators of IQ testing did not intend for the tests to gauge a person's worth, and in many (or, as some people suggest, all) situations, IQ may have little relevance.
{{cite web
|url=http://www.americanscientist.org/template/AssetDetail/assetid/24538/page/4
===The Mismeasure of Man===
|title=The Role of Intelligence in Modern Society (July–Aug, 1995)
|pages=4 (Nonlinearities in Intelligence)
|date=July 1995
|first=Earl B. |last=Hunt
|publisher=American Scientist
|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20060521185816/http://www.americanscientist.org/template/AssetDetail/assetid/24538/page/4 |archive-date=21 May 2006}}</ref>


The link from IQ to wealth is much less strong than that from IQ to job performance. Some studies indicate that IQ is unrelated to net worth.<ref>{{Cite news |work=The Times |location=London |title=Brains don't make you rich IQ study finds |first=Mark |last=Henderson |date=25 April 2007 |url=http://www.timesonline.co.uk/tol/news/uk/science/article1701377.ece |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080906215126/http://www.timesonline.co.uk/tol/news/uk/science/article1701377.ece |url-status=dead |archive-date=6 September 2008 |access-date=5 May 2010}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |title=You Don't Have To Be Smart To Be Rich, Study Finds |website=ScienceDaily |url=https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2007/04/070424204519.htm |access-date=26 August 2014}}</ref> The American Psychological Association's 1995 report '']'' stated that IQ scores accounted for about a quarter of the social status variance and one-sixth of the income variance. Statistical controls for parental SES eliminate about a quarter of this predictive power. Psychometric intelligence appears as only one of a great many factors that influence social outcomes.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}} ] (1998) showed a more substantial effect of IQ on income independent of family background.{{sfn|Murray|1998|p={{Page needed|date=January 2011}}}} In a meta-analysis, Strenze (2006) reviewed much of the literature and estimated the correlation between IQ and income to be about 0.23.<ref name="Strenze2007">{{cite journal |last1=Strenze |first1=Tarmo |date=September 2007 |title=Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=5 |pages=401–426 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.004 |quote=The correlation with income is considerably lower, perhaps even disappointingly low, being about the average of the previous meta-analytic estimates (.15 by Bowles et al., 2001; and .27 by Ng et al., 2005). But...other predictors, studied in this paper, are not doing any better in predicting income, which demonstrates that financial success is difficult to predict by any variable. This assertion is further corroborated by the meta-analysis of Ng et al. (2005) where the best predictor of salary was educational level with a correlation of only .29. It should also be noted that the correlation of .23 is about the size of the average meta-analytic result in psychology(Hemphill, 2003) and cannot, therefore, be treated as insignificant.}}</ref>
Some scientists dispute ] entirely. In '']'', Professor ] argues that intelligence tests are based on faulty assumptions and shows their history of being used as the basis for ]. He writes:


Some studies assert that IQ only accounts for (explains) a sixth of the variation in income because many studies are based on young adults, many of whom have not yet reached their peak earning capacity, or even their education. On pg 568 of '']'', ] says that although the correlation between IQ and income averages a moderate 0.4 (one-sixth or 16% of the variance), the relationship increases with age, and peaks at middle age when people have reached their maximum career potential. In the book, ''A Question of Intelligence'', ] cites an IQ income correlation of 0.5 (25% of the variance).
:&hellip;the abstraction of intelligence as a single entity, its location within the brain, its quantification as one number for each individual, and the use of these numbers to rank people in a single series of worthiness, invariably to find that oppressed and disadvantaged groups&mdash;races, classes, or sexes&mdash;are innately inferior and deserve their status. (pp. 24&ndash;25)


A 2002 study further examined the impact of non-IQ factors on income and concluded that an individual's location, inherited wealth, race, and schooling are more important as factors in determining income than IQ.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bowles |first1=Samuel |last2=Gintis |first2=Herbert |year=2002 |title=The Inheritance of Inequality |journal=Journal of Economic Perspectives |volume=16 |issue=3 |pages=3–30 |doi=10.1257/089533002760278686 |doi-access=free}}</ref>
He spends much of the book debunking the concept of IQ, including a historical discussion of how the IQ tests were created and a technical discussion of why ''g'' is simply a mathematical artifact. Later editions of the book include criticism of '']''.


===Crime===
], Professor of Educational Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, responds to Gould's criticisms in a paper titled ''The Debunking of Scientific Fossils and Straw Persons''.
The American Psychological Association's 1995 report ''Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns'' stated that the ] was −0.2. This association is generally regarded as small and prone to disappearance or a substantial reduction after controlling for the proper covariates, being much smaller than typical sociological correlates.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Cullen |first1=Francis T. |last2=Gendreau |first2=Paul |last3=Jarjoura |first3=G. Roger |last4=Wright |first4=John Paul |title=Crime and the Bell Curve: Lessons from Intelligent Criminology |journal=Crime & Delinquency |date=October 1997 |volume=43 |issue=4 |pages=387–411 |doi=10.1177/0011128797043004001 |s2cid=145418972}}</ref> It was −0.19 between IQ scores and the number of juvenile offenses in a large Danish sample; with social class controlled for, the correlation dropped to −0.17. A correlation of 0.20 means that the ] accounts for 4% of the total variance. The causal links between psychometric ability and social outcomes may be indirect. Children with poor scholastic performance may feel alienated. Consequently, they may be more likely to engage in delinquent behavior, compared to other children who do well.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}


In his book '']'' (1998), ] cited data which showed that, regardless of race, people with IQs between 70 and 90 have higher crime rates than people with IQs below or above this range, with the peak range being between 80 and 90.
===The view of the American Psychological Association===
In response to the controversy surrounding ''The Bell Curve'', the ]'s Board of Scientific Affairs established a task force to write a consensus statement on the state of intelligence research which could be used by all sides as a basis for discussion. The full text of the report is available at a third-party website.


The 2009 ''Handbook of Crime Correlates'' stated that reviews have found that around eight IQ points, or 0.5 SD, separate criminals from the general population, especially for persistent serious offenders. It has been suggested that this simply reflects that "only dumb ones get caught" but there is similarly a negative relation between IQ and self-reported offending. That children with ] have lower IQ than their peers "strongly argues" for the theory.<ref>''Handbook of Crime Correlates''; Lee Ellis, Kevin M. Beaver, John Wright; 2009; Academic Press</ref>
The findings of the task force state that IQ scores ''do'' have high predictive validity for individual (but not necessarily population) differences in school achievement. They confirm the predictive validity of IQ for adult occupational status, even when variables such as education and family background have been statistically controlled. They agree that individual (again, not necessarily population) differences in intelligence are substantially influenced by genetics.


A study of the relationship between US county-level IQ and US county-level crime rates found that higher average IQs were very weakly associated with lower levels of property crime, burglary, larceny rate, motor vehicle theft, violent crime, robbery, and aggravated assault. These results were "not confounded by a measure of concentrated disadvantage that captures the effects of race, poverty, and other social disadvantages of the county."<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Beaver |first1=Kevin M. |last2=Schwartz |first2=Joseph A. |last3=Nedelec |first3=Joseph L. |last4=Connolly |first4=Eric J. |last5=Boutwell |first5=Brian B. |last6=Barnes |first6=J.C. |title=Intelligence is associated with criminal justice processing: Arrest through incarceration |journal=Intelligence |date=September 2013 |volume=41 |issue=5 |pages=277–288 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2013.05.001}}</ref> However, this study is limited in that it extrapolated Add Health estimates to the respondent's counties, and as the dataset was not designed to be representative on the state or county level, it may not be generalizable.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Beaver |first1=Kevin M. |last2=Wright |first2=John Paul |title=The association between county-level IQ and county-level crime rates |journal=Intelligence |date=January 2011 |volume=39 |issue=1 |pages=22–26 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2010.12.002}}</ref>
They state there is little evidence to show that childhood diet influences intelligence except in cases of severe malnutrition. They agree that there are no significant differences between the average IQ scores of males and females. The task force agrees that large differences do exist between the average IQ scores of blacks and whites, and that these differences cannot be attributed to biases in test construction. While they admit there is no empirical evidence supporting it, the APA task force suggests that explanations based on social status and cultural differences may be possible. Regarding genetic causes, they noted that there is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis.


It has also been shown that the effect of IQ is heavily dependent on socioeconomic status and that it cannot be easily controlled away, with many methodological considerations being at play.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Mears |first1=Daniel P. |last2=Cochran |first2=Joshua C. |title=What is the effect of IQ on offending? |journal=Criminal Justice and Behavior |date=November 2013 |volume=40 |issue=11 |pages=1280–1300 |doi=10.1177/0093854813485736 |s2cid=147219554}}</ref> Indeed, there is evidence that the small relationship is mediated by well-being, substance abuse, and other confounding factors that prohibit simple causal interpretation.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Freeman |first1=James |title=The relationship between lower intelligence, crime and custodial outcomes: a brief literary review of a vulnerable group |journal=Vulnerable Groups & Inclusion |date=January 2012 |volume=3 |issue=1 |pages=14834 |doi=10.3402/vgi.v3i0.14834 |s2cid=145305072|doi-access=free }}</ref> A recent meta-analysis has shown that the relationship is only observed in higher risk populations such as those in poverty without direct effect, but without any causal interpretation.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Ttofi |first1=Maria M. |last2=Farrington |first2=David P. |last3=Piquero |first3=Alex R. |last4=Lösel |first4=Friedrich |last5=DeLisi |first5=Matthew |last6=Murray |first6=Joseph |title=Intelligence as a protective factor against offending: A meta-analytic review of prospective longitudinal studies |journal=Journal of Criminal Justice |date=1 June 2016 |volume=45 |pages=4–18 |doi=10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.02.003}}</ref> A nationally representative longitudinal study has shown that this relationship is entirely mediated by school performance.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=McGloin |first1=Jean Marie |last2=Pratt |first2=Travis C. |last3=Maahs |first3=Jeff |title=Rethinking the IQ-delinquency relationship: A longitudinal analysis of multiple theoretical models |journal=Justice Quarterly |date=1 September 2004 |volume=21 |issue=3 |pages=603–635 |doi=10.1080/07418820400095921 |s2cid=143305924}}</ref>
The APA journal that published the statement, '']'', subsequently published eleven critical responses in January 1997, most arguing that the report failed to examine adequately the evidence for partly-genetic explanations.


===Health and mortality===
The report was published in 1995 and thus does not include a decade of recent research.
Multiple studies conducted in Scotland have found that higher IQs in early life are associated with lower mortality and morbidity rates later in life.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Gottfredson |first1=Linda S. |last2=Deary |first2=Ian J. |date=22 June 2016 |title=Intelligence Predicts Health and Longevity, but Why? |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |language=en |volume=13 |issue=1 |pages=1–4 |doi=10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301001.x |s2cid=15176389}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Batty |first1=G. David |last2=Deary |first2=Ian J. |last3=Gottfredson |first3=Linda S. |title=Premorbid (early life) IQ and Later Mortality Risk: Systematic Review |journal=Annals of Epidemiology |volume=17 |issue=4 |pages=278–288 |doi=10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.07.010 |pmid=17174570 |year=2007|citeseerx=10.1.1.693.9671}}</ref>


===Other accomplishments===
==Improving IQ==
<!-- This whole section, repetitive of a section above, needs to be checked against reliable sources for missing information and due weight -->
While a large amount of one's IQ is predetermined by genetic factors, the environment can play a role as well. IQ can be improved to a certain extent through reading and application. Improvement in diet and regular exercise can help certain cognitive functions, and getting more sleep may help as well. Depression and stress reduce IQ somewhat, so removal of these factors might also help.
{| class=wikitable
|+ Average adult combined IQs associated with real-life accomplishments by various tests:{{sfn|Kaufman|2009|p=126}}{{sfn|Kaufman|Lichtenberger|2006}}
! Accomplishment !! IQ !! Test/study !! Year
|-
| MDs, JDs, and PhDs || 125 || ] || 1987
|-
| rowspan=3 | College graduates || rowspan=2 | 112 || ] || 2000
|-
| ] || 1992
|-
| 115 || WAIS-R ||
|-
| rowspan=3 | 1–3 years of college || rowspan=2 | 104 || KAIT ||
|-
| K-BIT ||
|-
| 105–110 || WAIS-R ||
|-
| Clerical and sales workers || 100–105 || ||
|-
| rowspan=3 | High school graduates, skilled workers (e.g., electricians, cabinetmakers) || rowspan=2 | 100 || KAIT ||
|-
| WAIS-R ||
|-
| 97 || K-BIT ||
|-
| rowspan=3 | 1–3 years of high school (completed 9–11 years of school) || 94 || KAIT ||
|-
| 90 || K-BIT ||
|-
| 95 || WAIS-R ||
|-
| Semi-skilled workers (e.g. truck drivers, factory workers) || 90–95 || ||
|-
| Elementary school graduates (completed eighth grade) || 90 || ||
|-
| Elementary school dropouts (completed 0–7 years of school) || 80–85 || ||
|-
| Have 50/50 chance of reaching high school || 75 || ||
|}


{| class=wikitable
Drugs designed to improve cognitive function, and sometimes IQ scores are called ]s.
|+ Average IQ of various occupational groups:{{sfn|Kaufman|2009|p=132}}
! Accomplishment !! IQ !! Test/study !! Year
|-
| Professional and technical || 112 || ||
|-
| Managers and administrators || 104 || ||
|-
| Clerical workers, sales workers, skilled workers, craftsmen, and foremen || 101 || ||
|-
| Semi-skilled workers (operatives, service workers, including private household) || 92 || ||
|-
| Unskilled workers || 87 || ||
|}


{| class=wikitable
Working memory training, an experimental treatment which has according to one study by Klingberg et al, improved raw scores substantially on Ravens progressive matrices and Ravens advanced progressive matrices, both IQ tests. It has also been claimed in some studies that ] can increase IQ. However, some would argue that these studies should not necessarily be interpreted as proof that neurofeedback can increase IQ as (a) they don't have a double blind component and (b) it is unknown whether their effects would apply to persons without ] (ADHD), as most of these studies were performed on persons with ADHD. It is possible that the increase in IQ was just a result of better concentration in the subjects.
|+ Type of work that can be accomplished:{{sfn|Kaufman|2009|p=126}}
! Accomplishment !! IQ !! Test/study !! Year
|-
| Adults can harvest vegetables, repair furniture || 60 || ||
|-
| Adults can do domestic work || 50|| ||
|}


There is considerable variation within and overlap among these categories. People with high IQs are found at all levels of education and occupational categories. The biggest difference occurs for low IQs with only an occasional college graduate or professional scoring below 90.<ref name=Kaufman2009/>
A recent scientific article on the concept of cognitive reserve included an argument that education and application of the mind can substantially increase IQ.


==Group differences==
The "]" is the claimed ability of certain musics to enhance intelligence, especially spatial reasoning. However, this effect is not universally accepted. Musical education, as opposed to appreciation, has been shown a number of times to marginally increase IQ in children; however, there is sparsity of information on whether such an effect might apply to adults.
Among the most controversial issues related to the study of intelligence is the observation that IQ scores vary on average between ethnic and racial groups, though these differences have fluctuated and in many cases steadily decreased over time.<ref name=":8">{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |date=2012 |title=Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments |journal=American Psychologist |volume=67 |issue=2 |pages=130–159 |doi=10.1037/a0026699 |issn=1935-990X |pmid=22233090}}</ref> While there is little scholarly debate about the continued existence of some of these differences, the current scientific consensus is that they stem from environmental rather than genetic causes.{{sfn|Ceci|Williams|2009|pages=788–789, "There is an emerging consensus about racial and gender equality in genetic determinants of intelligence; most researchers, including ourselves, agree that genes do not explain between-group differences"}}<ref name=":9">{{Cite journal |date=25 May 2017 |title=Intelligence research should not be held back by its past |journal=Nature |volume=545 |issue=7655 |pages=385–386 |doi=10.1038/nature.2017.22021 |pmid=28541341 |bibcode=2017Natur.545R.385. |s2cid=4449918 |quote=Intelligence science has undoubtedly been dogged by ugly prejudice. Historical measurements of skull volume and brain weight were done to advance claims of the racial superiority of white people. More recently, the (genuine but closing) gap between the average IQ scores of groups of black and white people in the United States has been falsely attributed to genetic differences between the races.|doi-access=free }}</ref><ref name=":03" /> The existence of differences in IQ between the sexes has been debated, and largely depends on which tests are performed.<ref name="Mackintosh2">{{harvnb|Mackintosh|2011|pp=}}</ref><ref name="Hunt 389">{{harvnb|Hunt|2011|p=}}</ref>


===Race===
The levels of a variety of chemicals in the brain, such as chlorine, have been shown to relate to intelligence in a variety of ways. It is possible that by adjusting diet, these could be substantially changed.
{{Main|Race and intelligence}}While the concept of "]" is a ],<ref name=":82">{{Cite book |url=https://nap.nationalacademies.org/read/26902/chapter/1 |title=Using Population Descriptors in Genetics and Genomics Research: A New Framework for an Evolving Field (Consensus Study Report) |date=2023 |publisher=] |isbn=978-0-309-70065-8 |doi=10.17226/26902 |pmid=36989389 |quote=In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups.}}</ref> discussions of a purported relationship between race and intelligence, as well as claims of genetic differences in intelligence along racial lines, have appeared in both ] and ] since the modern concept of race was first introduced.


Genetics do not explain differences in IQ test performance between racial or ethnic groups.<ref name=":12"/>{{sfn|Ceci|Williams|2009|pages=788–789, "There is an emerging consensus about racial and gender equality in genetic determinants of intelligence; most researchers, including ourselves, agree that genes do not explain between-group differences"}}<ref name=":9" /><ref name=":03" /> Despite the tremendous amount of research done on the topic, no scientific evidence has emerged that the average IQ scores of different population groups can be attributed to genetic differences between those groups.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Jencks |first1=Christopher |author-link=Christopher Jencks |title=The Black-White Test Score Gap |last2=Phillips |first2=Meredith |publisher=Brookings Institution Press |year=2011 |isbn=9780815746119 |pages=503 |quote=The available evidence reviewed by several authors in this volume provides, as Richard E. Nisbett puts it, 'no evidence for genetic superiority of either race while providing strong evidence for a substantial environmental contribution to the black-white IQ gap.' |orig-date=1998}}</ref><ref name=":10">{{Cite web |last1=Birney |first1=Ewan |author-link=Ewan Birney |last2=Raff |first2=Jennifer |author-link2=Jennifer Raff |last3=Rutherford |first3=Adam |author-link3=Adam Rutherford |last4=Scally |first4=Aylwyn |date=24 October 2019 |title=Race, genetics and pseudoscience: an explainer |url=http://ewanbirney.com/2019/10/race-genetics-and-pseudoscience-an-explainer.html |website=Ewan's Blog: Bioinformatician at large |quote=‘Human biodiversity’ proponents sometimes assert that alleged differences in the mean value of IQ when measured in different populations – such as the claim that IQ in some sub-Saharan African countries is measurably lower than in European countries – are caused by genetic variation, and thus are inherent. . . . Such tales, and the claims about the genetic basis for population differences, are not scientifically supported. In reality for most traits, including IQ, it is not only unclear that genetic variation explains differences between populations, it is also unlikely.}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Aaron |first1=Panofsky |author-link=Aaron Panofsky |last2=Dasgupta |first2=Kushan |date=28 September 2020 |title=How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics |journal=American Journal of Biological Anthropology |volume=175 |issue=2 |pages=387–398 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.24150 |pmc=9909835 |pmid=32986847 |s2cid=222163480 |quote=he claims that genetics defines racial groups and makes them different, that IQ and cultural differences among racial groups are caused by genes, and that racial inequalities within and between nations are the inevitable outcome of long evolutionary processes are neither new nor supported by science (either old or new).}}</ref> In recent decades, as understanding of ] has advanced, claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have been broadly rejected by scientists on both ] and ] grounds.<ref name=":322" /><ref name=":03" /><ref name=":02">{{Cite news |last=Mitchell |first=Kevin |date=2 May 2018 |title=Why genetic IQ differences between 'races' are unlikely: The idea that intelligence can differ between populations has made headlines again, but the rules of evolution make it implausible |url=https://www.theguardian.com/science/blog/2018/may/02/why-genetic-iq-differences-between-races-are-unlikely |access-date=13 June 2020 |work=The Guardian}}</ref><ref name=":2" /><ref name=":10" />
Future possibilities for improving the skills IQ tests measure include stem cells treatment, genetic modification, better education based on neurological and cognitive discoveries, better nootropics, etc.


Growing evidence indicates that environmental factors, not genetic ones, explain the racial IQ gap.<ref name=":2">{{Cite journal |last=Kaplan |first=Jonathan Michael |date=January 2015 |title=Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis" |journal=Biology & Philosophy |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=1–17 |doi=10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0 |issn=0169-3867 |s2cid=85351431}}</ref><ref name=":322">{{Cite journal|last1=Dickens|first1=William T.|last2=Flynn|first2=James R.|date=2006|title=Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples|url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/dickens2006a.pdf|journal=Psychological Science|volume=17|issue=10|pages=913–920|doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x|pmid=17100793|s2cid=6593169}}</ref><ref name=":03">{{cite journal|last1=Nisbett|first1=Richard E.|last2=Aronson|first2=Joshua|last3=Blair|first3=Clancy|last4=Dickens|first4=William|last5=Flynn|first5=James|author-link5=Jim Flynn (academic)|last6=Halpern|first6=Diane F.|author-link6=Diane F. Halpern|last7=Turkheimer|first7=Eric|date=2012|title=Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin|url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Nisbett%20(2012)%20Group.pdf|journal=American Psychologist|volume=67|pages=503–504|doi=10.1037/a0029772|issn=0003-066X|pmid=22963427|access-date=22 July 2013|number=6|author-link1=Richard E. Nisbett}}</ref> A 1996 task force investigation on intelligence sponsored by the ] concluded that "because ethnic differences in intelligence reflect complex patterns, no overall generalization about them is appropriate," with environmental factors the most plausible reason for the shrinking gap.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}} A systematic analysis by ] and ] (2006) showed the gap between black and white Americans to have closed dramatically during the period between 1972 and 2002, suggesting that, in their words, the "constancy of the Black–White IQ gap is a myth".<ref name=":32">{{Cite journal|last1=Dickens|first1=William T.|last2=Flynn|first2=James R.|date=2006|title=Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/dickens2006a.pdf|journal=Psychological Science |volume=17|issue=10|pages=913–920|doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x|pmid=17100793|s2cid=6593169}}</ref> The effects of ] have been proposed as an explanation for differences in IQ test performance between racial groups,{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=348}}<ref>{{cite book |last1=Inzlicht |first1=Michael |title=Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application |date=2011 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0199732449 |pages=5, 141–143}}</ref> as have issues related to cultural difference and access to education.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Shuttleworth-Edwards |first1=Ann B. |title=Minority and cross-cultural aspects of neuropsychological assessment |last2=Van der Merwe |first2=Adele S. |publisher=Swets & Zeitlinger |year=2002 |isbn=9026518307 |editor-last=Ferraro |editor-first=F. Richard |location=Exton, PA |pages=72–75 |chapter=WAIS-III and WISC-IV South African Cross-Cultural Normative Data Stratified for Quality of Education}}</ref><ref name=":6">Barbara P. Uzzell, Marcel Ponton, Alfredo Ardila International Handbook of Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology book {{ISBN|978-0805835854}} (2013)</ref>
==Controversy==


Despite the strong scientific consensus to the contrary, fringe figures continue to promote ] about group-level IQ averages in ] and popular culture.<ref name=":12" /><ref name=":13" /><ref name=":11" />
See article on ].


==End material== ===Sex===
{{Main|Sex differences in intelligence}}
===See also===


With the advent of the concept of ''g'' or ], many researchers have found that there are no significant sex differences in average IQ,<ref name="Hunt 389"/><ref name="Plotnik2">{{cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=6jLTCQAAQBAJ&pg=PT324|title=Introduction to Psychology |vauthors=Plotnik R, Kouyoumdjian H|publisher=]|year=2013|isbn=978-1133939535|pages=282–283}}</ref><ref name="Hunt 378-379">{{harvnb|Hunt|2011|pp=}}</ref> though ability in particular types of intelligence does vary.<ref name="Mackintosh2"/><ref name="Hunt 378-379"/> Thus, while some test batteries show slightly greater intelligence in males, others show greater intelligence in females.<ref name="Mackintosh2"/><ref name="Hunt 378-379"/> In particular, studies have shown female subjects performing better on tasks related to ],<ref name="Hunt 389"/> and males performing better on tasks related to rotation of objects in space, often categorized as ].<ref name="Terry2">{{cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=4s5WCgAAQBAJ&pg=PA356|title=Learning and Memory: Basic Principles, Processes, and Procedures, Fourth Edition|vauthors=Terry WS|publisher=] |year=2015|isbn=978-1317350873 |page=356}}</ref> These differences remain, as {{harvp|Hunt|2011}} observes, "even though men and women are essentially equal in general intelligence".
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* Societies:
** ] &mdash; 95th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 126
** ] &mdash; 97.7th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 132
** ] &mdash; 98th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 133
** ] &mdash; 99.5th percentile
** ] &mdash; 99.7th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 144
** ] &mdash; 99.87th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 148
** ] &mdash; 99.87th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 148
** ] &mdash; 99.9th percentile
** ]
** ] &mdash; 99.9th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 150
** ] &mdash; 99.997th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 164
** ] &mdash; 99.997th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 164
** ] &mdash; 99.997th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 164
** ] &mdash; 99.9999th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 176
** ] &mdash; 99.9999th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 176
** ] &mdash; 99.99997th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 180
** ] &mdash; 99.99997th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 180
** ] &mdash; 99.99997th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 180
** ] &mdash; 99.9999999th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 196
** ] &mdash; 99.9999999th percentile, or SD16 IQ of 196


Some research indicates that male advantages on some ]s are minimized when controlling for socioeconomic factors.<ref name="Mackintosh2"/><ref name="Plotnik2"/> Other research has concluded that there is slightly ] in male scores in certain areas compared to female scores, which results in slightly more males than females in the ] and ] of the IQ distribution.<ref name="Chrisler JC">{{cite book|url= https://books.google.com/books?id=Xtq0M1f_aIMC&pg=PA302 |title=Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology: Volume 1: Gender Research in General and Experimental Psychology |vauthors=Chrisler JC, McCreary DR |publisher=] |year=2010 |isbn=978-1441914651|page=302}}</ref>
===External links===
{{Cleanup-spam}}
* &mdash; Nice intelligence test (not a standard IQ test)
* &mdash; The History of IQ Testing and Research
* &mdash; Available in 14 languages, accepted for admission in dozens of high IQ societies (with articles on Psychometry, history of IQ tests etc.)
*
* ] &mdash;
*
*
*
*
* ''(note: these sorts of estimates are considered highly suspect; the psychological community generally regards it impossible to infer an "IQ" from writing samples or accomplishments)''
*
*


The existence of differences between male and female performance on math-related tests is contested,<ref name=":5">{{cite journal|last1=Hyde|first1=J. S.|last2=Linn|first2=M. C.|date=27 October 2006 |title=DIVERSITY: Enhanced: Gender Similarities in Mathematics and Science|journal=Science|volume=314 |issue=5799 |pages=599–600|doi=10.1126/science.1132154|pmid=17068246|s2cid=34045261}}</ref> and a meta-analysis focusing on average gender differences in math performance found nearly identical performance for boys and girls.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Hyde|first1=Janet S.|last2=Fennema|first2=Elizabeth|last3=Lamon|first3=Susan J.|date=1990|title=Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|volume=107|issue=2|pages=139–155|doi=10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.139|pmid=2138794}}</ref> Currently, most IQ tests, including popular batteries such as the WAIS and the WISC-R, are constructed so that there are no overall score differences between females and males.{{sfn|Neisser et al.|1995}}<ref name="apa">{{cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |title=Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. |journal=American Psychologist |date=2012 |volume=67 |issue=2 |pages=130–159 |doi=10.1037/a0026699 |pmid=22233090 |s2cid=7001642}}</ref>{{sfn|Jensen|1998|p=531}}
===References===


==Public policy==
*Carroll, J.B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytical studies. New York: Cambridge University Press.
{{Main|Intelligence and public policy}}
*Coward, W.M. and Sackett, P.R. (1990). Linearity of ability-performance relationships: A reconfirmation. ''Journal of Applied Psychology,'' 75:297&#8211;300.
*Duncan, J., P. Burgess, and H. Emslie (1995) Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 33(3): p. 261-8.
*Duncan, J., et al., A neural basis for general intelligence. Science, 2000. 289(5478): p. 457-60.
* Frey, M.C. and Detterman, D.K. (2003) Scholastic Assessment or ''g''? The Relationship Between the Scholastic Assessment Test and General Cognitive Ability. ''Psychological Science,'' 15(6):373&ndash;378.
*Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). "Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life." ''Intelligence'', 24(1), 79&ndash;132.
*Gottfredson, L.S. (1998). The general intelligence factor. ''Scientific American Presents,'' 9(4):24&ndash;29.
*Gottfredson, L. S. (2005). Suppressing intelligence research: Hurting those we intend to help. In R. H. Wright & N. A. Cummings (Eds.), Destructive trends in mental health: The well-intentioned path to harm (pp. 155&ndash;186). New York: Taylor and Francis.
* Gottfredson, L. S. (in press). "Social consequences of group differences in cognitive ability (Consequencias sociais das diferencas de grupo em habilidade cognitiva)". In C. E. Flores-Mendoza & R. Colom (Eds.), ''Introducau a psicologia das diferncas individuais''. Porto Allegre, Brazil: ArtMed Publishers.
*Gray, J.R., C.F. Chabris, and T.S. Braver, Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat Neurosci, 2003. 6(3): p. 316-22.
*Gray, J.R. and P.M. Thompson, Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics. Nat Rev Neurosci, 2004. 5(6): p. 471-82.
*{{cite journal | author=Haier RJ, Jung RE, Yeo RA, et al. | title=The neuroanatomy of general intelligence: sex matters | journal=NeuroImage | year=2005 | volume=25 | pages=320–327}}
*Hunt, E. (2001). Multiple views of multiple intelligence.
*Jensen, A.R. (1998). ''The ''g'' Factor.'' Praeger, Connecticut, USA.
*Jensen, A.R. (2006). "Clocking the Mind: Mental Chronometry and Individual Differences." Elsevier Science. --->Highly anticipated new release scheduled for early June, 2006.
*McClearn, G. E., Johansson, B., Berg, S., Pedersen, N. L., Ahern, F., Petrill, S. A., & Plomin, R. (1997). Substantial genetic influence on cognitive abilities in twins 80 or more years old. Science, 276, 1560&ndash;1563.
*Murray, Charles (1998). Income Inequality and IQ, AEI Press
*Noguera, P.A. (2001). Racial politics and the elusive quest for excellence and equity in education.
*Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Craig, I. W., & McGuffin, P. (2003). ''Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era''. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
*Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. (2001). ''Behavioral genetics (4th ed.)''. New York: Worth Publishers.
* Rowe, D. C., W. J. Vesterdal, and J. L. Rodgers, "The Bell Curve Revisited: How Genes and Shared Environment Mediate IQ-SES Associations," University of Arizona, 1997
*Schoenemann, P.T., M.J. Sheehan, and L.D. Glotzer, Prefrontal white matter volume is disproportionately larger in humans than in other primates. Nat Neurosci, 2005.
* Tambs K, Sundet JM, Magnus P, Berg K. "Genetic and environmental contributions to the covariance between occupational status, educational attainment, and IQ: a study of twins." Behav Genet. 1989 Mar;19(2):209&ndash;22. PMID 2719624.
*Thompson, P.M., Cannon, T.D., Narr, K.L., Van Erp, T., Poutanen, V.-P., Huttunen, M., Lönnqvist, J., Standertskjöld-Nordenstam, C.-G., Kaprio, J., Khaledy, M., Dail, R., Zoumalan, C.I., Toga, A.W. (2001). "Genetic influences on brain structure." Nature Neuroscience 4, 1253-1258.


In the United States, certain ] and laws regarding military service,<ref>{{cite report|url=https://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/technical_reports/2005/RAND_TR193.pdf |title=Determinants of Productivity for Military Personnel|last=Kavanagh|first=Jennifer|date=2005 |publisher=RAND Corporation|location=Santa Monica, CA |isbn=0-8330-3754-4|access-date=1 July 2017}}</ref><ref>{{cite report |url=https://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/monograph_reports/2009/MR818.pdf |title=Estimating AFQT Scores for National Educational Longitudinal Study (NELS) Respondents|last1=Kilburn |first1=M. Rebecca|last2=Hanser |first2=Lawrence M.|date=2009 |publisher=RAND Corporation|location=Santa Monica, CA |last3=Klerman|first3=Jacob A.|access-date=1 July 2017}}</ref> education, public benefits,<ref>{{Cite web |url=https://www.ssa.gov/disability/professionals/bluebook/12.00-MentalDisorders-Adult.htm |title=12.00-Mental Disorders-Adult |website=www.ssa.gov|publisher=U. S. Social Security Administration |access-date=1 July 2017}}</ref> capital punishment,{{sfn|Flynn|2009|p={{Page needed|date=January 2011}}}} and employment incorporate an individual's IQ into their decisions. However, in the case of '']'' in 1971, for the purpose of minimizing employment practices that disparately impacted racial minorities, the ] banned the use of IQ tests in employment, except when linked to job performance via a ]. Internationally, certain public policies, such as improving nutrition and prohibiting ], have as one of their goals raising, or preventing a decline in, intelligence.
]
]


A diagnosis of ] is in part based on the results of IQ testing. ] is the categorization of individuals of below-average cognitive ability (an IQ of 71–85), although not as low as those with an intellectual disability (70 or below).
]

]
In the United Kingdom, the ] which incorporated an intelligence test has been used from 1945 to decide, at eleven years of age, which type of school a child should go to. They have been much less used since the widespread introduction of ]s.
]

]
==Classification==
]
{{Main|IQ classification}}
]
]. When asked his IQ, he replied: "I have no idea. People who boast about their IQ are losers."<ref>{{Cite news|last=Solomon |first=Deborah |date=12 December 2004|title=The Science of Second-Guessing|work=The New York Times |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2004/12/12/magazine/the-science-of-secondguessing.html}}</ref>]]
]

]
IQ classification is the practice used by IQ test publishers for designating IQ score ranges into various categories with labels such as "superior" or "average".{{sfn|Kaufman|Lichtenberger|2006}} IQ classification was preceded historically by attempts to classify human beings by general ability based on other forms of behavioral observation. Those other forms of behavioral observation are still important for validating classifications based on IQ tests.
]

]
==High-IQ societies==
]
{{Main|High-IQ society}}
]

]
There are social organizations, some international, which limit membership to people who have scores as high as or higher than the 98th percentile (two standard deviations above the mean) on some IQ test or equivalent. ] is perhaps the best known of these. The largest 99.9th percentile (three standard deviations above the mean) society is the ].
]

]
==See also==
]
* ]
]
* ] (EI), also known as emotional quotient (EQ) and emotional intelligence quotient (EIQ)
]
* ]
]
* ]
]
* ]
]
* ]
]

==Citations==
<!-- This article uses the citation templates described in ]. Thank you to RexxS for the detailed tips. -->

{{Reflist|30em|refs=
<ref name="Gottfredson2009pp31–32">{{Harvnb |Gottfredson|2009|pages=31–32}}</ref>
<ref name="Hunt2011p5">{{Harvnb |Hunt|2011|page=5}} "As mental testing expanded to the evaluation of adolescents and adults, however, there was a need for a measure of intelligence that did not depend upon mental age. Accordingly the intelligence quotient (IQ) was developed.&nbsp;... The narrow definition of IQ is a score on an intelligence test&nbsp;... where 'average' intelligence, that is the median level of performance on an intelligence test, receives a score of 100, and other scores are assigned so that the scores are distributed normally about 100, with a standard deviation of 15. Some of the implications are that: 1. Approximately two-thirds of all scores lie between 85 and 115. 2. Five percent (1/20) of all scores are above 125, and one percent (1/100) are above 135. Similarly, five percent are below 75 and one percent below 65."</ref>
<ref name="TermanOldClasses">{{Harvnb |Terman|1916|page=}} "What do the above IQ's imply in such terms as feeble-mindedness, border-line intelligence, dullness, normality, superior intelligence, genius, etc.? When we use these terms two facts must be born in mind: (1) That the boundary lines between such groups are absolutely arbitrary, a matter of definition only; and (2) that the individuals comprising one of the groups do not make up a homogeneous type."</ref>
<ref name="WechslerOldClasses">{{Harvnb |Wechsler|1939|page=37}} "The earliest classifications of intelligence were very rough ones. To a large extent they were practical attempts to define various patterns of behavior in medical-legal terms."</ref>
<ref name="Kaufman2009p21">{{Harvnb |Kaufman|2009|page=21}} "Galton's so-called intelligence test was misnamed."</ref>
<ref name="Kaufman2009">{{Harvnb |Kaufman|2009}}</ref>
<ref name="Urbina2011Table2.1">{{Harvnb |Urbina|2011|loc=Table 2.1 Major Examples of Current Intelligence Tests}}</ref>
<ref name="FlanaganHarrison2012chs8-16">{{Harvnb |Flanagan|Harrison|2012|loc=chapters 8–13, 15–16}} (discussing Wechsler, Stanford–Binet, Kaufman, Woodcock–Johnson, DAS, CAS, and RIAS tests)</ref>
<ref name="Kaufman2009Fig5.1">{{Harvnb |Kaufman|2009|loc=Figure 5.1 IQs earned by preadolescents (ages 12–13) who were given three different IQ tests in the early 2000s}}</ref>
<ref name="KaufmanSB2013Fig3.1">{{Harvnb |Kaufman|2013|loc=Figure 3.1 "Source: {{harvp|Kaufman|2009}}. Adapted with permission."}}</ref>
}}

==General and cited references==
{{Refbegin|30em}}
<!-- This article uses the citation templates described in ]. Thank you to RexxS for the detailed tips. -->
<!-- Aiken1979 -->
* {{cite book |last=Aiken |first=Lewis |title=Psychological Testing and Assessment |edition=3rd |location=Boston |publisher=Allyn and Bacon |isbn=978-0-205-06613-1 |date=1979}}
<!-- Aiken1996 -->
* {{cite book |last=Aiken |first=Lewis R. |title=Assessment of Intellectual Functioning |date=1996 |edition=2nd |location=New York |publisher=Plenum Press |isbn=978-0-306-48431-5 |series=Perspectives on Individual Differences |lccn=95026038}}
<!-- DSM5 alphabetizes as American Psychiatric Association -->
* {{cite book |author=American Psychiatric Association |date=2013 |title=Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders |edition=5th |publisher=American Psychiatric Publishing |location=Arlington, VA |isbn=978-0-89042-555-8}}
<!-- AnastasiUrbina1997 -->
* {{cite book |last1=Anastasi |first1=Anne |last2=Urbina |first2=Susana |author-link1=Anne Anastasi |author-link2=Susana Urbina |title=Psychological Testing |edition=7th |location=Upper Saddle River, NJ |publisher=] |isbn=978-0023030857 |date=1997 }}
<!-- BinetSimon1916 the citation here is to the English translation -->
* {{cite book |last1=Binet |first1=Alfred |last2=Simon |first2=Th. |author-link1=Alfred Binet |title=The development of intelligence in children (The Binet–Simon Scale) |translator=E. S. Kite |location=Baltimore, MD |publisher=Williams & Wilkins |date=1916 |series=Publications of the Training School at Vineland New Jersey Department of Research |volume=11 |url=https://archive.org/details/developmentofint00binerich |access-date=18 July 2010 }}
<!-- Borsboom2006 -->
* {{cite journal |last=Borsboom |first=Denny |title=The attack of the psychometricians |journal=Psychometrika |volume=71 |issue=3 |pages=425–440 |date=September 2006 |doi=10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6 |pmc=2779444 |pmid=19946599}}
<!-- Brody2005 -->
* {{cite book |last=Brody |first=Nathan |chapter=Chapter 26: To g or Not to g—That Is the Question |title=Handbook of Understanding and Measuring Intelligence |editor1-last=Wilhelm |editor1-first=Oliver |editor2-last=Engle |editor2-first=Randall W. |location=Thousand Oaks, CA |publisher=] |date=2005 |pages= |isbn=978-0-7619-2887-4 |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/handbookofunders00wilh/page/489 |chapter-url-access=registration }}
<!-- Campbell2006 -->
* {{cite book |last=Campbell |first=Jonathan M. |title=Psychodiagnostic Assessment of Children: Dimensional and Categorical Approaches |editor1-last=Campbell |editor1-first=Jonathan M. |editor2-last=Kamphaus |editor2-first=Randy W. |chapter=Chapter 3: Mental Retardation/Intellectual Disability |location=Hoboken, NJ |publisher=Wiley |isbn=978-0-471-21219-5 |date=2006 }}
<!-- Carroll1993 -->
* {{cite book |last=Carroll |first=John B. |title=Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies |location=New York |publisher=] |isbn=978-0-521-38275-5 |date=1993 |url=http://steinhardtapps.es.its.nyu.edu/create/courses/2174/reading/Carroll_1.pdf |access-date=15 May 2014 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20140714201804/http://steinhardtapps.es.its.nyu.edu/create/courses/2174/reading/Carroll_1.pdf |archive-date=14 July 2014 |url-status=dead }}
<!-- Carroll1998 -->
* {{cite book |last=Carroll |first=John B. |chapter=Human Cognitive Abilities: A Critique |title=Human Cognitive Abilities in Theory and Practice |editor1-last=McArdle |editor1-first=John J. |editor2-last=Woodcock |editor2-first=Richard W. |location=Mahwah, NJ |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |date=1998 |pages=5–23 |isbn=978-0-8058-2717-0}}
<!-- Ceci2009 -->
* {{Cite journal|last1=Ceci|first1=Stephen|last2=Williams|first2=Wendy M.|date=1 February 2009|title=Should scientists study race and IQ? YES: The scientific truth must be pursued|journal=Nature|volume=457|issue=7231|pages=788–789|doi=10.1038/457788a|pmid=19212385|s2cid=205044224|quote=There is an emerging consensus about racial and gender equality in genetic determinants of intelligence; most researchers, including ourselves, agree that genes do not explain between-group differences.|doi-access=free}}
<!-- Cox1926 -->
* {{cite book |title=The Early Mental Traits of 300 Geniuses |last=Cox |first=Catherine M. |date=1926 |publisher=Stanford University Press |location=Stanford, CA |series=Genetic Studies of Genius Volume 2}}
<!-- Deary2001 -->
* {{cite book |last=Deary |first=Ian |author-link=Ian Deary |title=Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction |location=Oxford |publisher=] |isbn=978-0-19-289321-5 |date=2001}}
<!-- DearyBatty2007 -->
* {{cite journal |last1=Deary |first1=Ian J. |last2=Batty |first2=G. David |date=2007 |title=Cognitive epidemiology |journal=Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health |volume=61 |issue=5 |pages=378–384 |pmc=2465694 |pmid=17435201 |doi=10.1136/jech.2005.039206}}
<!-- Deary Johnson Houlihan 2009 -->
* {{Cite journal |last1=Deary |first1=I. J. |last2=Johnson |first2=W. |last3=Houlihan |first3=L. M. |year=2009 |title=Genetic foundations of human intelligence |journal=Human Genetics |volume=126 |issue=1 |pages=215–232 |doi=10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 |pmid=19294424 |s2cid=4975607 |hdl=20.500.11820/c3e0a75b-dad6-4860-91c6-b242221af681 |url=https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895353/genetic_foundations_of_human_intelligence.pdf |hdl-access=free }}
<!-- Deary et al. 2007 -->
* {{Cite journal |last1=Deary|first1=I. J.|last2=Strand|first2=S.|last3=Smith|first3=P. |last4=Fernandes |first4=C. |year=2007 |title=Intelligence and educational achievement |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=1 |pages=13–21 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001}}
<!-- Detterman Daniel 1989 -->
* {{cite journal |last1=Detterman |first1=D.K. |last2=Daniel |first2=M.H. |year=1989 |title=Correlations of mental tests with each other and with cognitive variables are highest for low IQ groups |journal=Intelligence |volume=13 |issue=4| pages=349–359 |doi=10.1016/s0160-2896(89)80007-8}}
<!-- DumontWillisElliot2009 -->
* {{cite book |title=Essentials of DAS-II® Assessment |url=https://archive.org/details/essentialsdasiia00dumo |url-access=limited |last1=Dumont |first1=Ron |last2=Willis |first2=John O. |last3=Elliot |first3=Colin D. |date=2009 |publisher=Wiley |location=Hoboken, NJ |isbn=978-0470-22520-2 |page= }}
<!-- DumontWillis2013 -->
* {{cite web |url=http://alpha.fdu.edu/psychology/range_of_scores.htm |title=Range of DAS Subtest Scaled Scores |last1=Dumont |first1=Ron |last2=Willis |first2=John O. |date=2013 |website=Dumont Willis |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20140407061357/http://alpha.fdu.edu/psychology/range_of_scores.htm |archive-date=7 April 2014 }}
<!-- Eysenck1995 -->
* {{cite book |last=Eysenck |first=Hans |title=Genius: The Natural History of Creativity |series=Problems in the Behavioural Sciences No. 12 |location=Cambridge |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=978-0-5-2148508-1 |date=1995 |url=https://archive.org/details/geniusnaturalhis00eyse }}
<!-- Eysenck1998 -->
* {{cite book |last=Eysenck |first=Hans |title=Intelligence: A New Look |location=New Brunswick, NJ |publisher=] |isbn=978-0-7658-0707-6 |date=1998}}
<!-- FlanaganHarrison2012 -->
* {{cite book |title=Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, tests, and issues |edition=3rd |editor1-last=Flanagan |editor1-first=Dawn P. |editor2-last=Harrison |editor2-first=Patti L. |location=New York |publisher=] |isbn=978-1-60918-995-2 |date=2012 }}
<!-- FlanaganKaufman2009 -->
* {{cite book |title=Essentials of WISC-IV Assessment |last1=Flanagan |first1=Dawn P. |last2=Kaufman |first2=Alan S. |author-link2=Alan S. Kaufman |edition=2nd |date=2009 |publisher=] |location=Hoboken, NJ |series=Essentials of Psychological Assessment |isbn=978-0470189153}}
<!-- FletcherHattie2011 -->
* {{cite book |last1=Fletcher |first1=Richard B. |last2=Hattie |first2=John |title=Intelligence and Intelligence Testing |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=8pzDawey6akC |access-date=31 August 2013 |date=11 March 2011 |publisher=Taylor & Francis |isbn=978-1-136-82321-3 }}
<!-- FlintGreenspanKendler2010 -->
* {{cite book |last1=Flint |first1=Jonathan |last2=Greenspan |first2=Ralph J. |last3=Kendler |first3=Kenneth S. |title=How Genes Influence Behavior |url=https://archive.org/details/howgenesinfl_flin_2010_000_10540804 |url-access=registration |date=28 January 2010 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-955990-9 }}
**{{cite journal |author=Debby Tsuang |author2=Andrew David |date=June 2011 |title=How Genes Influence Behavior |journal=American Journal of Psychiatry |volume=168 |issue=6 |pages=656–657 |doi=10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11010097 |doi-access=free}}
<!-- Flynn2009 -->
* {{cite book |last=Flynn |first=James R. |author-link=Jim Flynn (academic) |title=What Is Intelligence?: Beyond the Flynn Effect |location=Cambridge |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=978-0-521-74147-7 |date=2009 |title-link=What Is Intelligence?}}
**{{cite web |author=Cosma Shalizi |date=27 April 2009 |title=The Domestication of the Savage Mind |website=bactra.org |url=http://bactra.org/reviews/flynn-beyond/ }}
<!-- Flynn2012 -->
* {{Cite book |author=Flynn, James R. |title=Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century |location=Cambridge |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=978-1-107-60917-4 |date=2012}}
**{{cite journal |first=Lea |last=Winerman |title=Smarter than ever? |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/2013/03/smarter.aspx |journal=Monitor on Psychology |volume=44 |issue=3 |page=30 |date=March 2013 }}
<!-- FreyDetterman2004 -->
* {{cite journal |last1=Frey |first1=Meredith C. |last2=Detterman |first2=Douglas K. |title=Scholastic Assessment org? |journal=Psychological Science |volume=15 |issue=6 |pages=373–8 |date=2004 |pmid=15147489 |doi=10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00687.x |s2cid=12724085}}
<!-- Freides1972 -->
* {{cite book |last=Freides |first=David |chapter=Review of Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale, Third Revision |title=Seventh Mental Measurements Yearbook |url=https://archive.org/details/seventhmentalmea01buro |url-access=limited |editor=Oscar Buros |location=Highland Park, NJ |publisher=Gryphon Press |date=1972 |pages=–773 }}
<!-- GeorgasWeissvan de VijverSaklofske2003 -->
* {{cite book |title=Culture and Children's Intelligence: Cross-Cultural Analysis of the WISC-III |first1=James |last1=Georgas |first2=Lawrence |last2=Weiss |first3=Fons |last3=van de Vijver |first4=Donald |last4=Saklofske |editor1-first=James |editor1-last=Georgas |editor2-first=Lawrence |editor2-last=Weiss |editor3-first=Fons |editor3-last=van de Vijver |editor4-first=Donald |editor4-last=Saklofske |editor3-link=Fons van de Vijver |chapter=Preface |pages=xvx–xxxii |date=2003 |publisher=Academic Press |location=San Diego, CA |isbn=978-0-12-280055-9}}
<!-- Gottfredson1997a -->
* {{cite journal |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S. |date=1997 |author-link=Linda Gottfredson |title=Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial) |journal=Intelligence |issn=0160-2896 |volume=24 |pages=13–23 |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf |doi=10.1016/s0160-2896(97)90011-8 }}
<!-- Gottfredson1997b -->
* {{cite journal |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S. |title=Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life |journal=Intelligence |volume=24 |issue=1 |date=1997 |pages=79–132 |issn=0160-2896 |doi=10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90014-3 |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997whygmatters.pdf |access-date=7 July 2014 |citeseerx=10.1.1.535.4596 }}
<!-- Gottfredson1998 -->
* {{cite journal |first1=Linda S. |last1=Gottfredson |title=The general intelligence factor |journal=Scientific American Presents |volume=9 |issue=4 |pages=24–29 |date=1998 |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1998generalintelligencefactor.pdf }}
<!-- Gottfredson2005 -->
* {{cite book |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S. |title=Destructive Trends In Mental Health: The Well-Intentioned Path to Harm |editor1-last=Wright |editor1-first=Rogers H. |editor2-last=Cummings |editor2-first=Nicholas A. |date=11 March 2005 |publisher=Taylor & Francis |isbn=978-0-203-95622-9 |chapter=Chapter 9: Suppressing Intelligence Research: Hurting Those We Intend to Help |pages=155–186 |chapter-url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/2005suppressingintelligence.pdf }}
<!-- Gottfredson2006 -->
* {{cite book |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S. |trans-chapter=Social consequences of group differences in cognitive ability |chapter=Conseqüências sociais das diferenças de grupo na capacidade cognitiva |title=Introdução à Psicologia das Diferenças Individuais |trans-title=Introduction to the psychology of individual differences |editor1-last=Flores-Mendoza |editor1-first=Carmen E. |editor2-last=Colom |editor2-first=Roberto |location=Porto Alegre, Brazil |publisher=ArtMed Publishers |date=2006 |pages=433–456 |isbn=978-85-363-1418-1 |chapter-url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/2004socialconsequences.pdf }}
<!--Gottfredson2009 -->
* {{cite book |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S. |chapter=Chapter 1: Logical Fallacies Used to Dismiss the Evidence on Intelligence Testing |title=Correcting Fallacies about Educational and Psychological Testing |editor-last=Phelps |editor-first=Richard F. |date=2009 |publisher=American Psychological Association |location=Washington, DC |isbn=978-1-4338-0392-5}}
<!-- Gould1981 -->
* {{cite book|last=Gould |first=Stephen Jay |author-link=Stephen Jay Gould |title=The Mismeasure of Man |location=New York |publisher=W. W. Norton |isbn=978-0-393-30056-7 |date=1981}}
**{{cite news |author=Christopher Lehmann-Haupt |date=21 October 1981 |title=Books Of The Times: The Mismeasure of Man |type=Review |url=https://www.nytimes.com/books/97/11/09/home/gould-mismeasure.html |newspaper=The New York Times }}
<!-- Gould1996 -->
* {{cite book |last=Gould |first=Stephen Jay |author-link=Stephen Jay Gould |title=The Mismeasure of Man |location=New York |publisher=W. W. Norton |isbn=978-0-393-31425-0 |date=1996 |edition=Rev. and expanded |url=https://archive.org/details/mismeasureofman00goul_1 }}
<!-- Gregory1995 -->
* {{cite encyclopedia |last=Gregory |first=Robert J. |title=Classification of Intelligence |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J. |editor-link=Robert J. Sternberg |encyclopedia=Encyclopedia of human intelligence |date=1995 |publisher=Macmillan |isbn=978-0-02-897407-1 |volume=1 |pages=260–266 |url=https://archive.org/details/encyclopediaofhu0000unse/page/260 }}
<!-- Groth-Marnat2009 -->
* {{cite book |title=Handbook of Psychological Assessment |last=Groth-Marnat |first=Gary |date=2009 |publisher=Wiley |location=Hoboken, NJ |isbn=978-0-470-08358-1 |edition=5th}}
<!-- Harris2009 -->
* {{cite book |last=Harris |first=Judith Rich |author-link=Judith Rich Harris |title=The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do |date=2009 |edition=2nd |publisher=Free Press |isbn=978-1-4391-0165-0}}
**{{cite interview |subject=Judith Rich Harris |interviewer=Jonah Lehrer |date=9 April 2009 |title=Do Parents Matter? |magazine=Scientific American |url=http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/parents-peers-children/ }}
<!-- HopkinsStanley1981 -->
* {{cite book |last1=Hopkins |first1=Kenneth D. |last2=Stanley |first2=Julian C. |title=Educational and Psychological Measurement and Evaluation |edition=6th |location=Engelwood Cliffs, NJ |publisher=] |isbn=978-0-13-236273-3 |date=1981}}
<!-- Hunt2001 -->
* {{cite journal |last1=Hunt |first1=Earl |title=Multiple Views of Multiple Intelligence |journal=PsycCRITIQUES |volume=46 |issue=1 |pages=5–7 |date=2001 |doi=10.1037/002513}}
<!-- Hunt2011 -->
* {{cite book |last1=Hunt |first1=Earl B. |author-link1=Earl B. Hunt |date=2011 |title=Human Intelligence |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge |isbn=978-0-521-70781-7 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=DwO4TtKAiCoC }}
<!-- Jensen1969 this is a citation to the one-volume reprint -->
* {{cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur |title=Environment, Heredity, and Intelligence |series=Harvard Educational Review Reprint Series |volume=2 |date=1969 |publisher=Harvard Educational Review |location=Cambridge, MA |pages=1–123 |chapter=How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement? |lccn=71087869 |isbn=978-0916690021}}
**{{cite periodical |date=9 October 1978 |title=Jensen A R. How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? |type=Review |periodical=Citation Classics |number=41 |url=http://www.garfield.library.upenn.edu/classics1978/A1978FQ53600002.pdf }}
<!-- Jensen1980 -->
* {{cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R. |title=Bias in mental testing |location=New York |publisher=] |isbn=978-0-02-916430-3 |date=1980}}<br/>{{*}}{{cite journal|ref=none |last=Scarr |first=Sandra |title=Implicit Messages: A Review of ''Bias in Mental Testing'' |journal=American Journal of Education |volume=89 |issue=3 |date=1981 |pages=330–338 |doi=10.1086/443584 |jstor=1084961|s2cid=147214993 }}
<!-- Jensen1998 -->
* {{cite book |title=The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R. |date=1998 |publisher=Praeger |location=Westport, CT |isbn=978-0-275-96103-9 |issn=1063-2158 |series=Human Evolution, Behavior, and Intelligence |url=https://archive.org/details/gfactorscienc00jens }}<br/>{{*}}{{cite journal |ref=none |last=Locurto |first=Charles |date=1999 |title=A Balance Sheet on Persistence: Book Review of Jensen on Intelligence-g-Factor |journal=Psycoloquy |volume=10 |issue=59 |at=9 |url=http://www.cogsci.ecs.soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?10.059 }}
<!-- Jensen2006 -->
* {{cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R. |title=Clocking the Mind: Mental Chronometry and Individual Differences |url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/book/9780080449395 |access-date=7 July 2014 |date=10 July 2006 |publisher=Elsevier |isbn=978-0-08-044939-5 }}<br/>{{*}}{{cite journal |ref=none |last=Wai |first=Jonathan |title=Book Review: Jensen, A. R. (2006). Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences. Amsterdam: Elsevier. (ISBN 978-0-08-044939-5) |journal=Gifted Child Quarterly |date=2008 |volume=52 |page=99 |doi=10.1177/0016986207310434 |s2cid=143666885 |url=http://www.psychologytoday.com/files/attachments/56143/bookreviewclockingthemind.pdf }}
<!-- Jensen2011 -->
* {{cite journal |title=The Theory of Intelligence and Its Measurement |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R. |date=2011 |journal=Intelligence |volume=39 |issue=4 |pages=171–177 |issn=0160-2896 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2011.03.004}}
<!-- Johnson2012 -->
* {{cite book |last=Johnson |first=Wendy |title=Developmental Psychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies |editor1-last=Slater |editor1-first=Alan M. |editor2-last=Quinn |editor2-first=Paul C. |chapter=How Much Can We Boost IQ? An Updated Look at Jensen's (1969) Question and Answer |location=Thousand Oaks, CA |publisher=SAGE |isbn=978-0-85702-757-3 |date=2012}}<br/>{{*}}{{cite journal |ref=none |last=Gamboa |first=Camille |date=May 2013 |title=(Review) Developmental Psychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies, by Alan M. Slater and Paul C. Quinn |journal=Choice |volume=50 |issue=9 |url=http://www.cne.psychol.cam.ac.uk/pdfs/choice |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20141010173731/http://www.cne.psychol.cam.ac.uk/pdfs/choice |archive-date=10 October 2014 }}
<!-- JohnsonTurkheimerGottesmanBouchard2009 -->
* {{cite journal |last1=Johnson |first1=Wendy |last2=Turkheimer |first2=E. |last3=Gottesman |first3=Irving |last4=Bouchard |first4=Thomas |date=2009 |title=Beyond Heritability: Twin Studies in Behavioral Research |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |volume=18 |issue=4 |pages=217–220 |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Johnson%20%282009%29.pdf |access-date=29 June 2010 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01639.x |pmc=2899491 |pmid=20625474 }}
<!-- Kaufman2009 -->
* {{cite book |title=IQ Testing 101 |last=Kaufman |first=Alan S. |author-link=Alan S. Kaufman |date=2009 |publisher=Springer Publishing |location=New York |isbn=978-0-8261-0629-2 |url=https://archive.org/details/iqtestingpsych00phdd |url-access=limited }}
<!-- KaufmanLichtenberger2006 -->
* {{cite book |title=Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence |last1=Kaufman |first1=Alan S. |last2=Lichtenberger |first2=Elizabeth O. |author-link1=Alan S. Kaufman |edition=3rd |date=2006 |publisher=] |location=Hoboken, NJ |isbn=978-0-471-73553-3 }}
<!-- KaufmanSB2013 -->
* {{cite book |last=Kaufman |first=Scott Barry |title=Ungifted: Intelligence Redefined |url=https://archive.org/details/ungiftedintellig0000kauf |url-access=registration |access-date=1 October 2013 |date=1 June 2013 |publisher=Basic Books |isbn=978-0-465-02554-1 }}
**{{cite magazine |title=Ungifted: Intelligence Redefined: The Truth about Talent, Practice, Creativity, and the Many Paths to Greatness |magazine=Publishers Weekly |type=Review |url=http://www.publishersweekly.com/978-0-465-02554-1 }}
<!-- KranzlerFloyd2013 -->
* {{cite book |last1=Kranzler |first1=John H. |last2=Floyd |first2=Randy G. |title=Assessing Intelligence in Children and Adolescents: A Practical Guide |date=1 August 2013 |publisher=Guilford Press |isbn=978-1-4625-1121-1 |url=http://www.guilfordpress.co.uk/books/details/9781462511211/ |access-date=9 June 2014 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20141016201213/http://www.guilfordpress.co.uk/books/details/9781462511211/ |archive-date=16 October 2014 |url-status=dead }}
<!-- LahnEbenstein2009 -->
* {{cite journal |last1=Lahn |first1=Bruce T. |last2=Ebenstein |first2=Lanny | author-link2 = Alan O. Ebenstein | title=Let's celebrate human genetic diversity |journal=Nature |volume=461 |issue=7265 |pages=726–728 |date=2009 |issn=0028-0836 |pmid=19812654 |doi=10.1038/461726a |bibcode=2009Natur.461..726L |s2cid=205050141}}
<!-- LohmanFoley Nicpon2012 -->
* {{cite book |title=Identification: The Theory and Practice of Identifying Students for Gifted and Talented Education Services |last1=Lohman |first1=David F. |last2=Foley Nicpon |first2=Megan |editor-last=Hunsaker |editor-first=Scott |chapter=Chapter 12: Ability Testing & Talent Identification |pages=287–386 |chapter-url=https://faculty.education.uiowa.edu/docs/default-source/dlohman/ability-testing-and-talent-identification.pdf?sfvrsn=0 |date=2012 |publisher=Prufrock |location=Waco, TX |isbn=978-1-931280-17-4 |access-date=2014-07-07 |archive-date=2016-03-15 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160315055952/https://faculty.education.uiowa.edu/docs/default-source/dlohman/ability-testing-and-talent-identification.pdf?sfvrsn=0 |url-status=dead }}
<!-- Mackintosh1998 -->
* {{Cite book |title=IQ and Human Intelligence |last=Mackintosh |first=N. J. |author-link=Nicholas Mackintosh |year=1998 |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford |isbn=978-0-19-852367-3 |url=https://archive.org/details/iqhumanintellige00mack }}
<!-- Mackintosh2011 -->
* {{cite book |title=IQ and Human Intelligence |last=Mackintosh |first=N. J. |author-link=Nicholas Mackintosh |date=2011 |edition=2nd |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford |isbn=978-0-19-958559-5 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=BcKcAQAAQBAJ }}
<!-- Matarazzo1972 -->
* {{cite book |title=Wechsler's Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence |last=Matarazzo |first=Joseph D. |date=1972 |edition=5th |publisher=Williams & Witkins |location=Baltimore, MD}}
**{{cite journal |author=R. D. Savage |date=April 1974 |title=Wechsler's Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence, 5th ed |type= Review |journal=British Journal of Industrial Medicine |volume=31 |issue=2 |page=169 |pmc=1009574}}
<!-- McIntoshDixonPierson2012 -->
* {{harvc |last1=McIntosh|first1=David E. |last2=Dixon|first2=Felicia A. |last3=Pierson|first3=Eric E. |in1=Flanagan|in2=Harrison |c=Chapter 25: Use of Intelligence Tests in the Identification of Giftedness |pp=623–642 |year=2012}}
<!-- Murray1998 -->
* {{Cite book |last=Murray |first=Charles |title=Income Inequality and IQ |location=Washington, DC |publisher=] |isbn=978-0-8447-7094-9 |date=1998 |url=https://www.aei.org/wp-content/uploads/2011/10/20040302_book443.pdf |access-date=7 July 2014 }}<br/>{{*}}{{cite magazine |ref=none |last=Loury |first=Glenn C. |date=18 May 1998 |title=Charles II |department=Hard Questions Column |url=http://www.bu.edu/irsd/articles/charles2.htm |magazine=The New Republic |access-date=7 July 2014 |archive-date=2015-09-24 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150924042546/http://www.bu.edu/irsd/articles/charles2.htm |url-status=dead }}
<!-- Naglieri1999 -->
* {{cite book |title=Essentials of CAS Assessment |last=Naglieri |first=Jack A. |date=1999 |publisher=Wiley |location=Hoboken, NJ |series=Essentials of Psychological Assessment |isbn=978-0-471-29015-5}}
<!-- NeisserBoodooBouchard1996 formerly named APA-report -->
* {{cite journal |ref={{sfnref|Neisser et al.|1995}} |display-authors=8 |last1=Neisser |first1=Ulrich |last2=Boodoo |first2=Gwyneth |last3=Bouchard |first3=Thomas J. |last4=Boykin |first4=A. Wade |last5=Brody |first5=Nathan |last6=Ceci |first6=Stephen J. |last7=Halpern |first7=Diane F. |last8=Loehlin |first8=John C. |last9=Perloff |first9=Robert |last10=Sternberg |first10=Robert J. |last11=Urbina |first11=Susana |author-link1=Ulrich Neisser |author-link10=Robert Sternberg |title=Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns |journal=American Psychologist |issn=0003-066X |volume=51 |issue=2 |pages=77–101 |year=1996 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.51.2.77 |url=http://psych.colorado.edu/~carey/pdfFiles/IQ_Neisser2.pdf |access-date=9 October 2014 |s2cid=20957095 }}
<!-- Noguera2001 -->
* {{Cite journal |last=Noguera |first=Pedro A. |title=Racial politics and the elusive quest for excellence and equity in education |journal=In Motion Magazine |date=30 September 2001 |id=Article # ER010930002 |url=http://www.inmotionmagazine.com/er/pnrp1.html |access-date=7 July 2014 }}
<!-- PerlethSchatzMönks2000 -->
* {{cite book |last1=Perleth |first1=Christoph |last2=Schatz |first2=Tanja |last3=Mönks |first3=Franz J. |title=International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent |url=https://archive.org/details/internationalhan00hell |url-access=limited |editor1-last=Heller |editor1-first=Kurt A. |editor2-last=Mönks |editor2-first=Franz J. |editor3-last=Sternberg |editor3-first=Robert J. |editor4-last=Subotnik |editor4-first=Rena F. |edition=2nd |date=2000 |publisher=Pergamon |location=Amsterdam |isbn=978-0-08-043796-5 |pages=–316 |chapter=Early Identification of High Ability |quote=a gifted sample gathered using IQ > 132 using the old SB L-M in 1985 does not contain the top 2% of the population but the best 10%. |editor3-link=Robert Sternberg }}
<!-- PlominDeFriesKnopikNeiderhiser2012 -->
* {{cite book|last1=Plomin |first1=Robert |last2=DeFries |first2=John C. |last3=Knopik |first3=Valerie S. |last4=Neiderhiser |first4=Jenae M. |title=Behavioral Genetics |date=2013 |publisher=Worth Publishers |isbn=978-1-4292-4215-8 |edition=6th}}
<!-- Shurkin1992 -->
* {{cite journal |last=Reddy |first=Ajitha |year=2008 |title=The Eugenic Origins of IQ Testing: Implications for Post-Atkins Litigation |url=https://via.library.depaul.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1270&context=law-review |journal=] |volume=57 |pages=667–677 }}
* {{cite book |title=Terman's Kids: The Groundbreaking Study of How the Gifted Grow Up |last=Shurkin |first=Joel |date=1992 |publisher=Little, Brown |location=Boston, MA |isbn=978-0-316-78890-8 |url=https://archive.org/details/termanskids00joel }}
** {{cite news |author=Frederic Golden |date=31 May 1992 |title=Tracking the IQ Elite : TERMAN'S KIDS: The Groundbreaking Study of How the Gifted Grow Up, By Joel N. Shurkin |url=https://www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-1992-05-31-bk-1247-story.html |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20121108031753/http://articles.latimes.com/1992-05-31/books/bk-1247_1_lewis-terman |archive-date=8 November 2012 |url-status=live |newspaper=Los Angeles Times }}
<!-- Stern1912, Stern1914 as harv ref -->
* {{cite book |last=Stern |first=William |author-link=William Stern (psychologist) |location=Baltimore, MD |publisher=Warwick & York |date=1914 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=D6h9AAAAMAAJ |access-date=15 June 2014 |title=The Psychological Methods of Testing Intelligence |translator=Guy Montrose Whipple |series=Educational psychology monographs |volume=13 |isbn=9781981604999 |oclc=4521857 |lccn=14010447 }}<br/>{{cite book|ref=none |last=Stern |first=William |title=Die psychologischen Methoden der Intelligenzprüfung: und deren Anwendung an Schulkindern |location=Leipzig |publisher=J. A. Barth |date=1912 |language=de |trans-title=The Psychological Methods of Testing Intelligence}}
<!-- Terman et al. 1915 -->
* {{cite journal |ref={{sfnref|Terman et al.|1915}} |last1=Terman |first1=Lewis M. |last2=Lyman |first2=Grace |last3=Ordahl |first3=George |last4=Ordahl |first4=Louise |last5=Galbreath |first5=Neva |last6=Talbert |first6=Wilford |title=The Stanford revision of the Binet–Simon scale and some results from its application to 1000 non-selected children |journal=Journal of Educational Psychology |volume=6 |issue=9 |pages=551–62 |year=1915 |doi=10.1037/h0075455 |url=https://zenodo.org/record/1429169 }}
<!-- Terman1916 -->
* {{cite book |title=The Measurement of Intelligence: An Explanation of and a Complete Guide to the Use of the Stanford Revision and Extension of the Binet–Simon Intelligence Scale |last=Terman |first=Lewis M. |author-link=Lewis Terman |editor=Ellwood P. Cubberley |year=1916 |publisher=Houghton Mifflin |location=Boston |series=Riverside Textbooks in Education |url=https://www.gutenberg.org/ebooks/20662 |access-date=26 June 2010 }}
<!-- TermanMerrill1937 -->
* {{cite book |title=Measuring Intelligence: A Guide to the Administration of the New Revised Stanford–Binet Tests of Intelligence |last1=Terman |first1=Lewis M. |last2=Merrill A. |first2=Maude |author-link=Lewis Terman |year=1937 |publisher=Houghton Mifflin |location=Boston}}
<!-- TermanMerrill1960 -->
* {{cite book |title=Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale: Manual for the Third Revision Form L-M with Revised IQ Tables by Samuel R. Pinneau |last1=Terman |first1=Lewis M. |last2=Merrill |first2=Maude A. |author-link1=Lewis Terman |year=1960 |publisher=Houghton Mifflin |location=Boston, MA}}
<!-- Turkheimer2008 -->
* {{cite journal |last=Turkheimer |first=Eric |date=April 2008 |title=A Better Way to Use Twins for Developmental Research |journal=LIFE Newsletter |volume=2 |issue=1 |pages=2–5 |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Turkheimer%20%282008%29.pdf |access-date=29 October 2010 }}
<!-- Urbina2011 -->
* {{Cite book |last=Urbina |first=Susana |title=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence |url=https://archive.org/details/cambridgehandboo00ster |url-access=limited |editor1-last=Sternberg |editor1-first=Robert J. |editor1-link=Robert Sternberg |editor2-last=Kaufman |editor2-first=Scott Barry |date=2011 |chapter=Chapter 2: Tests of Intelligence |pages=–38 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge |isbn=9780521739115 }}
<!-- Wasserman2012 -->
* {{harvc |last=Wasserman |first=John D. |in1=Flanagan|in2=Harrison |c=Chapter 1: A History of Intelligence Assessment: The Unfinished Tapestry |pp=3–55 |year=2012}}
<!-- Wechsler1939 -->
* {{cite book |last=Wechsler |first=David |author-link=David Wechsler |title=The Measurement of Adult Intelligence |date=1939 |edition=1st |publisher=Williams & Witkins |location=Baltimore, MD |lccn=39014016}}
<!-- Wechsler1997 -->
* {{Cite book |last=Wechsler |first=David |title=Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale |edition=3rd |location=San Antonio, TX |publisher=The Psychological Corporation |date=1997}}
<!-- Wechsler2003 -->
* {{Cite book |last=Wechsler |first=David |title=Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children |edition=4th |location=San Antonio, TX |publisher=The Psychological Corporation |date=2003}}
* {{cite book |editor1-last=Weiner |editor1-first=Irving B. |editor2-last=Graham |editor2-first=John R. |editor3-last=Naglieri |editor3-first=Jack A. |title=Handbook of Psychology, Volume 10: Assessment Psychology |url=http://www.wiley.com/WileyCDA/WileyTitle/productCd-0470891270.html |access-date=25 November 2013 |date=2 October 2012 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons |isbn=978-0-470-89127-8 }}
<!-- WeissSaklofskePrifiteraHoldnack2006 -->
* {{cite book |editor1-last=Weiss |editor1-first=Lawrence G. |editor2-last=Saklofske |editor2-first=Donald H. |editor3-last=Prifitera |editor3-first=Aurelio |editor4-last=Holdnack |editor4-first=James A. |title=WISC-IV Advanced Clinical Interpretation |date=2006 |publisher=Academic Press |location=Burlington, MA |isbn=978-0-12-088763-7 |series=Practical Resources for the Mental Health Professional}} This practitioner's handbook includes chapters by L.G. Weiss, J.G. Harris, A. Prifitera, T. Courville, E. Rolfhus, D.H. Saklofske, J.A. Holdnack, D. Coalson, S.E. Raiford, D.M. Schwartz, P. Entwistle, V. L. Schwean, and T. Oakland.
* {{cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Dolan |first2=Conor V. |last3=Carlson |first3=Jerry S. |last4=van der Maas |first4=Han L.J. |year=2010|title=Raven's test performance of sub-Saharan Africans: Average performance, psychometric properties, and the Flynn Effect |journal=Learning and Individual Differences |volume=20 |issue=3 |pages=135–151 |doi=10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.001 }}
* {{cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Dolan |first2=Conor V. |last3=van der Maas |first3=Han L.J. |year =2010|title=A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans |journal=Intelligence |volume=38 |issue=1 |pages=1–20 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002 }}
{{Refend}}

==External links==
{{Library resources box
|by=no
|onlinebooks=no
|others=no
|about=yes
|label=Intelligence quotient
}}
{{wikiquote}}
{{wiktionary|IQer|IQed}}

*

{{Human intelligence topics}}
{{High IQ|state=collapsed}}
{{Authority control}}

{{DEFAULTSORT:Intelligence Quotient}}
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 16:10, 6 January 2025

Score from a test designed to assess intelligence This article is about the history and applications of IQ. For the classifications of IQ, see IQ classification."IQ" redirects here. For other uses, see IQ (disambiguation).

Medical diagnostic method
Intelligence quotient
One kind of IQ test item, modelled after items in the Raven's Progressive Matrices test
ICD-10-PCSZ01.8
ICD-9-CM94.01
Part of a series on
Psychology
Basic psychology
Applied psychology
Concepts
Lists

An intelligence quotient (IQ) is a total score derived from a set of standardized tests or subtests designed to assess human intelligence. Originally, IQ was a score obtained by dividing a person's mental age score, obtained by administering an intelligence test, by the person's chronological age, both expressed in terms of years and months. The resulting fraction (quotient) was multiplied by 100 to obtain the IQ score. For modern IQ tests, the raw score is transformed to a normal distribution with mean 100 and standard deviation 15. This results in approximately two-thirds of the population scoring between IQ 85 and IQ 115 and about 2 percent each above 130 and below 70.

Scores from intelligence tests are estimates of intelligence. Unlike, for example, distance and mass, a concrete measure of intelligence cannot be achieved given the abstract nature of the concept of "intelligence". IQ scores have been shown to be associated with such factors as nutrition, parental socioeconomic status, morbidity and mortality, parental social status, and perinatal environment. While the heritability of IQ has been investigated for nearly a century, there is still debate about the significance of heritability estimates and the mechanisms of inheritance.

IQ scores are used for educational placement, assessment of intellectual ability, and evaluating job applicants. In research contexts, they have been studied as predictors of job performance and income. They are also used to study distributions of psychometric intelligence in populations and the correlations between it and other variables. Raw scores on IQ tests for many populations have been rising at an average rate that scales to three IQ points per decade since the early 20th century, a phenomenon called the Flynn effect. Investigation of different patterns of increases in subtest scores can also inform current research on human intelligence.

Historically, many proponents of IQ testing have been eugenicists who used pseudoscience to push now-debunked views of racial hierarchy in order to justify segregation and oppose immigration. Such views are now rejected by a strong consensus of mainstream science, though fringe figures continue to promote them in pseudo-scholarship and popular culture.

History

See also: History of the race and intelligence controversy

Precursors to IQ testing

Historically, even before IQ tests were devised, there were attempts to classify people into intelligence categories by observing their behavior in daily life. Those other forms of behavioral observation are still important for validating classifications based primarily on IQ test scores. Both intelligence classification by observation of behavior outside the testing room and classification by IQ testing depend on the definition of "intelligence" used in a particular case and on the reliability and error of estimation in the classification procedure.

The English statistician Francis Galton (1822–1911) made the first attempt at creating a standardized test for rating a person's intelligence. A pioneer of psychometrics and the application of statistical methods to the study of human diversity and the study of inheritance of human traits, he believed that intelligence was largely a product of heredity (by which he did not mean genes, although he did develop several pre-Mendelian theories of particulate inheritance). He hypothesized that there should exist a correlation between intelligence and other observable traits such as reflexes, muscle grip, and head size. He set up the first mental testing center in the world in 1882 and he published "Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development" in 1883, in which he set out his theories. After gathering data on a variety of physical variables, he was unable to show any such correlation, and he eventually abandoned this research.

Psychologist Alfred Binet, co-developer of the Stanford–Binet test

French psychologist Alfred Binet and psychiatrist Théodore Simon, had more success in 1905, when they published the Binet–Simon Intelligence test, which focused on verbal abilities. It was intended to identify "mental retardation" in school children, but in specific contradistinction to claims made by psychiatrists that these children were "sick" (not "slow") and should therefore be removed from school and cared for in asylums. The score on the Binet–Simon scale would reveal the child's mental age. For example, a six-year-old child who passed all the tasks usually passed by six-year-olds—but nothing beyond—would have a mental age that matched his chronological age, 6.0. (Fancher, 1985). Binet and Simon thought that intelligence was multifaceted, but came under the control of practical judgment.

In Binet and Simon's view, there were limitations with the scale and they stressed what they saw as the remarkable diversity of intelligence and the subsequent need to study it using qualitative, as opposed to quantitative, measures (White, 2000). American psychologist Henry H. Goddard published a translation of it in 1910. American psychologist Lewis Terman at Stanford University revised the Binet–Simon scale, which resulted in the Stanford revision of the Binet-Simon Intelligence Scale (1916). It became the most popular test in the United States for decades.

The abbreviation "IQ" was coined by the psychologist William Stern for the German term Intelligenzquotient, his term for a scoring method for intelligence tests at University of Breslau he advocated in a 1912 book.

General factor (g)

Main article: g factor

The many different kinds of IQ tests include a wide variety of item content. Some test items are visual, while many are verbal. Test items vary from being based on abstract-reasoning problems to concentrating on arithmetic, vocabulary, or general knowledge.

The British psychologist Charles Spearman in 1904 made the first formal factor analysis of correlations between the tests. He observed that children's school grades across seemingly unrelated school subjects were positively correlated, and reasoned that these correlations reflected the influence of an underlying general mental ability that entered into performance on all kinds of mental tests. He suggested that all mental performance could be conceptualized in terms of a single general ability factor and a large number of narrow task-specific ability factors. Spearman named it g for "general factor" and labeled the specific factors or abilities for specific tasks s. In any collection of test items that make up an IQ test, the score that best measures g is the composite score that has the highest correlations with all the item scores. Typically, the "g-loaded" composite score of an IQ test battery appears to involve a common strength in abstract reasoning across the test's item content.

United States military selection in World War I

During World War I, the Army needed a way to evaluate and assign recruits to appropriate tasks. This led to the development of several mental tests by Robert Yerkes, who worked with major hereditarians of American psychometrics—including Terman, Goddard—to write the test. The testing generated controversy and much public debate in the United States. Nonverbal or "performance" tests were developed for those who could not speak English or were suspected of malingering. Based on Goddard's translation of the Binet–Simon test, the tests had an impact in screening men for officer training:

...the tests did have a strong impact in some areas, particularly in screening men for officer training. At the start of the war, the army and national guard maintained nine thousand officers. By the end, two hundred thousand officers presided, and two- thirds of them had started their careers in training camps where the tests were applied. In some camps, no man scoring below C could be considered for officer training.

In total 1.75 million men were tested, making the results the first mass-produced written tests of intelligence, though considered dubious and non-usable, for reasons including high variability of test implementation throughout different camps and questions testing for familiarity with American culture rather than intelligence. After the war, positive publicity promoted by army psychologists helped to make psychology a respected field. Subsequently, there was an increase in jobs and funding in psychology in the United States. Group intelligence tests were developed and became widely used in schools and industry.

The results of these tests, which at the time reaffirmed contemporary racism and nationalism, are considered controversial and dubious, having rested on certain contested assumptions: that intelligence was heritable, innate, and could be relegated to a single number, the tests were enacted systematically, and test questions actually tested for innate intelligence rather than subsuming environmental factors. The tests also allowed for the bolstering of jingoist narratives in the context of increased immigration, which may have influenced the passing of the Immigration Restriction Act of 1924.

L.L. Thurstone argued for a model of intelligence that included seven unrelated factors (verbal comprehension, word fluency, number facility, spatial visualization, associative memory, perceptual speed, reasoning, and induction). While not widely used, Thurstone's model influenced later theories.

David Wechsler produced the first version of his test in 1939. It gradually became more popular and overtook the Stanford–Binet in the 1960s. It has been revised several times, as is common for IQ tests, to incorporate new research. One explanation is that psychologists and educators wanted more information than the single score from the Binet. Wechsler's ten or more subtests provided this. Another is that the Stanford–Binet test reflected mostly verbal abilities, while the Wechsler test also reflected nonverbal abilities. The Stanford–Binet has also been revised several times and is now similar to the Wechsler in several aspects, but the Wechsler continues to be the most popular test in the United States.

IQ testing and the eugenics movement in the United States

Eugenics, a set of beliefs and practices aimed at improving the genetic quality of the human population by excluding people and groups judged to be inferior and promoting those judged to be superior, played a significant role in the history and culture of the United States during the Progressive Era, from the late 19th century until US involvement in World War II.

The American eugenics movement was rooted in the biological determinist ideas of the British Scientist Sir Francis Galton. In 1883, Galton first used the word eugenics to describe the biological improvement of human genes and the concept of being "well-born". He believed that differences in a person's ability were acquired primarily through genetics and that eugenics could be implemented through selective breeding in order for the human race to improve in its overall quality, therefore allowing for humans to direct their own evolution.

Henry H. Goddard was a eugenicist. In 1908, he published his own version, The Binet and Simon Test of Intellectual Capacity, and cordially promoted the test. He quickly extended the use of the scale to the public schools (1913), to immigration (Ellis Island, 1914) and to a court of law (1914).

Unlike Galton, who promoted eugenics through selective breeding for positive traits, Goddard went with the US eugenics movement to eliminate "undesirable" traits. Goddard used the term "feeble-minded" to refer to people who did not perform well on the test. He argued that "feeble-mindedness" was caused by heredity, and thus feeble-minded people should be prevented from giving birth, either by institutional isolation or sterilization surgeries. At first, sterilization targeted the disabled, but was later extended to poor people. Goddard's intelligence test was endorsed by the eugenicists to push for laws for forced sterilization. Different states adopted the sterilization laws at different paces. These laws, whose constitutionality was upheld by the Supreme Court in their 1927 ruling Buck v. Bell, forced over 60,000 people to go through sterilization in the United States.

California's sterilization program was so effective that the Nazis turned to the government for advice on how to prevent the birth of the "unfit". While the US eugenics movement lost much of its momentum in the 1940s in view of the horrors of Nazi Germany, advocates of eugenics (including Nazi geneticist Otmar Freiherr von Verschuer) continued to work and promote their ideas in the United States. In later decades, some eugenic principles have made a resurgence as a voluntary means of selective reproduction, with some calling them "new eugenics". As it becomes possible to test for and correlate genes with IQ (and its proxies), ethicists and embryonic genetic testing companies are attempting to understand the ways in which the technology can be ethically deployed.

Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory

Main article: Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory
Psychologist Raymond Cattell defined fluid and crystallized intelligence and authored the Cattell Culture Fair III IQ test.

Raymond Cattell (1941) proposed two types of cognitive abilities in a revision of Spearman's concept of general intelligence. Fluid intelligence (Gf) was hypothesized as the ability to solve novel problems by using reasoning, and crystallized intelligence (Gc) was hypothesized as a knowledge-based ability that was very dependent on education and experience. In addition, fluid intelligence was hypothesized to decline with age, while crystallized intelligence was largely resistant to the effects of aging. The theory was almost forgotten, but was revived by his student John L. Horn (1966) who later argued Gf and Gc were only two among several factors, and who eventually identified nine or ten broad abilities. The theory continued to be called Gf-Gc theory.

John B. Carroll (1993), after a comprehensive reanalysis of earlier data, proposed the three stratum theory, which is a hierarchical model with three levels. The bottom stratum consists of narrow abilities that are highly specialized (e.g., induction, spelling ability). The second stratum consists of broad abilities. Carroll identified eight second-stratum abilities. Carroll accepted Spearman's concept of general intelligence, for the most part, as a representation of the uppermost, third stratum.

In 1999, a merging of the Gf-Gc theory of Cattell and Horn with Carroll's Three-Stratum theory has led to the Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory (CHC Theory), with g as the top of the hierarchy, ten broad abilities below, and further subdivided into seventy narrow abilities on the third stratum. CHC Theory has greatly influenced many of the current broad IQ tests.

Modern tests do not necessarily measure all of these broad abilities. For example, quantitative knowledge and reading and writing ability may be seen as measures of school achievement and not IQ. Decision speed may be difficult to measure without special equipment. g was earlier often subdivided into only Gf and Gc, which were thought to correspond to the nonverbal or performance subtests and verbal subtests in earlier versions of the popular Wechsler IQ test. More recent research has shown the situation to be more complex. Modern comprehensive IQ tests do not stop at reporting a single IQ score. Although they still give an overall score, they now also give scores for many of these more restricted abilities, identifying particular strengths and weaknesses of an individual.

Other theories

An alternative to standard IQ tests, meant to test the proximal development of children, originated in the writings of psychologist Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934) during his last two years of his life. According to Vygotsky, the maximum level of complexity and difficulty of problems that a child is capable to solve under some guidance indicates their level of potential development. The difference between this level of potential and the lower level of unassisted performance indicates the child's zone of proximal development. Combination of the two indexes—the level of actual and the zone of the proximal development—according to Vygotsky, provides a significantly more informative indicator of psychological development than the assessment of the level of actual development alone. His ideas on the zone of development were later developed in a number of psychological and educational theories and practices, most notably under the banner of dynamic assessment, which seeks to measure developmental potential (for instance, in the work of Reuven Feuerstein and his associates, who has criticized standard IQ testing for its putative assumption or acceptance of "fixed and immutable" characteristics of intelligence or cognitive functioning). Dynamic assessment has been further elaborated in the work of Ann Brown, and John D. Bransford and in theories of multiple intelligences authored by Howard Gardner and Robert Sternberg.

J.P. Guilford's Structure of Intellect (1967) model of intelligence used three dimensions, which, when combined, yielded a total of 120 types of intelligence. It was popular in the 1970s and early 1980s, but faded owing to both practical problems and theoretical criticisms.

Alexander Luria's earlier work on neuropsychological processes led to the PASS theory (1997). It argued that only looking at one general factor was inadequate for researchers and clinicians who worked with learning disabilities, attention disorders, intellectual disability, and interventions for such disabilities. The PASS model covers four kinds of processes (planning process, attention/arousal process, simultaneous processing, and successive processing). The planning processes involve decision making, problem solving, and performing activities and require goal setting and self-monitoring.

The attention/arousal process involves selectively attending to a particular stimulus, ignoring distractions, and maintaining vigilance. Simultaneous processing involves the integration of stimuli into a group and requires the observation of relationships. Successive processing involves the integration of stimuli into serial order. The planning and attention/arousal components comes from structures located in the frontal lobe, and the simultaneous and successive processes come from structures located in the posterior region of the cortex. It has influenced some recent IQ tests, and been seen as a complement to the Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory described above.

Current tests

Normalized IQ distribution with mean 100 and standard deviation 15

There are a variety of individually administered IQ tests in use in the English-speaking world. The most commonly used individual IQ test series is the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) for adults and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) for school-age test-takers. Other commonly used individual IQ tests (some of which do not label their standard scores as "IQ" scores) include the current versions of the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales, Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, the Cognitive Assessment System, and the Differential Ability Scales.

There are various other IQ tests, including:

  1. Raven's Progressive Matrices (RPM)
  2. Cattell Culture Fair III (CFIT)
  3. Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales (RIAS)
  4. Thurstone's Primary Mental Abilities
  5. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (KBIT)
  6. Multidimensional Aptitude Battery II
  7. Das–Naglieri cognitive assessment system (CAS)
  8. Naglieri Nonverbal Ability Test (NNAT)
  9. Wide Range Intelligence Test (WRIT)

IQ scales are ordinally scaled. The raw score of the norming sample is usually (rank order) transformed to a normal distribution with mean 100 and standard deviation 15. While one standard deviation is 15 points, and two SDs are 30 points, and so on, this does not imply that mental ability is linearly related to IQ, such that IQ 50 would mean half the cognitive ability of IQ 100. In particular, IQ points are not percentage points.

Reliability and validity

Reliability

IQ scores can differ to some degree for the same person on different IQ tests, so a person does not always belong to the same IQ score range each time the person is tested. (IQ score table data and pupil pseudonyms adapted from description of KABC-II norming study cited in Kaufman (2009).)
Pupil KABC-II WISC-III WJ-III
A 90 95 111
B 125 110 105
C 100 93 101
D 116 127 118
E 93 105 93
F 106 105 105
G 95 100 90
H 112 113 103
I 104 96 97
J 101 99 86
K 81 78 75
L 116 124 102

Psychometricians generally regard IQ tests as having high statistical reliability. Reliability represents the measurement consistency of a test. A reliable test produces similar scores upon repetition. On aggregate, IQ tests exhibit high reliability, although test-takers may have varying scores when taking the same test on differing occasions, and may have varying scores when taking different IQ tests at the same age. Like all statistical quantities, any particular estimate of IQ has an associated standard error that measures uncertainty about the estimate. For modern tests, the confidence interval can be approximately 10 points and reported standard error of measurement can be as low as about three points. Reported standard error may be an underestimate, as it does not account for all sources of error.

Outside influences such as low motivation or high anxiety can occasionally lower a person's IQ test score. For individuals with very low scores, the 95% confidence interval may be greater than 40 points, potentially complicating the accuracy of diagnoses of intellectual disability. By the same token, high IQ scores are also significantly less reliable than those near to the population median. Reports of IQ scores much higher than 160 are considered dubious.

Validity as a measure of intelligence

Reliability and validity are very different concepts. While reliability reflects reproducibility, validity refers to whether the test measures what it purports to measure. While IQ tests are generally considered to measure some forms of intelligence, they may fail to serve as an accurate measure of broader definitions of human intelligence inclusive of, for example, creativity and social intelligence. For this reason, psychologist Wayne Weiten argues that their construct validity must be carefully qualified, and not be overstated. According to Weiten, "IQ tests are valid measures of the kind of intelligence necessary to do well in academic work. But if the purpose is to assess intelligence in a broader sense, the validity of IQ tests is questionable."

Some scientists have disputed the value of IQ as a measure of intelligence altogether. In The Mismeasure of Man (1981, expanded edition 1996), evolutionary biologist Stephen Jay Gould compared IQ testing with the now-discredited practice of determining intelligence via craniometry, arguing that both are based on the fallacy of reification, "our tendency to convert abstract concepts into entities". Gould's argument sparked a great deal of debate, and the book is listed as one of Discover Magazine's "25 Greatest Science Books of All Time".

Along these same lines, critics such as Keith Stanovich do not dispute the capacity of IQ test scores to predict some kinds of achievement, but argue that basing a concept of intelligence on IQ test scores alone neglects other important aspects of mental ability. Robert Sternberg, another significant critic of IQ as the main measure of human cognitive abilities, argued that reducing the concept of intelligence to the measure of g does not fully account for the different skills and knowledge types that produce success in human society.

Despite these objections, clinical psychologists generally regard IQ scores as having sufficient statistical validity for many clinical purposes.

Test bias or differential item functioning

Differential item functioning (DIF), sometimes referred to as measurement bias, is a phenomenon when participants from different groups (e.g. gender, race, disability) with the same latent abilities give different answers to specific questions on the same IQ test. DIF analysis measures such specific items on a test alongside measuring participants' latent abilities on other similar questions. A consistent different group response to a specific question among similar types of questions can indicate an effect of DIF. It does not count as differential item functioning if both groups have an equally valid chance of giving different responses to the same questions. Such bias can be a result of culture, educational level and other factors that are independent of group traits. DIF is only considered if test-takers from different groups with the same underlying latent ability level have a different chance of giving specific responses. Such questions are usually removed in order to make the test equally fair for both groups. Common techniques for analyzing DIF are item response theory (IRT) based methods, Mantel-Haenszel, and logistic regression.

A 2005 study found that "differential validity in prediction suggests that the WAIS-R test may contain cultural influences that reduce the validity of the WAIS-R as a measure of cognitive ability for Mexican American students," indicating a weaker positive correlation relative to sampled white students. Other recent studies have questioned the culture-fairness of IQ tests when used in South Africa. Standard intelligence tests, such as the Stanford–Binet, are often inappropriate for autistic children; the alternative of using developmental or adaptive skills measures are relatively poor measures of intelligence in autistic children, and may have resulted in incorrect claims that a majority of autistic children are of low intelligence.

Flynn effect

Main article: Flynn effect

Since the early 20th century, raw scores on IQ tests have increased in most parts of the world. When a new version of an IQ test is normed, the standard scoring is set so performance at the population median results in a score of IQ 100. The phenomenon of rising raw score performance means if test-takers are scored by a constant standard scoring rule, IQ test scores have been rising at an average rate of around three IQ points per decade. This phenomenon was named the Flynn effect in the book The Bell Curve after James R. Flynn, the author who did the most to bring this phenomenon to the attention of psychologists.

Researchers have been exploring the issue of whether the Flynn effect is equally strong on performance of all kinds of IQ test items, whether the effect may have ended in some developed nations, whether there are social subgroup differences in the effect, and what possible causes of the effect might be. A 2011 textbook, IQ and Human Intelligence, by N. J. Mackintosh, noted the Flynn effect demolishes the fears that IQ would be decreased. He also asks whether it represents a real increase in intelligence beyond IQ scores. A 2011 psychology textbook, lead authored by Harvard Psychologist Professor Daniel Schacter, noted that humans' inherited intelligence could be going down while acquired intelligence goes up.

Research has suggested that the Flynn effect has slowed or reversed course in some Western countries beginning in the late 20th century. The phenomenon has been termed the negative Flynn effect. A study of Norwegian military conscripts' test records found that IQ scores have been falling for generations born after the year 1975, and that the underlying cause of both initial increasing and subsequent falling trends appears to be environmental rather than genetic.

Age

Ronald S. Wilson is largely credited with the idea that IQ heritability rises with age. Researchers building on this phenomenon dubbed it "The Wilson Effect," named after the behavioral geneticist. A paper by Thomas J. Bouchard Jr., examining twin and adoption studies, including twins "reared apart," finds that IQ "reaches an asymptote at about 0.80 at 18–20 years of age and continuing at that level well into adulthood. In the aggregate, the studies also confirm that shared environmental influence decreases across age, approximating about 0.10 at 18–20 years of age and continuing at that level into adulthood." IQ can change to some degree over the course of childhood. In one longitudinal study, the mean IQ scores of tests at ages 17 and 18 were correlated at r = 0.86 with the mean scores of tests at ages five, six, and seven and at r = 0.96 with the mean scores of tests at ages 11, 12, and 13.

The current consensus is that fluid intelligence generally declines with age after early adulthood, while crystallized intelligence remains intact. However, the exact peak age of fluid intelligence or crystallized intelligence remains elusive. Cross-sectional studies usually show that especially fluid intelligence peaks at a relatively young age (often in the early adulthood) while longitudinal data mostly show that intelligence is stable until mid-adulthood or later. Subsequently, intelligence seems to decline slowly.

For decades, practitioners' handbooks and textbooks on IQ testing have reported IQ declines with age after the beginning of adulthood. However, later researchers pointed out this phenomenon is related to the Flynn effect and is in part a cohort effect rather than a true aging effect. A variety of studies of IQ and aging have been conducted since the norming of the first Wechsler Intelligence Scale drew attention to IQ differences in different age groups of adults. Both cohort effects (the birth year of the test-takers) and practice effects (test-takers taking the same form of IQ test more than once) must be controlled to gain accurate data. It is unclear whether any lifestyle intervention can preserve fluid intelligence into older ages.

Genetics and environment

Environmental and genetic factors play a role in determining IQ. Their relative importance has been the subject of much research and debate.

Heritability

See also: Heritability of IQ and Environment and intelligence

The general figure for the heritability of IQ, according to an American Psychological Association report, is 0.45 for children, and rises to around 0.75 for late adolescents and adults. Heritability measures for g factor in infancy are as low as 0.2, around 0.4 in middle childhood, and as high as 0.9 in adulthood. One proposed explanation is that people with different genes tend to reinforce the effects of those genes, for example by seeking out different environments.

Shared family environment

Family members have aspects of environments in common (for example, characteristics of the home). This shared family environment accounts for 0.25–0.35 of the variation in IQ in childhood. By late adolescence, it is quite low (zero in some studies). The effect for several other psychological traits is similar. These studies have not looked at the effects of extreme environments, such as in abusive families.

Non-shared family environment and environment outside the family

Although parents treat their children differently, such differential treatment explains only a small amount of nonshared environmental influence. One suggestion is that children react differently to the same environment because of different genes. More likely influences may be the impact of peers and other experiences outside the family.

Individual genes

A very large proportion of the over 17,000 human genes are thought to have an effect on the development and functionality of the brain. While a number of individual genes have been reported to be associated with IQ, none have a strong effect. Deary and colleagues (2009) reported that no finding of a strong single gene effect on IQ has been replicated. Recent findings of gene associations with normally varying intellectual differences in adults and children continue to show weak effects for any one gene.

A 2017 meta-analysis conducted on approximately 78,000 subjects identified 52 genes associated with intelligence. FNBP1L is reported to be the single gene most associated with both adult and child intelligence.

Gene-environment interaction

David Rowe reported an interaction of genetic effects with socioeconomic status, such that the heritability was high in high-SES families, but much lower in low-SES families. In the US, this has been replicated in infants, children, adolescents, and adults. Outside the US, studies show no link between heritability and SES. Some effects may even reverse sign outside the US.

Dickens and Flynn (2001) have argued that genes for high IQ initiate an environment-shaping feedback cycle, with genetic effects causing bright children to seek out more stimulating environments that then further increase their IQ. In Dickens' model, environment effects are modeled as decaying over time. In this model, the Flynn effect can be explained by an increase in environmental stimulation independent of it being sought out by individuals. The authors suggest that programs aiming to increase IQ would be most likely to produce long-term IQ gains if they enduringly raised children's drive to seek out cognitively demanding experiences.

Interventions

In general, educational interventions, as those described below, have shown short-term effects on IQ, but long-term follow-up is often missing. For example, in the US, very large intervention programs such as the Head Start Program have not produced lasting gains in IQ scores. Even when students improve their scores on standardized tests, they do not always improve their cognitive abilities, such as memory, attention and speed. More intensive, but much smaller projects, such as the Abecedarian Project, have reported lasting effects, often on socioeconomic status variables, rather than IQ.

Recent studies have shown that training in using one's working memory may increase IQ. A study on young adults published in April 2008 by a team from the Universities of Michigan and Bern supports the possibility of the transfer of fluid intelligence from specifically designed working memory training. Further research will be needed to determine nature, extent and duration of the proposed transfer. Among other questions, it remains to be seen whether the results extend to other kinds of fluid intelligence tests than the matrix test used in the study, and if so, whether, after training, fluid intelligence measures retain their correlation with educational and occupational achievement or if the value of fluid intelligence for predicting performance on other tasks changes. It is also unclear whether the training is durable for extended periods of time.

Music

Further information: Mozart effect

Musical training in childhood correlates with higher than average IQ. However, a study of 10,500 twins found no effects on IQ, suggesting that the correlation was caused by genetic confounders. A meta-analysis concluded that "Music training does not reliably enhance children and young adolescents' cognitive or academic skills, and that previous positive findings were probably due to confounding variables."

It is popularly thought that listening to classical music raises IQ. However, multiple attempted replications (e.g.) have shown that this is at best a short-term effect (lasting no longer than 10 to 15 minutes), and is not related to IQ-increase.

Brain anatomy

Main article: Neuroscience and intelligence

Several neurophysiological factors have been correlated with intelligence in humans, including the ratio of brain weight to body weight and the size, shape, and activity level of different parts of the brain. Specific features that may affect IQ include the size and shape of the frontal lobes, the amount of blood and chemical activity in the frontal lobes, the total amount of gray matter in the brain, the overall thickness of the cortex, and the glucose metabolic rate.

Health

Main articles: Impact of health on intelligence and Cognitive epidemiology

Health is important in understanding differences in IQ test scores and other measures of cognitive ability. Several factors can lead to significant cognitive impairment, particularly if they occur during pregnancy and childhood when the brain is growing and the blood–brain barrier is less effective. Such impairment may sometimes be permanent, or sometimes be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth.

Since about 2010, researchers such as Eppig, Hassel, and MacKenzie have found a very close and consistent link between IQ scores and infectious diseases, especially in the infant and preschool populations and the mothers of these children. They have postulated that fighting infectious diseases strains the child's metabolism and prevents full brain development. Hassel postulated that it is by far the most important factor in determining population IQ. However, they also found that subsequent factors such as good nutrition and regular quality schooling can offset early negative effects to some extent.

Developed nations have implemented several health policies regarding nutrients and toxins known to influence cognitive function. These include laws requiring fortification of certain food products and laws establishing safe levels of pollutants (e.g. lead, mercury, and organochlorides). Improvements in nutrition, and in public policy in general, have been implicated in IQ increases.

Cognitive epidemiology is a field of research that examines the associations between intelligence test scores and health. Researchers in the field argue that intelligence measured at an early age is an important predictor of later health and mortality differences.

Social correlations

School performance

The American Psychological Association's report Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns states that wherever it has been studied, children with high scores on tests of intelligence tend to learn more of what is taught in school than their lower-scoring peers. The correlation between IQ scores and grades is about .50. This means that the explained variance is 25%. Achieving good grades depends on many factors other than IQ, such as "persistence, interest in school, and willingness to study" (p. 81).

It has been found that the correlation of IQ scores with school performance depends on the IQ measurement used. For undergraduate students, the Verbal IQ as measured by WAIS-R has been found to correlate significantly (0.53) with the grade point average (GPA) of the last 60 hours (credits). In contrast, Performance IQ correlation with the same GPA was only 0.22 in the same study.

Some measures of educational aptitude correlate highly with IQ tests – for instance, Frey & Detterman (2004) reported a correlation of 0.82 between g (general intelligence factor) and SAT scores; another research found a correlation of 0.81 between g and GCSE scores, with the explained variance ranging "from 58.6% in Mathematics and 48% in English to 18.1% in Art and Design".

Job performance

According to Schmidt and Hunter, "for hiring employees without previous experience in the job the most valid predictor of future performance is general mental ability." The validity of IQ as a predictor of job performance is above zero for all work studied to date, but varies with the type of job and across different studies, ranging from 0.2 to 0.6. The correlations were higher when the unreliability of measurement methods was controlled for. While IQ is more strongly correlated with reasoning and less so with motor function, IQ-test scores predict performance ratings in all occupations.

That said, for highly qualified activities (research, management) low IQ scores are more likely to be a barrier to adequate performance, whereas for minimally-skilled activities, athletic strength (manual strength, speed, stamina, and coordination) is more likely to influence performance. The prevailing view among academics is that it is largely through the quicker acquisition of job-relevant knowledge that higher IQ mediates job performance. This view has been challenged by Byington & Felps (2010), who argued that "the current applications of IQ-reflective tests allow individuals with high IQ scores to receive greater access to developmental resources, enabling them to acquire additional capabilities over time, and ultimately perform their jobs better."

Newer studies find that the effects of IQ on job performance have been greatly overestimated. The current estimates of the correlation between job performance and IQ are about 0.23 correcting for unreliability and range restriction.

In establishing a causal direction to the link between IQ and work performance, longitudinal studies by Watkins and others suggest that IQ exerts a causal influence on future academic achievement, whereas academic achievement does not substantially influence future IQ scores. Treena Eileen Rohde and Lee Anne Thompson write that general cognitive ability, but not specific ability scores, predict academic achievement, with the exception that processing speed and spatial ability predict performance on the SAT math beyond the effect of general cognitive ability.

However, large-scale longitudinal studies indicate an increase in IQ translates into an increase in performance at all levels of IQ: i.e. ability and job performance are monotonically linked at all IQ levels.

Income

It has been suggested that "in economic terms it appears that the IQ score measures something with decreasing marginal value" and it "is important to have enough of it, but having lots and lots does not buy you that much".

The link from IQ to wealth is much less strong than that from IQ to job performance. Some studies indicate that IQ is unrelated to net worth. The American Psychological Association's 1995 report Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns stated that IQ scores accounted for about a quarter of the social status variance and one-sixth of the income variance. Statistical controls for parental SES eliminate about a quarter of this predictive power. Psychometric intelligence appears as only one of a great many factors that influence social outcomes. Charles Murray (1998) showed a more substantial effect of IQ on income independent of family background. In a meta-analysis, Strenze (2006) reviewed much of the literature and estimated the correlation between IQ and income to be about 0.23.

Some studies assert that IQ only accounts for (explains) a sixth of the variation in income because many studies are based on young adults, many of whom have not yet reached their peak earning capacity, or even their education. On pg 568 of The g Factor, Arthur Jensen says that although the correlation between IQ and income averages a moderate 0.4 (one-sixth or 16% of the variance), the relationship increases with age, and peaks at middle age when people have reached their maximum career potential. In the book, A Question of Intelligence, Daniel Seligman cites an IQ income correlation of 0.5 (25% of the variance).

A 2002 study further examined the impact of non-IQ factors on income and concluded that an individual's location, inherited wealth, race, and schooling are more important as factors in determining income than IQ.

Crime

The American Psychological Association's 1995 report Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns stated that the correlation between IQ and crime was −0.2. This association is generally regarded as small and prone to disappearance or a substantial reduction after controlling for the proper covariates, being much smaller than typical sociological correlates. It was −0.19 between IQ scores and the number of juvenile offenses in a large Danish sample; with social class controlled for, the correlation dropped to −0.17. A correlation of 0.20 means that the explained variance accounts for 4% of the total variance. The causal links between psychometric ability and social outcomes may be indirect. Children with poor scholastic performance may feel alienated. Consequently, they may be more likely to engage in delinquent behavior, compared to other children who do well.

In his book The g Factor (1998), Arthur Jensen cited data which showed that, regardless of race, people with IQs between 70 and 90 have higher crime rates than people with IQs below or above this range, with the peak range being between 80 and 90.

The 2009 Handbook of Crime Correlates stated that reviews have found that around eight IQ points, or 0.5 SD, separate criminals from the general population, especially for persistent serious offenders. It has been suggested that this simply reflects that "only dumb ones get caught" but there is similarly a negative relation between IQ and self-reported offending. That children with conduct disorder have lower IQ than their peers "strongly argues" for the theory.

A study of the relationship between US county-level IQ and US county-level crime rates found that higher average IQs were very weakly associated with lower levels of property crime, burglary, larceny rate, motor vehicle theft, violent crime, robbery, and aggravated assault. These results were "not confounded by a measure of concentrated disadvantage that captures the effects of race, poverty, and other social disadvantages of the county." However, this study is limited in that it extrapolated Add Health estimates to the respondent's counties, and as the dataset was not designed to be representative on the state or county level, it may not be generalizable.

It has also been shown that the effect of IQ is heavily dependent on socioeconomic status and that it cannot be easily controlled away, with many methodological considerations being at play. Indeed, there is evidence that the small relationship is mediated by well-being, substance abuse, and other confounding factors that prohibit simple causal interpretation. A recent meta-analysis has shown that the relationship is only observed in higher risk populations such as those in poverty without direct effect, but without any causal interpretation. A nationally representative longitudinal study has shown that this relationship is entirely mediated by school performance.

Health and mortality

Multiple studies conducted in Scotland have found that higher IQs in early life are associated with lower mortality and morbidity rates later in life.

Other accomplishments

Average adult combined IQs associated with real-life accomplishments by various tests:
Accomplishment IQ Test/study Year
MDs, JDs, and PhDs 125 WAIS-R 1987
College graduates 112 KAIT 2000
K-BIT 1992
115 WAIS-R
1–3 years of college 104 KAIT
K-BIT
105–110 WAIS-R
Clerical and sales workers 100–105
High school graduates, skilled workers (e.g., electricians, cabinetmakers) 100 KAIT
WAIS-R
97 K-BIT
1–3 years of high school (completed 9–11 years of school) 94 KAIT
90 K-BIT
95 WAIS-R
Semi-skilled workers (e.g. truck drivers, factory workers) 90–95
Elementary school graduates (completed eighth grade) 90
Elementary school dropouts (completed 0–7 years of school) 80–85
Have 50/50 chance of reaching high school 75
Average IQ of various occupational groups:
Accomplishment IQ Test/study Year
Professional and technical 112
Managers and administrators 104
Clerical workers, sales workers, skilled workers, craftsmen, and foremen 101
Semi-skilled workers (operatives, service workers, including private household) 92
Unskilled workers 87
Type of work that can be accomplished:
Accomplishment IQ Test/study Year
Adults can harvest vegetables, repair furniture 60
Adults can do domestic work 50

There is considerable variation within and overlap among these categories. People with high IQs are found at all levels of education and occupational categories. The biggest difference occurs for low IQs with only an occasional college graduate or professional scoring below 90.

Group differences

Among the most controversial issues related to the study of intelligence is the observation that IQ scores vary on average between ethnic and racial groups, though these differences have fluctuated and in many cases steadily decreased over time. While there is little scholarly debate about the continued existence of some of these differences, the current scientific consensus is that they stem from environmental rather than genetic causes. The existence of differences in IQ between the sexes has been debated, and largely depends on which tests are performed.

Race

Main article: Race and intelligence

While the concept of "race" is a social construct, discussions of a purported relationship between race and intelligence, as well as claims of genetic differences in intelligence along racial lines, have appeared in both popular science and academic research since the modern concept of race was first introduced.

Genetics do not explain differences in IQ test performance between racial or ethnic groups. Despite the tremendous amount of research done on the topic, no scientific evidence has emerged that the average IQ scores of different population groups can be attributed to genetic differences between those groups. In recent decades, as understanding of human genetics has advanced, claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have been broadly rejected by scientists on both theoretical and empirical grounds.

Growing evidence indicates that environmental factors, not genetic ones, explain the racial IQ gap. A 1996 task force investigation on intelligence sponsored by the American Psychological Association concluded that "because ethnic differences in intelligence reflect complex patterns, no overall generalization about them is appropriate," with environmental factors the most plausible reason for the shrinking gap. A systematic analysis by William Dickens and James Flynn (2006) showed the gap between black and white Americans to have closed dramatically during the period between 1972 and 2002, suggesting that, in their words, the "constancy of the Black–White IQ gap is a myth". The effects of stereotype threat have been proposed as an explanation for differences in IQ test performance between racial groups, as have issues related to cultural difference and access to education.

Despite the strong scientific consensus to the contrary, fringe figures continue to promote scientific racism about group-level IQ averages in pseudo-scholarship and popular culture.

Sex

Main article: Sex differences in intelligence

With the advent of the concept of g or general intelligence, many researchers have found that there are no significant sex differences in average IQ, though ability in particular types of intelligence does vary. Thus, while some test batteries show slightly greater intelligence in males, others show greater intelligence in females. In particular, studies have shown female subjects performing better on tasks related to verbal ability, and males performing better on tasks related to rotation of objects in space, often categorized as spatial ability. These differences remain, as Hunt (2011) observes, "even though men and women are essentially equal in general intelligence".

Some research indicates that male advantages on some cognitive tests are minimized when controlling for socioeconomic factors. Other research has concluded that there is slightly larger variability in male scores in certain areas compared to female scores, which results in slightly more males than females in the top and bottom of the IQ distribution.

The existence of differences between male and female performance on math-related tests is contested, and a meta-analysis focusing on average gender differences in math performance found nearly identical performance for boys and girls. Currently, most IQ tests, including popular batteries such as the WAIS and the WISC-R, are constructed so that there are no overall score differences between females and males.

Public policy

Main article: Intelligence and public policy

In the United States, certain public policies and laws regarding military service, education, public benefits, capital punishment, and employment incorporate an individual's IQ into their decisions. However, in the case of Griggs v. Duke Power Co. in 1971, for the purpose of minimizing employment practices that disparately impacted racial minorities, the U.S. Supreme Court banned the use of IQ tests in employment, except when linked to job performance via a job analysis. Internationally, certain public policies, such as improving nutrition and prohibiting neurotoxins, have as one of their goals raising, or preventing a decline in, intelligence.

A diagnosis of intellectual disability is in part based on the results of IQ testing. Borderline intellectual functioning is the categorization of individuals of below-average cognitive ability (an IQ of 71–85), although not as low as those with an intellectual disability (70 or below).

In the United Kingdom, the eleven plus exam which incorporated an intelligence test has been used from 1945 to decide, at eleven years of age, which type of school a child should go to. They have been much less used since the widespread introduction of comprehensive schools.

Classification

Main article: IQ classification
Physicist Stephen Hawking. When asked his IQ, he replied: "I have no idea. People who boast about their IQ are losers."

IQ classification is the practice used by IQ test publishers for designating IQ score ranges into various categories with labels such as "superior" or "average". IQ classification was preceded historically by attempts to classify human beings by general ability based on other forms of behavioral observation. Those other forms of behavioral observation are still important for validating classifications based on IQ tests.

High-IQ societies

Main article: High-IQ society

There are social organizations, some international, which limit membership to people who have scores as high as or higher than the 98th percentile (two standard deviations above the mean) on some IQ test or equivalent. Mensa International is perhaps the best known of these. The largest 99.9th percentile (three standard deviations above the mean) society is the Triple Nine Society.

See also

Citations

  1. Braaten, Ellen B.; Norman, Dennis (1 November 2006). "Intelligence (IQ) Testing". Pediatrics in Review. 27 (11): 403–408. doi:10.1542/pir.27-11-403. ISSN 0191-9601. PMID 17079505. Retrieved 22 January 2020.
  2. "intelligence quotient (IQ)". Glossary of Important Assessment and Measurement Terms. Philadelphia, PA: National Council on Measurement in Education. 2016. Archived from the original on 22 July 2017. Retrieved 1 July 2017.
  3. ^ Gottfredson 2009, pp. 31–32
  4. Neisser, Ulrich (1997). "Rising Scores on Intelligence Tests". American Scientist. 85 (5): 440–447. Bibcode:1997AmSci..85..440N. Archived from the original on 4 November 2016. Retrieved 1 December 2017.
  5. Hunt 2011, p. 5 "As mental testing expanded to the evaluation of adolescents and adults, however, there was a need for a measure of intelligence that did not depend upon mental age. Accordingly the intelligence quotient (IQ) was developed. ... The narrow definition of IQ is a score on an intelligence test ... where 'average' intelligence, that is the median level of performance on an intelligence test, receives a score of 100, and other scores are assigned so that the scores are distributed normally about 100, with a standard deviation of 15. Some of the implications are that: 1. Approximately two-thirds of all scores lie between 85 and 115. 2. Five percent (1/20) of all scores are above 125, and one percent (1/100) are above 135. Similarly, five percent are below 75 and one percent below 65."
  6. Haier, Richard (28 December 2016). The Neuroscience of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. pp. 18–19. ISBN 9781107461437.
  7. Cusick, Sarah E.; Georgieff, Michael K. (1 August 2017). "The Role of Nutrition in Brain Development: The Golden Opportunity of the 'First 1000 Days'". The Journal of Pediatrics. 175: 16–21. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.05.013. ISSN 0022-3476. PMC 4981537. PMID 27266965.
  8. Saloojee, Haroon; Pettifor, John M (15 December 2001). "Iron deficiency and impaired child development". British Medical Journal. 323 (7326): 1377–1378. doi:10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377. ISSN 0959-8138. PMC 1121846. PMID 11744547.
  9. Qian, Ming; Wang, Dong; Watkins, William E.; Gebski, Val; Yan, Yu Qin; Li, Mu; Chen, Zu Pei (2005). "The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China". Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 14 (1): 32–42. ISSN 0964-7058. PMID 15734706.
  10. Poh, Bee Koon; Lee, Shoo Thien; Yeo, Giin Shang; Tang, Kean Choon; Noor Afifah, Ab Rahim; Siti Hanisa, Awal; Parikh, Panam; Wong, Jyh Eiin; Ng, Alvin Lai Oon; SEANUTS Study Group (13 June 2019). "Low socioeconomic status and severe obesity are linked to poor cognitive performance in Malaysian children". BMC Public Health. 19 (Suppl 4): 541. doi:10.1186/s12889-019-6856-4. ISSN 1471-2458. PMC 6565598. PMID 31196019.
  11. Galván, Marcos; Uauy, Ricardo; Corvalán, Camila; López-Rodríguez, Guadalupe; Kain, Juliana (September 2013). "Determinants of cognitive development of low SES children in Chile: a post-transitional country with rising childhood obesity rates". Maternal and Child Health Journal. 17 (7): 1243–1251. doi:10.1007/s10995-012-1121-9. ISSN 1573-6628. PMID 22915146. S2CID 19767926.
  12. Markus Jokela; G. David Batty; Ian J. Deary; Catharine R. Gale; Mika Kivimäki (2009). "Low Childhood IQ and Early Adult Mortality: The Role of Explanatory Factors in the 1958 British Birth Cohort". Pediatrics. 124 (3): e380 – e388. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0334. PMID 19706576. S2CID 25256969.
  13. ^ Deary & Batty 2007.
  14. ^ Neisser et al. 1995.
  15. Ronfani, Luca; Vecchi Brumatti, Liza; Mariuz, Marika; Tognin, Veronica (2015). "The Complex Interaction between Home Environment, Socioeconomic Status, Maternal IQ and Early Child Neurocognitive Development: A Multivariate Analysis of Data Collected in a Newborn Cohort Study". PLOS ONE. 10 (5): e0127052. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1027052R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0127052. PMC 4440732. PMID 25996934.
  16. Johnson, Wendy; Turkheimer, Eric; Gottesman, Irving I.; Bouchard, Thomas J. (August 2009). "Beyond Heritability". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 18 (4): 217–220. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01639.x. PMC 2899491. PMID 20625474.
  17. Turkheimer 2008.
  18. Devlin, B.; Daniels, Michael; Roeder, Kathryn (1997). "The heritability of IQ". Nature. 388 (6641): 468–71. Bibcode:1997Natur.388..468D. doi:10.1038/41319. PMID 9242404. S2CID 4313884.
  19. ^ Schmidt, Frank L.; Hunter, John E. (1998). "The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 124 (2): 262–74. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.172.1733. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262. S2CID 16429503. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 June 2014. Retrieved 25 October 2017.
  20. ^ Strenze, Tarmo (September 2007). "Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research". Intelligence. 35 (5): 401–426. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.004. The correlation with income is considerably lower, perhaps even disappointingly low, being about the average of the previous meta-analytic estimates (.15 by Bowles et al., 2001; and .27 by Ng et al., 2005). But...other predictors, studied in this paper, are not doing any better in predicting income, which demonstrates that financial success is difficult to predict by any variable. This assertion is further corroborated by the meta-analysis of Ng et al. (2005) where the best predictor of salary was educational level with a correlation of only .29. It should also be noted that the correlation of .23 is about the size of the average meta-analytic result in psychology(Hemphill, 2003) and cannot, therefore, be treated as insignificant.
  21. ^ Winston, Andrew S. (29 May 2020). "Scientific Racism and North American Psychology". Oxford Research Encyclopedias: Psychology. The use of psychological concepts and data to promote ideas of an enduring racial hierarchy dates from the late 1800s and has continued to the present. The history of scientific racism in psychology is intertwined with broader debates, anxieties, and political issues in American society. With the rise of intelligence testing, joined with ideas of eugenic progress and dysgenic reproduction, psychological concepts and data came to play an important role in naturalizing racial inequality. Although racial comparisons were not the primary concern of most early mental testing, results were employed to justify beliefs regarding Black "educability" and the dangers of Southern and Eastern European immigration.
  22. Newitz, Annalee (4 June 2024). "Chapter 4". Stories Are Weapons: Psychological Warfare and the American Mind. W. W. Norton & Company.
  23. ^ Bird, Kevin; Jackson, John P.; Winston, Andrew S. (2024). "Confronting Scientific Racism in Psychology: Lessons from Evolutionary Biology and Genetics". American Psychologist. 79 (4): 497–508. doi:10.1037/amp0001228. PMID 39037836. Recent articles claim that the folk categories of race are genetically meaningful divisions, and that evolved genetic differences among races and nations are important for explaining immutable differences in cognitive ability, educational attainment, crime, sexual behavior, and wealth; all claims that are opposed by a strong scientific consensus to the contrary.
  24. ^ Panofsky, Aaron; Dasgupta, Kushan; Iturriaga, Nicole (2021). "How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 175 (2): 387–398. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24150. ISSN 0002-9483. PMC 9909835. PMID 32986847. he claims that genetics defines racial groups and makes them different, that IQ and cultural differences among racial groups are caused by genes, and that racial inequalities within and between nations are the inevitable outcome of long evolutionary processes are neither new nor supported by science (either old or new).
  25. Terman 1916, p. 79 "What do the above IQ's imply in such terms as feeble-mindedness, border-line intelligence, dullness, normality, superior intelligence, genius, etc.? When we use these terms two facts must be born in mind: (1) That the boundary lines between such groups are absolutely arbitrary, a matter of definition only; and (2) that the individuals comprising one of the groups do not make up a homogeneous type."
  26. Wechsler 1939, p. 37 "The earliest classifications of intelligence were very rough ones. To a large extent they were practical attempts to define various patterns of behavior in medical-legal terms."
  27. Bulmer, M (1999). "The development of Francis Galton's ideas on the mechanism of heredity". Journal of the History of Biology. 32 (3): 263–292. doi:10.1023/a:1004608217247. PMID 11624207. S2CID 10451997.
  28. Cowan, R. S. (1972). "Francis Galton's contribution to genetics". Journal of the History of Biology. 5 (2): 389–412. doi:10.1007/bf00346665. PMID 11610126. S2CID 30206332.
  29. Burbridge, D (2001). "Francis Galton on twins, heredity and social class". British Journal for the History of Science. 34 (3): 323–340. doi:10.1017/s0007087401004332. PMID 11700679.
  30. Fancher, R. E. (1983). "Biographical origins of Francis Galton's psychology". Isis. 74 (2): 227–233. doi:10.1086/353245. PMID 6347965. S2CID 40565053.
  31. Kaufman 2009, p. 21 "Galton's so-called intelligence test was misnamed."
  32. Gillham, Nicholas W. (2001). "Sir Francis Galton and the birth of eugenics". Annual Review of Genetics. 35 (1): 83–101. doi:10.1146/annurev.genet.35.102401.090055. PMID 11700278.
  33. ^ Nicolas, S.; Andrieu, B.; Croizet, J.-C.; Sanitioso, R. B.; Burman, J. T. (2013). "Sick? Or slow? On the origins of intelligence as a psychological object". Intelligence. 41 (5): 699–711. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.08.006. (This is an open access article, made freely available by Elsevier.)
  34. ^ Kaufman 2009
  35. Terman et al. 1915.
  36. Wallin, J. E. W. (1911). "The new clinical psychology and the psycho-clinicist". Journal of Educational Psychology. 2 (3): 121–32. doi:10.1037/h0075544.
  37. Richardson, John T. E. (2003). "Howard Andrew Knox and the origins of performance testing on Ellis Island, 1912-1916". History of Psychology. 6 (2): 143–70. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.6.2.143. PMID 12822554.
  38. Stern 1914, pp. 70–84 (1914 English translation), pp. 48–58 (1912 original German edition).
  39. Deary 2001, pp. 6–12.
  40. ^ Gould 1996
  41. Kennedy, Carrie H.; McNeil, Jeffrey A. (2006). "A history of military psychology". In Kennedy, Carrie H.; Zillmer, Eric (eds.). Military Psychology: Clinical and Operational Applications. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 1–17. ISBN 978-1-57230-724-7.
  42. Katzell, Raymond A.; Austin, James T. (1992). "From then to now: The development of industrial-organizational psychology in the United States". Journal of Applied Psychology. 77 (6): 803–35. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.77.6.803.
  43. Kevles, D. J. (1968). "Testing the Army's Intelligence: Psychologists and the Military in World War I". The Journal of American History. 55 (3): 565–81. doi:10.2307/1891014. JSTOR 1891014.
  44. Spektorowski, Alberto; Ireni-Saban, Liza (2013). Politics of Eugenics: Productionism, Population, and National Welfare. London: Routledge. p. 24. ISBN 978-0-203-74023-1. Retrieved 16 January 2017. As an applied science, thus, the practice of eugenics referred to everything from prenatal care for mothers to forced sterilization and euthanasia. Galton divided the practice of eugenics into two types—positive and negative—both aimed at improving the human race through selective breeding.
  45. "Eugenics". Unified Medical Language System (Psychological Index Terms). National Library of Medicine. 26 September 2010.
  46. Galton, Francis (July 1904). "Eugenics: Its Definition, Scope, and Aims". The American Journal of Sociology. X (1): 82, 1st paragraph. Bibcode:1904Natur..70...82.. doi:10.1038/070082a0. Archived from the original on 3 November 2007. Retrieved 27 December 2010. Eugenics is the science which deals with all influences that improve the inborn qualities of a race; also with those that develop them to the utmost advantage.
  47. Susan Currell; Christina Cogdell (2006). Popular Eugenics: National Efficiency and American Mass Culture in the 1930s. Ohio University Press. pp. 2–3. ISBN 978-0-8214-1691-4.
  48. "Eugenics and Economics in the Progressive Era" (PDF).
  49. "Origins of Eugenics: From Sir Francis Galton to Virginia's Racial Integrity Act of 1924". University of Virginia: Historical Collections at the Claude Moore Health Sciences Library. Retrieved 25 October 2019.
  50. Norrgard, K. (2008). "Human testing, the eugenics movement, and IRBs". Nature Education. 1: 170.
  51. Galton, Francis (1869). "Hereditary Genius" (PDF). p. 64. Retrieved 1 October 2019.
  52. ^ "The birth of American intelligence testing". Retrieved 11 November 2017.
  53. "America's Hidden History: The Eugenics Movement | Learn Science at Scitable". www.nature.com. Retrieved 11 November 2017.
  54. "Social Origins of Eugenics". www.eugenicsarchive.org. Retrieved 11 November 2017.
  55. ^ "The Horrifying American Roots of Nazi Eugenics". hnn.us. September 2003. Retrieved 11 November 2017.
  56. Vizcarrondo, Felipe E. (August 2014). "Human Enhancement: The New Eugenics". The Linacre Quarterly. 81 (3): 239–243. doi:10.1179/2050854914Y.0000000021. PMC 4135459. PMID 25249705.
  57. Regalado, Antonio. "Eugenics 2.0: We're at the Dawn of Choosing Embryos by Health, Height, and More". Technology Review. Retrieved 20 November 2019.
  58. LeMieux, Julianna (1 April 2019). "Polygenic Risk Scores and Genomic Prediction: Q&A with Stephen Hsu". Genetic Engineering and Biotechnology News. Retrieved 20 November 2019.
  59. Lubinski, David (2004). "Introduction to the Special Section on Cognitive Abilities: 100 Years After Spearman's (1904) "'General Intelligence,' Objectively Determined and Measured"". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 86 (1): 96–111. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.96. PMID 14717630. S2CID 6024297.
  60. Carroll 1993, p. .
  61. Mindes, Gayle (2003). Assessing Young Children. Merrill/Prentice Hall. p. 158. ISBN 9780130929082.
  62. Haywood, H. Carl; Lidz, Carol S. (2006). Dynamic Assessment in Practice: Clinical and Educational Applications. Cambridge University Press. p. 1. ISBN 9781139462075.
  63. Vygotsky, L.S. (1934). "The Problem of Age". The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky, Volume 5 (published 1998). pp. 187–205.
  64. Chaiklin, S. (2003). "The Zone of Proximal Development in Vygotsky's analysis of learning and instruction". In Kozulin, A.; Gindis, B.; Ageyev, V.; Miller, S. (eds.). Vygotsky's educational theory and practice in cultural context. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 39–64.
  65. Zaretskii, V.K. (November–December 2009). "The Zone of Proximal Development What Vygotsky Did Not Have Time to Write". Journal of Russian and East European Psychology. 47 (6): 70–93. doi:10.2753/RPO1061-0405470604. S2CID 146894219.
  66. Sternberg, R.S.; Grigorenko, E.L. (2001). "All testing is dynamic testing". Issues in Education. 7 (2): 137–170.
  67. Sternberg, R.J. & Grigorenko, E.L. (2002). Dynamic testing: The nature and measurement of learning potential. Cambridge: University of Cambridge
  68. Haywood & Lidz 2006, p. .
  69. Feuerstein, R., Feuerstein, S., Falik, L & Rand, Y. (1979; 2002). Dynamic assessments of cognitive modifiability. ICELP Press, Jerusalem: Israel
  70. Dodge, Kenneth A. (2006). Foreword. Dynamic Assessment in Practice: Clinical And Educational Applications. By Haywood, H. Carl; Lidz, Carol S. Cambridge University Press. pp. xiii–xv.
  71. Kozulin, A. (2014). "Dynamic assessment in search of its identity". In Yasnitsky, A.; van der Veer, R.; Ferrari, M. (eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 126–147.
  72. Das, J.P.; Kirby, J.; Jarman, R.F. (1975). "Simultaneous and successive synthesis: An alternative model for cognitive abilities". Psychological Bulletin. 82: 87–103. doi:10.1037/h0076163.
  73. Das, J.P. (2000). "A better look at intelligence". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 11: 28–33. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00162. S2CID 146129242.
  74. Naglieri, J.A.; Das, J.P. (2002). "Planning, attention, simultaneous, and successive cognitive processes as a model for assessment". School Psychology Review. 19 (4): 423–442. doi:10.1080/02796015.1990.12087349.
  75. Urbina 2011, Table 2.1 Major Examples of Current Intelligence Tests
  76. Flanagan & Harrison 2012, chapters 8–13, 15–16 (discussing Wechsler, Stanford–Binet, Kaufman, Woodcock–Johnson, DAS, CAS, and RIAS tests)
  77. Stanek, Kevin C.; Ones, Deniz S. (2018), "Taxonomies and Compendia of Cognitive Ability and Personality Constructs and Measures Relevant to Industrial, Work and Organizational Psychology", The SAGE Handbook of Industrial, Work and Organizational Psychology: Personnel Psychology and Employee Performance, 1 Oliver's Yard, 55 City Road London EC1Y 1SP: SAGE Publications Ltd, pp. 366–407, doi:10.4135/9781473914940.n14, ISBN 978-1-4462-0721-5, retrieved 8 January 2024{{citation}}: CS1 maint: location (link)
  78. "Primary Mental Abilities Test | psychological test". Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved 26 November 2015.
  79. "Defining and Measuring Psychological Attributes". homepages.rpi.edu. Archived from the original on 15 October 2018. Retrieved 26 November 2015.
  80. Bain, Sherry K.; Jaspers, Kathryn E. (1 April 2010). "Test Review: Review of Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition. Bloomington, MN: Pearson, Inc". Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment. 28 (2): 167–174. doi:10.1177/0734282909348217. ISSN 0734-2829. S2CID 143961429.
  81. Mussen, Paul Henry (1973). Psychology: An Introduction. Lexington, MA: Heath. p. 363. ISBN 978-0-669-61382-7. The I.Q. is essentially a rank; there are no true "units" of intellectual ability.
  82. Truch, Steve (1993). The WISC-III Companion: A Guide to Interpretation and Educational Intervention. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. p. 35. ISBN 978-0-89079-585-9. An IQ score is not an equal-interval score, as is evident in Table A.4 in the WISC-III manual.
  83. Bartholomew, David J. (2004). Measuring Intelligence: Facts and Fallacies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 50. ISBN 978-0-521-54478-8. When we come to quantities like IQ or g, as we are presently able to measure them, we shall see later that we have an even lower level of measurement—an ordinal level. This means that the numbers we assign to individuals can only be used to rank them—the number tells us where the individual comes in the rank order and nothing else.
  84. Mackintosh 1998, pp. 30–31 "In the jargon of psychological measurement theory, IQ is an ordinal scale, where we are simply rank-ordering people. ... It is not even appropriate to claim that the 10-point difference between IQ scores of 110 and 100 is the same as the 10-point difference between IQs of 160 and 150"
  85. Stevens, S. S. (1946). "On the Theory of Scales of Measurement". Science. 103 (2684): 677–680. Bibcode:1946Sci...103..677S. doi:10.1126/science.103.2684.677. PMID 17750512. S2CID 4667599.
  86. Kaufman 2009, Figure 5.1 IQs earned by preadolescents (ages 12–13) who were given three different IQ tests in the early 2000s
  87. Kaufman 2013, Figure 3.1 "Source: Kaufman (2009). Adapted with permission."
  88. Mackintosh 2011, p. 169 "after the age of 8–10, IQ scores remain relatively stable: the correlation between IQ scores from age 8 to 18 and IQ at age 40 is over 0.70."
  89. ^ Weiten W (2016). Psychology: Themes and Variations. Cengage Learning. p. 281. ISBN 978-1305856127.
  90. "WISC-V Interpretive Report Sample" (PDF). Pearson. p. 18. Archived from the original (PDF) on 22 April 2022. Retrieved 29 September 2020.
  91. Kaufman, Alan S.; Raiford, Susan Engi; Coalson, Diane L. (2016). Intelligent testing with the WISC-V. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. pp. 683–702. ISBN 978-1-118-58923-6. Reliability estimates in Table 4.1 and standard errors of measurement in Table 4.4 should be considered best-case estimates because they do not consider other major sources of error, such as transient error, administration error, or scoring error (Hanna, Bradley, & Holen, 1981), which influence test scores in clinical assessments. Another factor that must be considered is the extent to which subtest scores reflect portions of true score variance due to a hierarchical general intelligence factor and variance due to specific group factors because these sources of true score variance are conflated.
  92. Whitaker, Simon (April 2010). "Error in the estimation of intellectual ability in the low range using the WISC-IV and WAIS-III". Personality and Individual Differences. 48 (5): 517–521. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.017. Retrieved 22 January 2020.
  93. Lohman & Foley Nicpon 2012, p. . "The concerns associated with SEMs are actually substantially worse for scores at the extremes of the distribution, especially when scores approach the maximum possible on a test ... when students answer most of the items correctly. In these cases, errors of measurement for scale scores will increase substantially at the extremes of the distribution. Commonly the SEM is from two to four times larger for very high scores than for scores near the mean (Lord, 1980)."
  94. Urbina 2011, p. 20 " is just one of the reasons to be suspicious of reported IQ scores much higher than 160"
  95. Gould 1981, p. 24. Gould 1996, p. 56.
  96. Kaplan, Jonathan Michael; Pigliucci, Massimo; Banta, Joshua Alexander (2015). "Gould on Morton, Redux: What can the debate reveal about the limits of data?" (PDF). Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences. 30: 1–10.
  97. Weisberg, Michael; Paul, Diane B. (19 April 2016). "Morton, Gould, and Bias: A Comment on "The Mismeasure of Science"". PLOS Biology. 14 (4). e1002444. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002444. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4836680. PMID 27092558.
  98. "25 Greatest Science Books of All Time". Discover. 7 December 2006.
  99. Brooks, David (14 September 2007). "The Waning of I.Q.". The New York Times.
  100. Sternberg, Robert J., and Richard K. Wagner. "The g-ocentric view of intelligence and job performance is wrong." Current directions in psychological science (1993): 1–5.
  101. Anastasi & Urbina 1997, pp. 326–327.
  102. Embretson, S. E., Reise, S. P. (2000).Item Response Theory for Psychologists. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  103. ^ Zumbo, B.D. (2007). "Three generations of differential item functioning (DIF) analyses: Considering where it has been, where it is now, and where it is going". Language Assessment Quarterly. 4 (2): 223–233. doi:10.1080/15434300701375832. S2CID 17426415.
  104. Verney, SP; Granholm, E; Marshall, SP; Malcarne, VL; Saccuzzo, DP (2005). "Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment: Information Processing and Psychophysiological Approaches". Assessment. 12 (3): 303–19. doi:10.1177/1073191105276674. PMID 16123251. S2CID 31024437.
  105. Shuttleworth-Edwards, Ann; Kemp, Ryan; Rust, Annegret; Muirhead, Joanne; Hartman, Nigel; Radloff, Sarah (2004). "Cross-cultural Effects on IQ Test Performance: AReview and Preliminary Normative Indications on WAIS-III Test Performance". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 26 (7): 903–20. doi:10.1080/13803390490510824. PMID 15742541. S2CID 16060622.
  106. Cronshaw, Steven F.; Hamilton, Leah K.; Onyura, Betty R.; Winston, Andrew S. (2006). "Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons (2004)". International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 14 (3): 278–87. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00346.x. S2CID 91179275.
  107. Edelson, M. G. (2006). "Are the Majority of Children With Autism Mentally Retarded?: A Systematic Evaluation of the Data". Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. 21 (2): 66–83. doi:10.1177/10883576060210020301. S2CID 145809356.
  108. Ulric Neisser; James R. Flynn; Carmi Schooler; Patricia M. Greenfield; Wendy M. Williams; Marian Sigman; Shannon E. Whaley; Reynaldo Martorell; et al. (1998). Neisser, Ulric (ed.). The Rising Curve: Long-Term Gains in IQ and Related Measures. APA Science Volume Series. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1-55798-503-3.
  109. Mackintosh 1998, p. .
  110. ^ Flynn 2009, p. .
  111. Flynn, James R. (1984). "The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 1932 to 1978". Psychological Bulletin. 95 (1): 29–51. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.29. S2CID 51999517.
  112. Flynn, James R. (1987). "Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure". Psychological Bulletin. 101 (2): 171–91. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171.
  113. Zhou, Xiaobin; Grégoire, Jacques; Zhu, Jianjin (2010). "The Flynn Effect and the Wechsler Scales". In Weiss, Lawrence G.; Saklofske, Donald H.; Coalson, Diane; Raiford, Susan (eds.). WAIS-IV Clinical Use and Interpretation: Scientist-Practitioner Perspectives. Practical Resources for the Mental Health Professional. Amsterdam: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-375035-8.
  114. Mackintosh 2011, pp. 25–27.
  115. Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Wegner, Daniel M. (2011). Psychology. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. p. 384. ISBN 978-0230579835.
  116. ^ Bratsberg, Bernt; Rogeberg, Ole (26 June 2018). "Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115 (26): 6674–6678. Bibcode:2018PNAS..115.6674B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1718793115. PMC 6042097. PMID 29891660.
  117. "Ronald S. Wilson (1933–1986)". Behavior Genetics. 17 (3): 211–217. May 1987. doi:10.1007/BF01065501. ISSN 0001-8244.
  118. ^ Bouchard, Thomas J. (October 2013). "The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 16 (5): 923–930. doi:10.1017/thg.2013.54. ISSN 1832-4274.
  119. Kaufman 2009, pp. 220–222.
  120. ^ Kaufman 2009, p. , "Chapter 8".
  121. Desjardins, Richard; Warnke, Arne Jonas (2012). "Ageing and Skills". OECD Education Working Papers. doi:10.1787/5k9csvw87ckh-en. hdl:10419/57089.
  122. Tucker-Drob, Elliot M; Briley, Daniel A (2014), "Continuity of Genetic and Environmental Influences on Cognition across the Life Span: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Twin and Adoption Studies", Psychological Bulletin, 140 (4): 949–979, doi:10.1037/a0035893, PMC 4069230, PMID 24611582
  123. Bouchard, Thomas J. (7 August 2013). "The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 16 (5): 923–930. doi:10.1017/thg.2013.54. PMID 23919982. S2CID 13747480.
  124. Panizzon, Matthew S.; Vuoksimaa, Eero; Spoon, Kelly M.; Jacobson, Kristen C.; Lyons, Michael J.; Franz, Carol E.; Xian, Hong; Vasilopoulos, Terrie; Kremen, William S. (March 2014). "Genetic and environmental influences on general cognitive ability: Is g a valid latent construct?". Intelligence. 43: 65–76. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.008. PMC 4002017. PMID 24791031.
  125. Huguet, Guillaume; Schramm, Catherine; Douard, Elise; Jiang, Lai; Labbe, Aurélie; Tihy, Frédérique; Mathonnet, Géraldine; Nizard, Sonia; et al. (May 2018). "Measuring and Estimating the Effect Sizes of Copy Number Variants on General Intelligence in Community-Based Samples". JAMA Psychiatry. 75 (5): 447–457. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0039. PMC 5875373. PMID 29562078.
  126. Bouchard, TJ Jr. (1998). "Genetic and environmental influences on adult intelligence and special mental abilities". Human Biology; an International Record of Research. 70 (2): 257–79. PMID 9549239.
  127. ^ Plomin, R; Asbury, K; Dunn, J (2001). "Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later". Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 46 (3): 225–33. doi:10.1177/070674370104600302. PMID 11320676.
  128. Harris 2009, p. .
  129. Pietropaolo, S.; Crusio, W. E. (2010). "Genes and cognition". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2 (3): 345–352. doi:10.1002/wcs.135. PMID 26302082.
  130. Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009.
  131. Davies G, Tenesa A, Payton A, Yang J, Harris SE, Liewald D, Ke X, Le Hellard S, et al. (2011). "Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic". Mol Psychiatry. 16 (10): 996–1005. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.85. PMC 3182557. PMID 21826061.
  132. Benyamin B, Pourcain B, Davis OS, Davies G, Hansell NK, Brion MJ, Kirkpatrick RM, Cents RA, et al. (2013). "Childhood intelligence is heritable, highly polygenic and associated with FNBP1L". Mol Psychiatry. 19 (2): 253–258. doi:10.1038/mp.2012.184. PMC 3935975. PMID 23358156.
  133. Sniekers S, Stringer S, Watanabe K, Jansen PR, Coleman JRI, Krapohl E, Taskesen E, Hammerschlag AR, Okbay A, Zabaneh D, Amin N, Breen G, Cesarini D, Chabris CF, Iacono WG, Ikram MA, Johannesson M, Koellinger P, Lee JJ, Magnusson PKE, McGue M, Miller MB, Ollier WER, Payton A, Pendleton N, Plomin R, Rietveld CA, Tiemeier H, van Duijn CM, Posthuma D. Genome-wide association meta-analysis of 78,308 individuals identifies new loci and genes influencing human intelligence. Nat Genet. 2017 Jul;49(7):1107-1112. doi: 10.1038/ng.3869. Epub 2017 May 22. Erratum in: Nat Genet. 2017 Sep 27;49(10 ):1558. PMID 28530673; PMCID: PMC5665562.
  134. Benyamin B, Pourcain B, Davis OS, Davies G, Hansell NK, Brion MJ, Kirkpatrick RM, Cents RA, Franić S, Miller MB, Haworth CM, Meaburn E, Price TS, Evans DM, Timpson N, Kemp J, Ring S, McArdle W, Medland SE, Yang J, Harris SE, Liewald DC, Scheet P, Xiao X, Hudziak JJ, de Geus EJ; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2 (WTCCC2); Jaddoe VW, Starr JM, Verhulst FC, Pennell C, Tiemeier H, Iacono WG, Palmer LJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Boomsma DI, Posthuma D, McGue M, Wright MJ, Davey Smith G, Deary IJ, Plomin R, Visscher PM. Childhood intelligence is heritable, highly polygenic and associated with FNBP1L. Mol Psychiatry. 2014 Feb;19(2):253-8. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.184. Epub 2013 Jan 29. PMID 23358156; PMCID: PMC3935975.
  135. Rowe, D. C.; Jacobson, K. C. (1999). "Genetic and environmental influences on vocabulary IQ: parental education level as moderator". Child Development. 70 (5): 1151–62. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00084. PMID 10546338. S2CID 10959764.
  136. Tucker-Drob, E. M.; Rhemtulla, M.; Harden, K. P.; Turkheimer, E.; Fask, D. (2011). "Emergence of a Gene x Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Infant Mental Ability Between 10 Months and 2 Years". Psychological Science. 22 (1): 125–33. doi:10.1177/0956797610392926. PMC 3532898. PMID 21169524.
  137. Turkheimer, E.; Haley, A.; Waldron, M.; D'Onofrio, B.; Gottesman, I. I. (2003). "Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children". Psychological Science. 14 (6): 623–628. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. PMID 14629696. S2CID 11265284.
  138. Harden, K. P.; Turkheimer, E.; Loehlin, J. C. (2005). "Genotype environment interaction in adolescents' cognitive ability". Behavior Genetics. 35 (6): 804. doi:10.1007/s10519-005-7287-9. S2CID 189842802.
  139. Bates, Timothy C.; Lewis, Gary J.; Weiss, Alexander (3 September 2013). "Childhood Socioeconomic Status Amplifies Genetic Effects on Adult Intelligence" (PDF). Psychological Science. 24 (10): 2111–2116. doi:10.1177/0956797613488394. hdl:20.500.11820/52797d10-f0d4-49de-83e2-a9cc3493703d. PMID 24002887. S2CID 1873699.
  140. ^ Tucker-Drob, Elliot M.; Bates, Timothy C. (15 December 2015). "Large Cross-National Differences in Gene × Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Intelligence". Psychological Science. 27 (2): 138–149. doi:10.1177/0956797615612727. PMC 4749462. PMID 26671911.
  141. Hanscombe, K. B.; Trzaskowski, M.; Haworth, C. M.; Davis, O. S.; Dale, P. S.; Plomin, R. (2012). "Socioeconomic Status (SES) and Children's Intelligence (IQ): In a UK-Representative Sample SES Moderates the Environmental, Not Genetic, Effect on IQ". PLOS ONE. 7 (2): e30320. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...730320H. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030320. PMC 3270016. PMID 22312423.
  142. Dickens, William T.; Flynn, James R. (2001). "Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved" (PDF). Psychological Review. 108 (2): 346–69. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.139.2436. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346. PMID 11381833.
  143. Dickens, William T.; Flynn, James R. (2002). "The IQ Paradox: Still Resolved" (PDF). Psychological Review. 109 (4): 764–771. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.109.4.764. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 March 2007.
  144. Bidwell, Allie (13 December 2013). "Study: High Standardized Test Scores Don't Translate to Better Cognition". U.S. News & World Report. Archived from the original on 14 December 2013. Retrieved 1 July 2017.
  145. Jaeggi, S. M.; Buschkuehl, M.; Jonides, J.; Perrig, W. J. (2008). "From the Cover: Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 105 (19): 6829–33. Bibcode:2008PNAS..105.6829J. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105. PMC 2383929. PMID 18443283.
  146. Sternberg, R. J. (2008). "Increasing fluid intelligence is possible after all". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 105 (19): 6791–2. Bibcode:2008PNAS..105.6791S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0803396105. PMC 2383939. PMID 18474863.
  147. Glenn Schellenberg, E. (2004). "Music Lessons Enhance IQ". Psychological Science. 15 (8): 511–514. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.152.4349. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00711.x. PMID 15270994.
  148. Glenn Schellenberg, E. (2006). "Long-term positive associations between music lessons and IQ". Journal of Educational Psychology. 98 (2): 457–468. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.397.5160. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.457.
  149. Mosing, Miriam A.; Madison, Guy; Pedersen, Nancy L.; Ullén, Fredrik (1 May 2015). "Investigating cognitive transfer within the framework of music practice: genetic pleiotropy rather than causality". Developmental Science. 19 (3): 504–512. doi:10.1111/desc.12306. PMID 25939545.
  150. Sala, Giovanni; Gobet, Fernand (1 February 2017). "When the music's over. Does music skill transfer to children's and young adolescents' cognitive and academic skills? A meta-analysis". Educational Research Review. 20: 55–67. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2016.11.005. ISSN 1747-938X.
  151. Stough, C.; Kerkin, B.; Bates, T. C.; Mangan, G. (1994). "Music and spatial IQ". Personality and Individual Differences. 17 (5): 695. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90145-7. Retrieved 3 October 2020.
  152. Chabris, C. F. (1999). "Prelude or requiem for the 'Mozart effect'?". Nature. 400 (6747): 826–827. Bibcode:1999Natur.400..826C. doi:10.1038/23608. PMID 10476958. S2CID 898161.
  153. Deary, I.J.; Penke, L.; Johnson, W. (2010). "The neuroscience of human intelligence differences" (PDF). Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 11 (3): 201–211. doi:10.1038/nrn2793. hdl:20.500.11820/9b11fac3-47d0-424c-9d1c-fe6f9ff2ecac. PMID 20145623. S2CID 5136934.
  154. Buss, Claudia; Entringer, Sonja; Swanson, James M.; D. Wadhwa, Pathik (2012). "The Role of Stress in Brain Development: The Gestational Environment's Long-Term Effects on the Brain". Cerebrum. 4: 4. PMC 3574809. PMID 23447790.
  155. Eppig. Christopher. Scientific American."Why is average IQ higher in some places?" 2011.
  156. Olson, Rebecca; Gavin-Smith, Breda; Ferraboschi, Chiara; Kraemer, Klaus (April 2021). "Food Fortification: The Advantages, Disadvantages and Lessons from Sight and Life Programs". Nutrients. 13 (4): 1118. doi:10.3390/nu13041118. PMC 8066912. PMID 33805305.
  157. Nisen, Max (22 July 2013). "How Adding Iodine To Salt Resulted In A Decade's Worth Of IQ Gains For The United States". Business Insider. Retrieved 16 December 2023.
  158. Kamphaus, Randy W. (2005). Clinical assessment of child and adolescent intelligence. Springer. ISBN 978-0-387-26299-4.
  159. Frey & Detterman 2004.
  160. Deary et al. 2007.
  161. Hunter, John E.; Hunter, Ronda F. (1984). "Validity and utility of alternative predictors of job performance". Psychological Bulletin. 96 (1): 72–98. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.96.1.72. S2CID 26858912.
  162. Warner, Molly; Ernst, John; Townes, Brenda; Peel, John; Preston, Michael (1987). "Relationships Between IQ and Neuropsychological Measures in Neuropsychiatric Populations: Within-Laboratory and Cross-Cultural Replications Using WAIS and WAIS-R". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 9 (5): 545–62. doi:10.1080/01688638708410768. PMID 3667899.
  163. Byington, Eliza; Felps, Will (2010). "Why do IQ scores predict job performance?". Research in Organizational Behavior. 30: 175–202. doi:10.1016/j.riob.2010.08.003.
  164. Sackett, Paul R.; Zhang, Charlene; Berry, Christopher M.; Lievens, Filip (2023). "Revisiting the design of selection systems in light of new findings regarding the validity of widely used predictors". Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice. 16 (3). Retrieved 24 April 2023.
  165. Sackett, Paul R.; Zhang, Charlene; Berry, Christopher M.; Lievens, Filip (2022). "Revisiting meta-analytic estimates of validity in personnel selection: Addressing systematic overcorrection for restriction of range". Journal of Applied Psychology. 107 (11): 2040–2068. doi:10.1037/apl0000994. PMID 34968080. S2CID 245594032.
  166. Watkins, M; Lei, P; Canivez, G (2007). "Psychometric intelligence and achievement: A cross-lagged panel analysis". Intelligence. 35 (1): 59–68. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.397.3155. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.04.005.
  167. Rohde, T; Thompson, L (2007). "Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability". Intelligence. 35 (1): 83–92. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.05.004.
  168. Coward, W. Mark; Sackett, Paul R. (1990). "Linearity of ability-performance relationships: A reconfirmation". Journal of Applied Psychology. 75 (3): 297–300. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.75.3.297.
  169. Robertson, Kimberley Ferriman; Smeets, Stijn; Lubinski, David; Benbow, Camilla P. (14 December 2010). "Beyond the Threshold Hypothesis" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 19 (6): 346–351. doi:10.1177/0963721410391442. S2CID 46218795.
  170. Detterman & Daniel 1989.
  171. Hunt, Earl B. (July 1995). "The Role of Intelligence in Modern Society (July–Aug, 1995)". American Scientist. pp. 4 (Nonlinearities in Intelligence). Archived from the original on 21 May 2006.
  172. Henderson, Mark (25 April 2007). "Brains don't make you rich IQ study finds". The Times. London. Archived from the original on 6 September 2008. Retrieved 5 May 2010.
  173. "You Don't Have To Be Smart To Be Rich, Study Finds". ScienceDaily. Retrieved 26 August 2014.
  174. Murray 1998, p. .
  175. Bowles, Samuel; Gintis, Herbert (2002). "The Inheritance of Inequality". Journal of Economic Perspectives. 16 (3): 3–30. doi:10.1257/089533002760278686.
  176. Cullen, Francis T.; Gendreau, Paul; Jarjoura, G. Roger; Wright, John Paul (October 1997). "Crime and the Bell Curve: Lessons from Intelligent Criminology". Crime & Delinquency. 43 (4): 387–411. doi:10.1177/0011128797043004001. S2CID 145418972.
  177. Handbook of Crime Correlates; Lee Ellis, Kevin M. Beaver, John Wright; 2009; Academic Press
  178. Beaver, Kevin M.; Schwartz, Joseph A.; Nedelec, Joseph L.; Connolly, Eric J.; Boutwell, Brian B.; Barnes, J.C. (September 2013). "Intelligence is associated with criminal justice processing: Arrest through incarceration". Intelligence. 41 (5): 277–288. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.05.001.
  179. Beaver, Kevin M.; Wright, John Paul (January 2011). "The association between county-level IQ and county-level crime rates". Intelligence. 39 (1): 22–26. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.12.002.
  180. Mears, Daniel P.; Cochran, Joshua C. (November 2013). "What is the effect of IQ on offending?". Criminal Justice and Behavior. 40 (11): 1280–1300. doi:10.1177/0093854813485736. S2CID 147219554.
  181. Freeman, James (January 2012). "The relationship between lower intelligence, crime and custodial outcomes: a brief literary review of a vulnerable group". Vulnerable Groups & Inclusion. 3 (1): 14834. doi:10.3402/vgi.v3i0.14834. S2CID 145305072.
  182. Ttofi, Maria M.; Farrington, David P.; Piquero, Alex R.; Lösel, Friedrich; DeLisi, Matthew; Murray, Joseph (1 June 2016). "Intelligence as a protective factor against offending: A meta-analytic review of prospective longitudinal studies". Journal of Criminal Justice. 45: 4–18. doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.02.003.
  183. McGloin, Jean Marie; Pratt, Travis C.; Maahs, Jeff (1 September 2004). "Rethinking the IQ-delinquency relationship: A longitudinal analysis of multiple theoretical models". Justice Quarterly. 21 (3): 603–635. doi:10.1080/07418820400095921. S2CID 143305924.
  184. Gottfredson, Linda S.; Deary, Ian J. (22 June 2016). "Intelligence Predicts Health and Longevity, but Why?". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 13 (1): 1–4. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301001.x. S2CID 15176389.
  185. Batty, G. David; Deary, Ian J.; Gottfredson, Linda S. (2007). "Premorbid (early life) IQ and Later Mortality Risk: Systematic Review". Annals of Epidemiology. 17 (4): 278–288. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.693.9671. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.07.010. PMID 17174570.
  186. ^ Kaufman 2009, p. 126.
  187. ^ Kaufman & Lichtenberger 2006.
  188. Kaufman 2009, p. 132.
  189. Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012). "Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments". American Psychologist. 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. ISSN 1935-990X. PMID 22233090.
  190. ^ Ceci & Williams 2009, pp. 788–789, "There is an emerging consensus about racial and gender equality in genetic determinants of intelligence, most researchers, including ourselves, agree that genes do not explain between-group differences".
  191. ^ "Intelligence research should not be held back by its past". Nature. 545 (7655): 385–386. 25 May 2017. Bibcode:2017Natur.545R.385.. doi:10.1038/nature.2017.22021. PMID 28541341. S2CID 4449918. Intelligence science has undoubtedly been dogged by ugly prejudice. Historical measurements of skull volume and brain weight were done to advance claims of the racial superiority of white people. More recently, the (genuine but closing) gap between the average IQ scores of groups of black and white people in the United States has been falsely attributed to genetic differences between the races.
  192. ^ Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012). "Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin" (PDF). American Psychologist. 67 (6): 503–504. doi:10.1037/a0029772. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22963427. Retrieved 22 July 2013.
  193. ^ Mackintosh 2011, pp. 362–363
  194. ^ Hunt 2011, p. 389
  195. Using Population Descriptors in Genetics and Genomics Research: A New Framework for an Evolving Field (Consensus Study Report). National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2023. doi:10.17226/26902. ISBN 978-0-309-70065-8. PMID 36989389. In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups.
  196. Jencks, Christopher; Phillips, Meredith (2011) . The Black-White Test Score Gap. Brookings Institution Press. p. 503. ISBN 9780815746119. The available evidence reviewed by several authors in this volume provides, as Richard E. Nisbett puts it, 'no evidence for genetic superiority of either race while providing strong evidence for a substantial environmental contribution to the black-white IQ gap.'
  197. ^ Birney, Ewan; Raff, Jennifer; Rutherford, Adam; Scally, Aylwyn (24 October 2019). "Race, genetics and pseudoscience: an explainer". Ewan's Blog: Bioinformatician at large. 'Human biodiversity' proponents sometimes assert that alleged differences in the mean value of IQ when measured in different populations – such as the claim that IQ in some sub-Saharan African countries is measurably lower than in European countries – are caused by genetic variation, and thus are inherent. . . . Such tales, and the claims about the genetic basis for population differences, are not scientifically supported. In reality for most traits, including IQ, it is not only unclear that genetic variation explains differences between populations, it is also unlikely.
  198. Aaron, Panofsky; Dasgupta, Kushan (28 September 2020). "How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics". American Journal of Biological Anthropology. 175 (2): 387–398. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24150. PMC 9909835. PMID 32986847. S2CID 222163480. he claims that genetics defines racial groups and makes them different, that IQ and cultural differences among racial groups are caused by genes, and that racial inequalities within and between nations are the inevitable outcome of long evolutionary processes are neither new nor supported by science (either old or new).
  199. ^ Dickens, William T.; Flynn, James R. (2006). "Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples" (PDF). Psychological Science. 17 (10): 913–920. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x. PMID 17100793. S2CID 6593169.
  200. Mitchell, Kevin (2 May 2018). "Why genetic IQ differences between 'races' are unlikely: The idea that intelligence can differ between populations has made headlines again, but the rules of evolution make it implausible". The Guardian. Retrieved 13 June 2020.
  201. ^ Kaplan, Jonathan Michael (January 2015). "Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis"". Biology & Philosophy. 30 (1): 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0. ISSN 0169-3867. S2CID 85351431.
  202. Dickens, William T.; Flynn, James R. (2006). "Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples" (PDF). Psychological Science. 17 (10): 913–920. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x. PMID 17100793. S2CID 6593169.
  203. Mackintosh 2011, p. 348.
  204. Inzlicht, Michael (2011). Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application. Oxford University Press. pp. 5, 141–143. ISBN 978-0199732449.
  205. Shuttleworth-Edwards, Ann B.; Van der Merwe, Adele S. (2002). "WAIS-III and WISC-IV South African Cross-Cultural Normative Data Stratified for Quality of Education". In Ferraro, F. Richard (ed.). Minority and cross-cultural aspects of neuropsychological assessment. Exton, PA: Swets & Zeitlinger. pp. 72–75. ISBN 9026518307.
  206. Barbara P. Uzzell, Marcel Ponton, Alfredo Ardila International Handbook of Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology book ISBN 978-0805835854 (2013)
  207. ^ Plotnik R, Kouyoumdjian H (2013). Introduction to Psychology. Cengage Learning. pp. 282–283. ISBN 978-1133939535.
  208. ^ Hunt 2011, pp. 378–379
  209. Terry WS (2015). Learning and Memory: Basic Principles, Processes, and Procedures, Fourth Edition. Psychology Press. p. 356. ISBN 978-1317350873.
  210. Chrisler JC, McCreary DR (2010). Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology: Volume 1: Gender Research in General and Experimental Psychology. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 302. ISBN 978-1441914651.
  211. Hyde, J. S.; Linn, M. C. (27 October 2006). "DIVERSITY: Enhanced: Gender Similarities in Mathematics and Science". Science. 314 (5799): 599–600. doi:10.1126/science.1132154. PMID 17068246. S2CID 34045261.
  212. Hyde, Janet S.; Fennema, Elizabeth; Lamon, Susan J. (1990). "Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 107 (2): 139–155. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.139. PMID 2138794.
  213. Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012). "Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments". American Psychologist. 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. PMID 22233090. S2CID 7001642.
  214. Jensen 1998, p. 531.
  215. Kavanagh, Jennifer (2005). Determinants of Productivity for Military Personnel (PDF) (Report). Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation. ISBN 0-8330-3754-4. Retrieved 1 July 2017.
  216. Kilburn, M. Rebecca; Hanser, Lawrence M.; Klerman, Jacob A. (2009). Estimating AFQT Scores for National Educational Longitudinal Study (NELS) Respondents (PDF) (Report). Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation. Retrieved 1 July 2017.
  217. "12.00-Mental Disorders-Adult". www.ssa.gov. U. S. Social Security Administration. Retrieved 1 July 2017.
  218. Solomon, Deborah (12 December 2004). "The Science of Second-Guessing". The New York Times.

General and cited references

External links

Library resources about
Intelligence quotient
Human intelligence topics
Types
Abilities, traits,
and constructs
Models and theories
Areas of research
High IQ
Topics
High IQ societies
Testing
Figures in high IQ research
Longitudinal studies
Related
Categories: