Misplaced Pages

Evolutionary approaches to depression: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 19:29, 21 October 2011 editMiradre (talk | contribs)9,214 edits better link← Previous edit Latest revision as of 06:43, 9 December 2024 edit undoCitation bot (talk | contribs)Bots5,409,520 edits Added bibcode. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | Suggested by Jay8g | #UCB_toolbar 
(190 intermediate revisions by 77 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|none}}
] has proposed several different evolutionary explanations for ].
Evolutionary approaches to ] are attempts by ] and ] to use the theory of ] to further understand ]s. <!-- This sentence is copied onto ]; consider reflecting any changes made here onto that article. -->Depression is generally thought of as dysfunction or a ], but its prevalence does not increase with age the way dementia and other organic dysfunction commonly does. Some researchers have surmised that the disorder may have evolutionary roots, in the same way that others suggest evolutionary contributions to ], ], ] and other disorders. The proposed explanations for the evolution of depression remain controversial.


== Background == == Background ==
{{Cleanup|date=November 2024|reason=This reads too much like a textbook.|section}}
] is the leading cause of disability worldwide, and in 2000 was the fourth leading contributor to the global burden of disease (measured in ]); it is also an important risk factor for suicide.<ref name=WHO>{{Cite document| first= | last=| coauthors=| contribution=The World Health Report 2001| title=Health Systems: Improving Performance| editor-first=| editor-last=| coeditors=| publisher=World Health Organization| place=Geneva| pages=| date=| year=2001| id= | contribution-url=| format=| accessdate=2008-03-22| postscript= <!--None--> }}</ref> It is understandable, then, that clinical depression is thought to be a pathology — a major dysfunction of the brain. In most cases, rates of organ dysfunction increase with age, with low rates in adolescents and young adults, and the highest rates in the elderly.<ref name="CDC">{{cite web |url=http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5606a2.htm |title=Prevalence of Heart Disease --- United States, 2005 |accessdate=2008-03-22 |work=}}{{cite web |url=http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5608a2.htm |title=Prevalence of Chronic Kidney Disease and Associated Risk Factors --- United States, 1999--2004 |accessdate=2008-03-22 |work=}}</ref> These patterns are consistent with ] which posit that selection against dysfunctional traits decreases with age (because there is a decreasing probability of surviving to later ages).
] (also called "major depressive disorder", "clinical depression" or often simply "depression") is a leading cause of disability worldwide, and in 2000 was the fourth leading contributor to the global burden of disease (measured in ]); it is also an important risk factor for suicide.<ref name=WHO>{{Citation |mode=cs1 |contribution=The World Health Report 2001| title=Health Systems: Improving Performance |publisher=World Health Organization |place=Geneva |year=2001}}</ref> It is understandable, then, that clinical depression is thought to be a pathology—a major dysfunction of the brain.


In most cases, rates of organ dysfunction increase with age, with low rates in adolescents and young adults, and the highest rates in the elderly.<ref name="CDC">{{cite web |url=https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5606a2.htm |title=Prevalence of Heart Disease --- United States, 2005 |access-date=2008-03-22 }}{{cite web |url=https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5608a2.htm |title=Prevalence of Chronic Kidney Disease and Associated Risk Factors --- United States, 1999--2004 |access-date=2008-03-22 }}</ref> These patterns are consistent with ] which posit that selection against dysfunctional traits decreases with age (because there is a decreasing probability of surviving to later ages).
In contrast to these patterns, prevalence of clinical depression is high in all age categories, including otherwise healthy adolescents and young adults. In one study of the US population, for example, the 12 month prevalence for a major depression episode was highest in the youngest age category (15–24 year olds).<ref name="pmid8300981">{{cite journal |author=Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz M, Blazer DG, Nelson CB |title=Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. I: Lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence |journal=J Affect Disord |volume=29 |issue=2–3 |pages=85–96 |year=1993 |pmid=8300981 |doi=10.1016/0165-0327(93)90026-G}}</ref> The high prevalence of depression is also an outlier when compared to the prevalence of major ], ], and ], all with prevalence rates about one tenth that of depression, or less.<ref>Epidemiologic Catchment Area, National Comorbidity Study</ref>


In contrast to these patterns, prevalence of clinical depression is high in all age categories, including otherwise healthy adolescents and young adults. In one study of the US population, for example, the 12 month prevalence for a major depression episode was highest in the youngest age category (15- to 24-year-olds).<ref name="pmid8300981">{{cite journal |vauthors=Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz M, Blazer DG, Nelson CB |title=Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. I: Lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence |journal=J Affect Disord |volume=29 |issue=2–3 |pages=85–96 |year=1993 |pmid=8300981 |doi=10.1016/0165-0327(93)90026-G|url=https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/30529/1/0000161.pdf |hdl=2027.42/30529 |hdl-access=free }}</ref> The high prevalence of unipolar depression (excluding depression associated ]) is also an outlier when compared to the prevalence of other mental disorders such as major ], ], ] and even the aforementioned bipolar disorder, all with prevalence rates about one tenth that of depression, or less.<ref>Epidemiologic Catchment Area, National Comorbidity Study</ref> {{as of|2017}}, the only mental disorders with a higher prevalence than depression are ].<ref>{{cite journal |url=https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health|title=Mental Health |last1= Ritchie |first1= Hannah |author1-link=Hannah Ritchie |last2 = Roser|first2 = Max |author2-link=Max Roser |date= 2018 |journal= Our World in Data |publisher= Global Change Data Lab}}</ref>
The common occurrence and persistence of a ] like clinical depression with such negative effects early in life is difficult to explain. (Rates of infectious disease are high in young people, of course, but clinical depression is not thought to be caused by an infection.) ] and its application in ] suggest how behaviour and mental states, including seemingly harmful states such as depression, may be past adaptations to recurring reproductive problems faced by our ancestors, actually having improved (however disadvantageous in the modern world), the ] of either the individual or their ].<ref name="NeeseEvol">{{cite book | last = Neese | first = Randolph M. | authorlink = Randolph M. Nesse | title = The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Mood Disorders - Chapter 10: Evolutionary Explanations for Mood and Mood Disorders | pages = 159–175 | publisher = American Psychiatric Publishing | location=Washington, DC | date = 2005-10 | url = http://www-personal.umich.edu/~nesse/Articles/Nesse-EvolMood-APAText-2006.pdf | isbn = 1-5856-2151-X | accessdate = 2007-10-23 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite document|publisher=New York Times| title=Viewing Depression As Tool for Survival|url=http://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res=9800E0D8163FF932A35751C0A9669C8B63|author=Erica Goode|date=February 1, 2000|postscript=<!--None-->
}}</ref><ref>{{Cite document|publisher=Australian Broadcasting Corporation|title=The Evolution of Depression – Does it Have a Role?|author=Natasha Mitchell|url=http://www.abc.net.au/rn/science/mind/stories/s1077027.htm|date=3 April 2004|postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref><ref>{{Cite document|publisher=LA Times|title=Depression as a survival tool? Some new treatments assume so|author=Julia M. Klein|url=http://www.biopsychiatry.com/depression/adaptive.html|date=12 February 2007|postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref> It has been argued, for example, that ] life-long depression was a source of insight and strength.<ref>{{Cite document|publisher=Houghton Mifflin|title=Lincoln's Melancholy: How Depression Challenged a President and Fueled His Greatness|author=Joshua Wolf Shenk|year=2005|postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref> Some even suggest that ''"we aren’t designed to have happiness as our natural default"'' and so a state of depression is the evolutionary norm.<ref>{{Cite document|title=Why the happiness industry can only lead to misery|publisher=The Times|date=January 19, 2008|author=John Naish|url=http://women.timesonline.co.uk/tol/life_and_style/women/body_and_soul/article3209845.ece | location=London|postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref>


The common occurrence and persistence of a ] like clinical depression with such negative effects early in life is difficult to explain. (Rates of infectious disease are high in young people, of course, but clinical depression is not thought to be caused by an infection.) ] and its application in ] suggest how behaviour and mental states, including seemingly harmful states such as depression, may have been beneficial adaptations of human ancestors which improved the ] of individuals or their ].<ref name="NeeseEvol">{{cite book | last1 = Neese | first1 = Randolph M. | author-link = Randolph M. Nesse | title = The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Mood Disorders - Chapter 10: Evolutionary Explanations for Mood and Mood Disorders | pages = | publisher = American Psychiatric Publishing | location = Washington, DC | date = October 2005 | url = https://archive.org/details/isbn_9781585621514/page/159 | isbn = 978-1-58562-151-4 | access-date = 2007-10-23 | url-access = registration }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|journal=New York Times| title=Viewing Depression As Tool for Survival|url=https://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res=9800E0D8163FF932A35751C0A9669C8B63|author=Erica Goode|date=February 1, 2000}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|publisher=Australian Broadcasting Corporation|title=The Evolution of Depression – Does it Have a Role?|author=Natasha Mitchell|url=http://www.abc.net.au/rn/science/mind/stories/s1077027.htm|date=3 April 2004}}</ref><ref>{{cite news|newspaper=LA Times|title=Depression as a survival tool? Some new treatments assume so|author=Julia M. Klein|url=http://www.biopsychiatry.com/depression/adaptive.html|date=12 February 2007}}</ref> It has been argued, for example, that ]'s lifelong depression was a source of insight and strength.<ref>{{Cite book|publisher=Houghton Mifflin|title=Lincoln's Melancholy: How Depression Challenged a President and Fueled His Greatness|url=https://archive.org/details/lincolnsmelancho00shen|url-access=registration|author=Joshua Wolf Shenk|year=2005|isbn=9780618551163 }}</ref> Some even suggest that "we aren't designed to have happiness as our natural default" and so a state of depression is the evolutionary norm.<ref>{{Cite journal|title=Why the happiness industry can only lead to misery|journal=The Times|date=January 19, 2008|author=John Naish|url=http://women.timesonline.co.uk/tol/life_and_style/women/body_and_soul/article3209845.ece |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110615200943/http://women.timesonline.co.uk/tol/life_and_style/women/body_and_soul/article3209845.ece |url-status=dead |archive-date=June 15, 2011 | location=London}}</ref>
The following ] attempt to identify a benefit of depression that outweighs its obvious costs. All take as their starting point the fact that one of the most potent, well-established causes of major depression is a severe negative life event.

The following ] attempt to identify a benefit of depression that outweighs its obvious costs.

Such hypotheses are not necessarily incompatible with one another and may explain different aspects, causes, and symptoms of depression.<ref name = "Allen and Badcock"/>


==Psychic pain hypothesis== ==Psychic pain hypothesis==
One reason depression is thought to be a pathology is that it causes so much psychic pain and distress. However, ] is also very distressful, yet it has an evolved function: to inform the organism that it is suffering damage, to motivate it to withdraw from the source of damage, and to learn to avoid such damage-causing circumstances in the future. One reason depression is thought to be a pathology is that it causes so much psychic pain and distress. However, ] is also very distressful, yet it has an evolved function: to inform the organism that it is being damaged, to motivate it to withdraw from the source of damage, and to learn to avoid such damage-causing circumstances in the future. Sadness is also distressing, yet is widely believed to be an evolved adaptation. In fact, perhaps the most influential evolutionary view is that most cases of depression are simply particularly intense cases of sadness in response to adversity, such as the loss of a loved one.<ref name="HorwitzWakefield2007">{{cite book|author1=Allan V. Horwitz|author2=Jerome C. Wakefield|title=The Loss of Sadness: How Psychiatry Transformed Normal Sorrow into Depressive Disorder|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=oWmtN3wSJmoC|date=18 June 2007|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0-19-804269-3}}</ref>

According to the psychic pain hypothesis, ] is analogous to physical pain in that it informs them that current circumstances, such as the loss of a friend, are imposing a threat to ]. It motivates them to cease activities that led to the costly situation, if possible, and it causes him or her to learn to avoid similar circumstances in the future. Proponents of this view tend to focus on low mood, and regard clinical depression as a dysfunctional extreme of low mood—and not as a unique set of characteristics that are physiologically distanced from regular depressed mood.

Alongside the absence of pleasure, other noticeable changes include psychomotor retardation, disrupted patterns of sleeping and feeding, a loss of sex drive and motivation—which are all also characteristics of the body's reaction to actual physical pain. In depressed people there is an increased activity in the regions of the cortex involved with the perception of pain, such as the ] and the left ]. This activity allows the cortex to manifest an abstract negative thought as a true physical stressor to the rest of the brain.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Thornhill NW, Thornhill R |title=An evolutionary analysis of psychological pain following human (Homo sapiens) rape: IV. The effect of the nature of the sexual assault |journal=J Comp Psychol |volume=105 |issue=3 |pages=243–52 |year=1991 |pmid=1935004 |doi=10.1037/0735-7036.105.3.243}}</ref><ref name=Thornhill1989>{{cite journal | author = Thornhill, R. |author2=Thornhill, N.W. | year = 1989 | title = The evolution of psychological pain | journal = Sociobiology and the Social Sciences
| pages = 73–103}}</ref><ref name="NeeseAdapt">{{cite journal | last1 = Neese | first1 = Randolph M. | author-link = Randolph M. Nesse | title = Is Depression an Adaptation? | journal = Archives of General Psychiatry | volume = 57 | issue = 1 | pages = 14–20 | date = January 2005 | url = http://www-personal.umich.edu/~nesse/Articles/IsDepAdapt-ArchGenPsychiat-2000.pdf | access-date = 2007-10-23 | doi = 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14 | pmid = 10632228 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.318.2659 | archive-date = 2020-11-09 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20201109220010/http://www-personal.umich.edu/~nesse/Articles/IsDepAdapt-ArchGenPsychiat-2000.pdf | url-status = dead }}</ref><ref name=Suarez1991>{{cite journal | author = Suarez, S.D. |author2=Gallup Jr, G.G. | year = 1991 | title = Depression as a response to reproductive failure | journal = Journal of Social and Biological Structures | volume = 8| pages = 279–287 | doi = 10.1016/0140-1750(85)90071-5 | issue = 3}}</ref><ref name="HagenPerinatal">{{cite journal | last1 = Hagen | first1 = Edward H. | author-link = Edward Hagen (anthropologist) | author2 = Barrett, Clark | author-link2 = H. Clark Barrett | title = Perinatal Sadness among Shuar Women: Support for an Evolutionary Theory of Psychic Pain | journal = Medical Anthropology Quarterly | volume = 21 | issue = 1 | pages = 22–40 | year = 2007 | issn = 0745-5194 | url = http://itb.biologie.hu-berlin.de/~hagen/papers/perinatal.pdf | access-date = 2007-10-23 | doi = 10.1525/maq.2007.21.1.22 | pmid = 17405696 | url-status = dead | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20070926114952/http://itb.biologie.hu-berlin.de/~hagen/papers/perinatal.pdf | archive-date = 2007-09-26 }}</ref><ref name="IsLowMood">{{cite journal | last1 = Keller | first1 = Matthew C. | author-link = Matthew Keller | author2 = Neese, Randolph M. | author-link2 = Randolph M. Nesse | title = Is low mood an adaptation? Evidence for subtypes with symptoms that match precipitants | journal = Journal of Affective Disorders | volume = 86 | issue = 1 | pages = 27–35 | date = May 2005 | url = http://www-personal.umich.edu/~nesse/Articles/Keller-Nesse-MoodSubtypes-JAD-2005.pdf | access-date = 2007-10-23 | doi = 10.1016/j.jad.2004.12.005 | pmid = 15820268 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.409.6927 | archive-date = 2007-08-10 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20070810112852/http://www-personal.umich.edu/~nesse/Articles/Keller-Nesse-MoodSubtypes-JAD-2005.pdf | url-status = dead }}</ref><ref name=Tooby1990>{{cite journal | author = Tooby, J. |author2=Cosmides, L. | year = 1990 | title = The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments | journal = Ethology and Sociobiology | volume = 11 | issue = 4–5 | pages = 375–424 | doi = 10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z| doi-access = free }}</ref><ref name=Hagen1999>{{cite journal | author = Hagen, E.H. | year = 1999 | title = The Functions of Postpartum Depression | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 20 | issue = 5 | pages = 325–359 | doi = 10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00016-1| bibcode = 1999EHumB..20..325H | citeseerx = 10.1.1.335.7173 }}</ref><ref name=Nettle2004>{{cite journal | author = Nettle, D. | year = 2004 | title = Evolutionary origins of depression: a review and reformulation | journal = Journal of Affective Disorders | volume = 81 | issue = 2 | pages = 91–102 | doi = 10.1016/j.jad.2003.08.009 | pmid = 15306134}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Sachar|first1=E|title=Neuroendocrine abnormalities in depressive illness|journal=Topics of Psychoendocrinology|year=1975|page=135}}</ref>

==Behavioral shutdown model==
The behavioral shutdown model states that if an organism faces more risk or expenditure than reward from activities, the best evolutionary strategy may be to withdraw from them. This model proposes that emotional pain, like physical pain, serves a useful adaptive purpose. Negative emotions like disappointment, sadness, grief, fear, anxiety, anger, and guilt are described as "evolved strategies that allow for the identification and avoidance of specific problems, especially in the social domain." Depression is characteristically associated with ] and lack of energy, and those experiencing it are risk-aversive and perceive more negative and pessimistic outcomes because they are focused on preventing further loss. Although the model views depression as an adaptive response, it does not suggest that it is beneficial by the standards of current society; but it does suggest that many approaches to depression treat symptoms rather than causes, and underlying social problems need to be addressed.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://psychweb.cisat.jmu.edu/ToKSystem/My%20ToK%20Papers/BSM%20Final.pdf|title=Depression: disease or behavioral shutdown mechanism|author=Gregg Henriques|publisher=Journal of Science and Health Policy|access-date=2009-12-14|archive-date=2016-03-03|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160303183911/http://psychweb.cisat.jmu.edu/ToKSystem/My%20ToK%20Papers/BSM%20Final.pdf|url-status=dead}}</ref>

A related phenomenon to the behavioral shutdown model is ]. In animal subjects, a loss of control or predictability in the subject's experiences results in a condition similar to clinical depression in humans. That is to say, if uncontrollable and unstoppable stressors are repeated for long enough, a rat subject will adopt a learned helplessness, which shares a number of behavioral and psychological features with human depression. The subject will not attempt to cope with problems, even when placed in a stressor-free novel environment. Should their rare attempts at coping prove successful in a new environment, a long lasting cognitive block prevents them from perceiving their action as useful and their coping strategy does not last long. From an evolutionary perspective, learned helplessness also allows a conservation of energy for an extended period of time should people find themselves in a predicament that is outside of their control, such as an illness or a dry season. However, for today's humans whose depression resembles learned helplessness, this phenomenon usually manifests as a loss of motivation and the distortion of one uncontrollable aspect of a person's life being viewed as representative of all aspects of their life – suggesting a mismatch between ultimate cause and modern manifestation.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Seligman|first1=Martin|title=Helplessness: On Depression, Development and Death|year=1975|publisher=W.H. Freeman|location=San Francisco}}</ref>

==Analytical rumination hypothesis==
This hypothesis suggests that depression is an adaptation that causes the affected individual to concentrate his or her attention and focus on a complex problem in order to analyze and solve it.<ref name = Andrews2009>{{cite journal |author1=Andrews, P.W. |author2=Thompson, J.A. | year = 2009 | title = The bright side of being blue: depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems | journal = Psychological Review | volume = 116 | pages = 620–654 | doi = 10.1037/a0016242 | pmid = 19618990 | issue = 3 | pmc = 2734449}}</ref>

One way depression increases the individual's focus on a problem is by inducing rumination. Depression activates the left ], which increases attention control and maintains problem-related information in an "active, accessible state" referred to as "]", or WM. As a result, depressed individuals have been shown to ruminate, reflecting on the reasons for their current problems. Feelings of regret associated with depression also cause individuals to reflect and analyze past events in order to determine why they happened and how they could have been prevented.<ref name = Andrews2009/> The rumination hypothesis has come under criticism. Evolutionary fitness is increased by ruminating before rather than after bad outcomes. A situation that resulted in a child being in danger but unharmed should lead the parent to ruminate on how to avoid the dangerous situation in the future. Waiting until the child dies and then ruminating in a state of depression is too late.<ref name=Wittman2014>{{cite journal | author = Wittman, D. | year = 2014 | title = Darwinian Depression | journal = Journal of Affective Disorders | volume = 168 | issue = 2 | pages = 142–150 | doi = 10.1016/j.jad.2014.06.052 | pmid = 25046740 }}</ref>

Some cognitive psychologists argue that ruminative tendency itself increases the likelihood of the onset of depression.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Nolen-Hoeksma|first1=S|s2cid=5026228|title=Sex differences in depression: theory and evidence|journal=Psychological Bulletin|year=1987|volume=101|doi=10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.259|pmid=3562707|issue=2|pages=259–82}}</ref>

Another way depression increases an individual's ability to concentrate on a problem is by reducing distraction from the problem. For example, anhedonia, which is often associated with depression, decreases an individual's desire to participate in activities that provide short-term rewards, and instead, allows the individual to concentrate on long-term goals. In addition, "psychomotoric changes", such as solitariness, decreased appetite, and insomnia also reduce distractions. For instance, insomnia enables conscious analysis of the problem to be maintained by preventing sleep from disrupting such processes. Likewise, solitariness, lack of physical activity, and lack of appetite all eliminate sources of distraction, such as social interactions, navigation through the environment,<ref name = Andrews2009/> and "oral activity", which disrupt stimuli from being processed.<ref name = Jacobs1999>{{cite journal |author1=Jacobs, B.L. |author2=Fornal, C.A. | year = 1999 | title = Activity of serotonergic neurons in behaving animals | journal = Neuropsychopharmacology | volume = 21 |issue=2 Suppl |pages=9S–15S | doi= 10.1038/sj.npp.1395336 |pmid=10432483 }}</ref>

==Possibilities of depression as a dysregulated adaptation==
Depression, especially in the modern context, may not necessarily be adaptive. The ability to feel pain and experience depression, are adaptive defense mechanisms,<ref name = Gilbert2006>{{cite journal|author=Gilbert, P. |s2cid=15408068|year=2006|title= Evolution and depression: issues and implications|journal= Psychological Medicine |volume=36|pages=287–297|doi=10.1017/S0033291705006112|pmid=16236231|issue=3}}</ref> but when they are "too easily triggered, too intense, or long lasting", they can become "dysregulated".<ref name=Gilbert2006/> In such a case, defense mechanisms, too, can become diseases, such as "chronic pain or dehydration from diarrhea". Depression, which may be a similar kind of defense mechanism, may have become dysregulated as well.<ref name = Nesse2000>{{cite journal | author = Nesse, R.M. | year = 2000 | title = Is Depression an Adaptation? | journal = Archives of General Psychiatry | volume = 57 | issue = 1 | pages= 14–20 | doi = 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14 | pmid=10632228| citeseerx = 10.1.1.318.2659 }}</ref>


Thus, unlike other evolutionary theories this one sees depression as a maladaptive extreme of something that is beneficial in smaller amounts. In particular, one theory focuses on the personality trait ]. Low amounts of neuroticism may increase a person's fitness through various processes, but too much may reduce fitness by, for example, recurring depressions. Thus, evolution will select for an optimal amount and most people will have neuroticism near this amount. However, genetic variation continually occurs, and some people will have high neuroticism which increases the risk of depressions.<ref name = "Allen and Badcock"/>
According to the psychic pain hypothesis, depression is analogous to physical pain in that it informs the sufferer that current circumstances, such as the loss of a friend, are imposing a threat to ], it motivates the sufferer to cease activities that led to the costly situation, if possible, and it causes him or her to learn to avoid similar circumstances in the future. Proponents of this view tend to focus on ], and regard clinical depression as a dysfunctional extreme of low mood.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Thornhill NW, Thornhill R |title=An evolutionary analysis of psychological pain following human (Homo sapiens) rape: IV. The effect of the nature of the sexual assault |journal=J Comp Psychol |volume=105 |issue=3 |pages=243–52 |year=1991 |pmid=1935004 |doi=10.1037/0735-7036.105.3.243}}</ref><ref name=Thornhill1989>{{cite journal | author = Thornhill, R. | coauthors = Thornhill, N.W. | year = 1989 | title = The evolution of psychological pain | journal = Sociobiology and the Social Sciences
| pages = 73–103}}</ref><ref name="NeeseAdapt">{{cite journal | last = Neese | first = Randolph M. | authorlink = Randolph M. Nesse | title = Is Depression an Adaptation? | journal = Archives of General Psychology | volume = 57 | issue = 1 | pages = 14–20 | publisher = American Medical Association | date = 2005-01 | url = http://www-personal.umich.edu/~nesse/Articles/IsDepAdapt-ArchGenPsychiat-2000.pdf | accessdate = 2007-10-23 | doi = 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14 | pmid=10632228}}</ref><ref name=Suarez1991>{{cite journal | author = Suarez, S.D. | coauthors = Gallup Jr, G.G. | year = 1991 | title = Depression as a response to reproductive failure | journal = Journal of Social and Biological Structures | volume = 8| pages = 279–287 | doi = 10.1016/0140-1750(85)90071-5 | issue = 3}}</ref><ref name="HagenPerinatal">{{cite journal | last = Hagen | first = Edward H. | authorlink = Edward Hagen | coauthors = ] | title = Perinatal Sadness among Shuar Women: Support for an Evolutionary Theory of Psychic Pain | journal = Medical Anthropology Quarterly | volume = | issue = | pages = 22–40 | publisher = American Anthropological Association | location = Arlington, Virginia | year = 2007| issn = 0745-5194 | url = http://itb.biologie.hu-berlin.de/~hagen/papers/perinatal.pdf | accessdate = 2007-10-23 }}</ref><ref name="IsLowMood">{{cite journal | last = Keller | first = Matthew C. | authorlink = Matthew Keller | coauthors = ] | title = Is low mood an adaptation? Evidence for subtypes with symptoms that match precipitants | journal = Journal of Affective Disorders | volume = 86 | issue = 1 | pages = 27–35 | publisher = Elsevier | location = Amsterdam, New York | date = 2005-05 | url = http://www-personal.umich.edu/~nesse/Articles/Keller-Nesse-MoodSubtypes-JAD-2005.pdf | accessdate = 2007-10-23 | doi = 10.1016/j.jad.2004.12.005 | pmid = 15820268 }}</ref><ref name=Tooby1990>{{cite journal | author = Tooby, J. | coauthors = Cosmides, L. | year = 1990 | title = The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments | journal = Ethology and Sociobiology | volume = 11 | issue = 4–5 | pages = 375–424 | url = http://doi.apa.org/?uid=1991-03617-001 | accessdate = 2008-02-28 | doi = 10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z}}</ref><ref name=Hagen1999>{{cite journal | author = Hagen, E.H. | year = 1999 | title = The Functions of Postpartum Depression | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 20 | issue = 5 | pages = 325–359 | url = http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1090513899000161 | accessdate = 2008-02-28 | doi = 10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00016-1}}</ref><ref name=Nettle2004>{{cite journal | author = Nettle, D. | year = 2004 | title = Evolutionary origins of depression: a review and reformulation | journal = Journal of Affective Disorders | volume = 81 | issue = 2 | pages = 91–102 | url = http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0165032703002623 | accessdate = 2008-02-28 | doi = 10.1016/j.jad.2003.08.009 | pmid = 15306134}}</ref>


==Rank theory== ==Rank theory==
] is the hypothesis that, if an individual is involved in a lengthy fight for ] in a social group and is clearly losing, then depression causes the individual to back down and accept the ] role. In doing so, the individual is protected from unnecessary harm. In this way, depression helps maintain a ]. This theory is a special case of a more general theory derived from the psychic pain hypothesis: that the cognitive response that produces modern-day depression evolved as a mechanism that allows people to assess whether they are in pursuit of an unreachable ], and if they are, to motivate them to desist.<ref name="NeeseAdapt" /><ref>{{cite book|url=http://books.google.com/?id=toRX7Zyo_pMC|title=Depression: The Evolution of Powerlessness|author=Paul Gilbert|publisher=Psychology Press|year=1992|isbn=0863772218}}</ref> ] is the hypothesis that, if an individual is involved in a lengthy fight for ] in a social group and is clearly losing, then depression causes the individual to back down and accept the ] role. In doing so, the individual is protected from unnecessary harm. In this way, depression helps maintain a ]. This theory is a special case of a more general theory derived from the psychic pain hypothesis: that the cognitive response that produces modern-day depression evolved as a mechanism that allows people to assess whether they are in pursuit of an unreachable ], and if they are, to motivate them to desist.<ref name="NeeseAdapt" /><ref>{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=toRX7Zyo_pMC|title=Depression: The Evolution of Powerlessness|author=Paul Gilbert|publisher=Psychology Press|year=1992|isbn=978-0-86377-221-4}}</ref>


==Social risk hypothesis==
==Honest signaling theory==
This hypothesis is similar to the social rank hypothesis but focuses more on the importance of avoiding exclusion from social groups, rather than direct dominance contests. The fitness benefits of forming cooperative bonds with others have long been recognised—during the Pleistocene period, for instance, social ties were vital for food foraging and finding protection from predators.<ref name = "Allen and Badcock">{{cite journal |author1=Allen, N.B. |author2=Badcock, P.B. | year = 2006 | title = Darwinian models of depression: a review of evolutionary accounts of mood and mood disorders | journal = Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry| volume = 30 | pages = 815–826 | doi = 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.007 | pmid = 16647176 | issue = 5|s2cid=5954047 }}</ref>
Another reason depression is thought to be a pathology is that key symptoms, such as loss of interest in virtually all activities, are extremely costly to the sufferer. Biologists and economists have proposed, however, that ] can credibly signal information when there are ]. In the wake of a serious negative life event, such as those that have been implicated in depression (e.g., death, divorce), "cheap" signals of need, such as crying, might not be believed when social partners have conflicts of interest. The symptoms of major depression, such as loss of interest in virtually all activities and ], are inherently costly, but, as costly signaling theory requires, the costs differ for individuals in different states. For individuals who are not genuinely in need, the ] cost of major depression is very high because it threatens the flow of fitness benefits. For individuals who are in genuine need, however, the fitness cost of major depression is low, because the individual is not generating many fitness benefits. Thus, only an individual in genuine need can afford to suffer major depression. Major depression therefore serves as an honest, or credible, signal of need.


As such, depression is seen to represent an adaptive, risk-averse response to the threat of exclusion from social relationships that would have had a critical impact on the survival and reproductive success of our ancestors. Multiple lines of evidence on the mechanisms and phenomenology of depression suggest that mild to moderate (or "normative") depressed states preserve an individual's inclusion in key social contexts via three intersecting features: a cognitive sensitivity to social risks and situations (e.g., "depressive realism"); it inhibits confident and competitive behaviours that are likely to put the individual at further risk of conflict or exclusion (as indicated by symptoms such as low self-esteem and social withdrawal); and it results in signalling behaviours directed toward significant others to elicit more of their support (e.g., the so-called "cry for help").<ref name = "Allen and Badcock"/><ref name = Averill1968>{{cite journal | author = Averill, J.R. | year = 1968 | title = Grief: its nature and significance | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 70 | pages= 721–748 | doi = 10.1037/h0026824 | pmid = 4889573 | issue = 6}}</ref> According to this view, the severe cases of depression captured by clinical diagnoses reflect the maladaptive, dysregulation of this mechanism, which may partly be due to the uncertainty and competitiveness of the modern, globalised world.
For example, individuals suffering a severe loss such as the death of a ] are often in need of help and assistance from others. Such individuals who have few conflicts with their social partners are predicted to experience grief—a means, in part, to signal need to others. Such individuals who have many conflicts with their social partners, in contrast, are predicted to experience depression—a means, in part, to ''credibly'' signal need to others who might be skeptical that the need is genuine. ''(A theologian might say that depression is like the difference between attrition and contrition. If it doesn't hurt it doesn't work). '' Put in simple language, depression may function to ''enforce sincerity''.<ref name=Hagen2003>{{cite journal | author = Hagen, E.H. | year = 2003
| title = The Bargaining Model of Depression | journal = Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation | url = http://books.google.com/?id=aVh9jtWbG0wC&pg=PA95&dq=%22the+bargaining+model+of+depression%22
| accessdate = 2008-02-28 | publisher = MIT Press | isbn = 9780262083263}}</ref><ref name=Watson2002>{{cite journal | author = Watson, P.J. | coauthors = Andrews, P.W. | year = 2002 | title = Toward a revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of depression: the social navigation hypothesis | journal = Journal of Affective Disorders | volume = 72 | issue = 1 | pages = 1–14 | url = http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0165032701004591 | accessdate = 2008-02-28 | doi = 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00459-1 | pmid = 12204312}}</ref>


==Social navigation or niche change theory== ==Honest signaling theory==
{{See also|Signalling theory}}
The social navigation, bargaining, or niche change hypothesis <ref name=Watson2002/><ref name=WatsonWebPage></ref> suggests that depression, operationally defined as a combination of prolonged ] and ] or agitation, provides a focused sober perspective on socially imposed constraints hindering a person’s pursuit of major fitness enhancing projects. Simultaneously, publicly displayed symptoms, which reduce the depressive's ability to conduct basic life activities, serve as a ] of need; the signal's costliness for the depressive certifies its honesty. Finally, for social partners who find it uneconomical to respond helpfully to an honest signal of need, the same depressive symptoms also have the potential to extort relevant concessions and compromises. Depression’s extortionary power comes from the fact that it retards the flow of just those goods and services such partners have come to expect from the depressive under status quo socioeconomic arrangements.
Another reason depression is thought to be a ] is that key symptoms, such as loss of interest in virtually all activities, are extremely costly to them. Biologists and economists have proposed, however, that ] can credibly signal information when there are ].<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Gaffney |first1=Michael R. |last2=Adams |first2=Kai H. |last3=Syme |first3=Kristen L. |last4=Hagen |first4=Edward H. |date=2022-05-01 |title=Depression and suicidality as evolved credible signals of need in social conflicts |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513822000149 |journal=Evolution and Human Behavior |volume=43 |issue=3 |pages=242–256 |doi=10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.02.004 |bibcode=2022EHumB..43..242G |issn=1090-5138}}</ref> In the wake of a serious negative life event, such as those that have been implicated in depression (e.g., death, divorce), "cheap" signals of need, such as crying, might not be believed when social partners have conflicts of interest. The symptoms of major depression, such as loss of interest in virtually all activities and ], are inherently costly, but, as costly signaling theory requires, the costs differ for individuals in different states. For individuals who are not genuinely in need, the ] cost of major depression is very high because it threatens the flow of fitness benefits. For individuals who are in genuine need, however, the fitness cost of major depression is low, because the individual is not generating many fitness benefits. Thus, only an individual in genuine need can afford to have major depression. Major depression therefore serves as an honest, or credible, signal of need.


For example, individuals suffering a severe loss such as the death of a ] are often in need of help and assistance from others. Such individuals who have few conflicts with their social partners are predicted to experience grief—a means, in part, to signal need to others. Such individuals who have many conflicts with their social partners, in contrast, are predicted to experience depression—a means, in part, to ''credibly'' signal need to others who might be skeptical that the need is genuine.<ref name=Hagen2003>{{cite book | author = Hagen, E.H. | year = 2003
Thus depression may be a social ] especially useful in motivating a variety of social partners, all at once, to help the depressive initiate major fitness-enhancing changes in their socioeconomic life. There are diverse circumstances under which this may become necessary in human social life, ranging from loss of rank or a key social ally which makes the current social niche uneconomic to having a set of creative new ideas about how to make a livelihood which begs for a new niche. The social navigation hypothesis emphasizes that an individual can become tightly ensnared in an overly restrictive matrix of social exchange contracts, and that this situation sometimes necessitates a radical contractual upheaval that is beyond conventional methods of negotiation. Regarding the treatment of depression, this hypothesis calls into question any assumptions by the clinician that the typical cause of depression is related to ] perverted thinking processes or other purely endogenous sources. The social navigation hypothesis calls instead for analysis of the depressive’s talents and dreams, identification of relevant social constraints (especially those with a relatively diffuse non-point source within the social network of the depressive), and practical social ] ] designed to relax those constraints enough to allow the depressive to move forward with their life under an improved set of social contracts.<ref name=Watson2002/><ref name=WatsonWebPage/> This theory has been the subject of criticism.<ref name=Nettle2004/>.
| chapter = The Bargaining Model of Depression | journal = Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation | pages = 95–124
| chapter-url = https://books.google.com/books?id=aVh9jtWbG0wC&q=%22the+bargaining+model+of+depression%22&pg=PA95
| access-date = 2008-02-28 | publisher = MIT Press | doi = 10.7551/mitpress/3232.003.0008
| isbn = 978-0-262-08326-3| url = http://cogprints.org/2471/1/depression.pdf
}}</ref><ref name=Watson2002>{{cite journal | author = Watson, P.J. |author2=Andrews, P.W. | year = 2002 | title = Toward a revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of depression: the social navigation hypothesis | journal = Journal of Affective Disorders | volume = 72 | issue = 1 | pages = 1–14 | doi = 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00459-1 | pmid = 12204312}}</ref>


==Bargaining theory== ==Bargaining theory==
Depression is not only costly to the sufferer, it also imposes a significant burden on family, friends, and society at large—yet another reason it is thought to be pathological. Yet if sufferers of depression have real but unmet needs, they might have to provide an incentive to others to address those needs. Depression is not only costly to the affected person, it also imposes a significant burden on family, friends, and society at large—yet another reason it is thought to be pathological. Yet if people with depression have real but unmet needs, they might have to provide an incentive to others to address those needs.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Hammerstein |first=Peter |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=aVh9jtWbG0wC&dq=Bargaining+theory+depression&pg=PA95 |title=Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation |date=2003 |publisher=MIT Press |isbn=978-0-262-08326-3 |pages=95–97 |language=en}}</ref>


The bargaining theory of depression is similar to the honest signaling, niche change, and social navigation theories of depression described above. It draws on theories of labor strikes developed by economists to basically add one additional element to honest signaling theory: The fitness of social partners is generally correlated. When a wife suffers depression and reduces her investment in offspring, for example, the husband's fitness is also put at risk. Thus, not only do the symptoms of major depression serve as costly and therefore honest signals of need, they also compel reluctant social partners to respond to that need in order to prevent their own fitness from being reduced.<ref name=Hagen1999/><ref name=Hagen2003/><ref name=Hagen2002>{{cite journal | author = Hagen, E.H. | year = 2002 | title = Depression as bargaining The case postpartum | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 23 | issue = 5 | pages = 323–336 | url = http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1090513801001027 | accessdate = 2008-02-28 | doi = 10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00102-7}}</ref> The bargaining theory of depression is similar to the honest signaling, niche change, and social navigation theories of depression described below. It draws on theories of labor strikes developed by economists to basically add one additional element to honest signaling theory: The fitness of social partners is generally correlated. When a wife has depression and reduces her investment in offspring, for example, the husband's fitness is also put at risk. Thus, not only do the symptoms of major depression serve as costly and therefore honest signals of need, they also compel reluctant social partners to respond to that need in order to prevent their own fitness from being reduced.<ref name=Hagen1999/><ref name=Hagen2003/><ref name=Hagen2002>{{cite journal | author = Hagen, E.H. | year = 2002 | title = Depression as bargaining The case postpartum | journal = Evolution and Human Behavior | volume = 23 | issue = 5 | pages = 323–336 | doi = 10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00102-7| bibcode = 2002EHumB..23..323H }}</ref> This explanation for depression has been challenged. Depression decreases the joint product of the family or group as the husband or helper only partially compensates for the loss of productivity by the depressed person. Instead of being depressed the person could break their own leg and gain help from the social group, but this obviously is a counterproductive strategy. And the lack of a sex drive certainly does not improve marital relations or fitness.<ref name=Wittman2014/>

==Social navigation or niche change theory==
The social navigation or niche change hypothesis<ref name=Watson2002/><ref name=WatsonWebPage>{{Cite web |url=http://biology.unm.edu/pwatson/dp1.htm |title=Watson, Paul J. "An Evolutionary Adaptationist Theory of Unipolar Depression: Depression as an adaptation for social navigation, especially for overcoming costly, complex contractual constraints of the individual's social niche" |access-date=2017-07-03 |archive-date=2019-12-31 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20191231232747/http://biology.unm.edu/pwatson/dp1.htm |url-status=dead }}</ref> proposes that depression is a social navigation adaptation of last resort, designed especially to help individuals overcome costly, complex contractual constraints on their social niche. The hypothesis combines the analytical rumination and bargaining hypotheses and suggests that depression, operationally defined as a combination of prolonged ] and ] or ], provides a focused sober perspective on socially imposed constraints hindering a person's pursuit of major fitness enhancing projects. Simultaneously, publicly displayed symptoms, which reduce the depressive's ability to conduct basic life activities, serve as a ] of need; the signal's costliness for the depressive certifies its honesty. Finally, for social partners who find it uneconomical to respond helpfully to an honest signal of need, the same depressive symptoms also have the potential to extort relevant concessions and compromises. Depression's extortionary power comes from the fact that it slows the flow of just those goods and services such partners have come to expect from the depressive under status quo socioeconomic arrangements.

Thus depression may be a social ] especially useful in motivating a variety of social partners, all at once, to help the depressive initiate major fitness-enhancing changes in their socioeconomic life. There are diverse circumstances under which this may become necessary in human social life, ranging from loss of rank or a key social ally which makes the current social niche uneconomic to having a set of creative new ideas about how to make a livelihood which begs for a new niche. The social navigation hypothesis emphasizes that an individual can become tightly ensnared in an overly restrictive matrix of social exchange contracts, and that this situation sometimes necessitates a radical contractual upheaval that is beyond conventional methods of negotiation. Regarding the treatment of depression, this hypothesis calls into question any assumptions by the clinician that the typical cause of depression is related to ] perverted thinking processes or other purely endogenous sources. The social navigation hypothesis calls instead for analysis of the depressive's talents and dreams, identification of relevant social constraints (especially those with a relatively diffuse non-point source within the social network of the depressive), and practical social ] ] designed to relax those constraints enough to allow the depressive to move forward with their life under an improved set of social contracts.<ref name=Watson2002/><ref name=WatsonWebPage/> This theory has been the subject of criticism.<ref name=Nettle2004/>

==Depression as an incentive device==
This approach argues that being in a depressed state is not adaptive (indeed quite the opposite), but the threat of depression for bad outcomes and the promise of pleasure for good outcomes are adaptive because they motivate the individual toward undertaking effort that increase fitness.<ref name=Wittman2014/> The reason for not relying on pleasure alone as an incentive device is because happiness is costly in terms of fitness as the individual becomes less cautious. This is most readily seen when an individual is manic and undertakes very risky behavior. The physiological manifestation of the incentives are most noticeable when an individual is bipolar with bouts of extreme elation and extreme depression as anxiety which is about the (possibly immediate) future is highly correlated with being bipolar.<ref name=Wittman2014/> As noted earlier, bipolar disorder and clinical depression, as opposed to event depression, are viewed as dysregulation just as persistently high (or low) blood pressure are viewed as dysregulation even though at times high or low blood pressure is fitness enhancing.


==Prevention of infection== ==Prevention of infection==
It has been hypothesized that depression is an evolutionary adaptation because it helps prevent infection in both the affected individual and his/her kin.<ref name = "Kinney and Tanaka">{{cite journal | author = Kinney, D.K.; Tanaka, M. | year = 2009 | title = An evolutionary hypothesis of depression and its symptoms, adaptive values, and risk factors | journal = Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease | volume = 197 | pages = 561–567 | doi = 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181b05fa8 | pmid = 19684491 | issue = 8}}</ref> It has been hypothesized that depression is an evolutionary adaptation because it helps prevent ] in both the affected individual and their kin.<ref name = "Kinney and Tanaka">{{cite journal |author1=Kinney, D.K. |author2=Tanaka, M. | year = 2009 | title = An evolutionary hypothesis of depression and its symptoms, adaptive values, and risk factors | journal = Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease | volume = 197 | pages = 561–567 | doi = 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181b05fa8 | pmid = 19684491 | issue = 8|s2cid=205881413 }}</ref><ref>Raison, C.L, Miller, A. N. (2012). The evolutionary significance of depression in Pathogen Host Defense (PATHOS-D) Molecular Psychiatry 1-23. .</ref>


First, the associated symptoms of depression, such as inactivity and lethargy, encourage the affected individual to rest. Energy conserved through such methods is highly crucial, as immune activation against infections is relatively costly; there must be, for instance, a 10% increase in metabolic activity for even a 1℃ change in body temperature.<ref name = Kluger1991>{{cite journal | author = Kluger, M.J. | year = 1991 | title = Fever: Role of pyrogens and cryogens | journal = Physiological Review | volume = 71 | pages = 93–127}}</ref> Therefore, depression allows one to conserve and allocate energy to the immune system more efficiently. First, the associated symptoms of depression, such as inactivity and lethargy, encourage the affected individual to rest. Energy conserved through such methods is highly crucial, as immune activation against infections is relatively costly; there must be, for instance, a 10% increase in metabolic activity for even a 1°C change in body temperature.<ref name = Kluger1991>{{cite journal | author = Kluger, M.J. | year = 1991 | title = Fever: Role of pyrogens and cryogens | journal = Physiological Reviews | volume = 71 | issue = 1 | pages = 93–127 | doi=10.1152/physrev.1991.71.1.93| pmid = 1986393 | pmc = 7191625 }}</ref> Therefore, depression allows one to conserve and allocate energy to the immune system more efficiently.


Depression further prevents infection by discouraging social interactions and activities that may result in exchange of infections. For example, the loss of interest discourages one from engaging in sexual activity, which, in turn, prevents the exchange of sexually transmitted diseases. Similarly, depressed mothers may interact less with their children, reducing the probability of the mother infecting her kin.<ref name = "Kinney and Tanaka"/> Depression further prevents infection by discouraging social interactions and activities that may result in exchange of infections. For example, the loss of interest discourages one from engaging in sexual activity, which, in turn, prevents the exchange of sexually transmitted diseases. Similarly, depressed mothers may interact less with their children, reducing the probability of the mother infecting her kin.<ref name = "Kinney and Tanaka"/>
Lastly, the lack of appetite associated with depression may also reduce exposure to food-borne parasites.<ref name = "Kinney and Tanaka"/> Lastly, the lack of appetite associated with depression may also reduce exposure to food-borne parasites.<ref name = "Kinney and Tanaka"/>


However, it should also be noted that chronic illness itself may be involved in causing depression. In animal models, the prolonged overreaction of the immune system, in response to the strain of chronic disease, results in an increased production of ] (a diverse group of hormonal regulators and ]). Cytokines interact with ] systems—mainly ], ], and ], and induce depressive characteristics. The onset of depression may help an individual recover from their illness by allowing them a more reserved, safe and energetically efficient lifestyle. The overproduction of these cytokines, beyond optimal levels due to the repeated demands of dealing with a chronic disease, may result in clinical depression and its accompanying behavioral manifestations that promote extreme energy reservation.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Dantzer|first1=E|title=Cytokines and depression: an update|journal=Brain, Behavior, and Immunity|year=2002|volume=16|issue=5|pages=501–502|doi=10.1016/s0889-1591(02)00002-8|pmid=12401463|s2cid=33116478}}</ref>
==Analytical rumination hypothesis==
This hypothesis suggests that depression is an adaptation that causes the affected individual to concentrate his or her attention and focus on a complex problem in order to analyze and solve it.<ref name = Andrews2009>{{cite journal | author = Andrews, P.W.; Thompson, J.A. | year = 2009 | title = The bright side of being blue: depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems | journal = Psychological Review | volume = 116 | pages = 620–654 | doi = 10.1037/a0016242 | pmid = 19618990 | issue = 3 | pmc = 2734449}}</ref>


==The third ventricle hypothesis==
One way depression increases the individual’s focus on a problem is by inducing rumination. Depression activates the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which increases attention control and maintains problem-related information in an “active, accessible state” referred to as “working memory, or WM. As a result, depressed individuals have been shown to ruminate, reflecting on the reasons for their current problems. Feelings of regret associated with depression also cause individuals to reflect and analyze past events in order to determine why they happened and how they could have been prevented.<ref name = Andrews2009/>
]
The ] hypothesis of depression proposes that the behavioural cluster associated with depression (hunched posture, avoidance of eye contact, reduced appetites for food and sex plus social withdrawal and sleep disturbance) serves to reduce an individual's attack-provoking stimuli within the context of a chronically hostile social environment.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Hendrie CA, Pickles AR | year = 2010 | title = Depression as an evolutionary adaptation: Implications for the development of new drug treatments | journal = European Psychiatric Review | volume = 3 | page = 46 }}</ref><ref>Hendrie CA, Pickles AR (2011) Depression: An evolutionary adaptation organised around the third ventricle In: Brinkworth M, Weinert F (eds) Darwinian Repercussions Darwinism in anInterdisciplinary Context Heidelberg, New York, London: Springer</ref><ref>{{cite journal | author = Hendrie CA, Pickles AR | year = 2012 | title = The failure of the antidepressant drug discovery process is systemic | journal = J Psychopharm | volume = 27| issue = 5| pages = 407-13; discussion 413-6 | doi = 10.1177/0269881112466185 | pmid = 23222042 | s2cid = 24171469 }}</ref> It further proposes that this response is mediated by the acute release of an unknown inflammatory agent (probably ]) into the third ventricular space. In support of this suggestion, imaging studies reveal that the third ventricle is enlarged in depressives.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Baumann B, Bornschlegl C, Krell D, Bogerts B | year = 1997 | title = Changes in CSF spaces differ in endogenous and neurotic depression A planimetric CT scan study | journal = J Affect Disord | volume = 45 | issue = 3| pages = 179–88 | doi = 10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00073-6 | pmid = 9298431 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author1=Cousins D. A. |author2=Brian Moore P. |author3=Watson S. |author4=Harrison L. |author5=Nicol Ferrier I. |author6=Young A. H. |author7=Lloyd A. J. | year = 2010 | title = Pituitary volume and third ventricle width in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder | journal = Psychoneuroendocrinology | volume = 35 | issue = 7| pages = 1074–1081 | doi=10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.01.008 | pmid=20171783|s2cid=24962012 }}</ref>


== Reception ==
Another way depression increases an individual’s ability to concentrate on a problem is by reducing distraction from the problem. For example, anhedonia, which is often associated with depression, decreases an individual’s desire to participate in activities that provide short-term rewards, and instead, allows the individual to concentrate on long-term goals. In addition, “psychomotory changes, such as solitariness, decreased appetite, and insomnia also reduce distractions. For instance, insomnia enables conscious analysis of the problem to be maintained by preventing sleep from disrupting such processes. Likewise, solitariness, lack of physical activity, and lack of appetite all eliminate sources of distraction, such as social interactions, navigation through the environment,<ref name = Andrews2009/> and “oral activity, which disrupt stimuli from being processed.<ref name = Jacobs1999>{{cite journal | author = Jacobs, B.L.; Fornal, C.A. | year = 1999 | title = Activity of serotonergic neurons in behaving animals | journal = Neuropsychopharmacology | volume = 21 | page= S9–S15 | doi= 10.1038/sj.npp.1395336 }}</ref>


Clinical psychology and psychiatry have historically been relatively isolated from the field of evolutionary psychology.<ref name = "Brune 2006">{{cite journal | journal=]|doi=10.1017/S0140525X06259090 | title=Evolutionary psychiatry is dead – Long liveth evolutionary psychopathology | year=2006 | last1=Brüne | first1=Martin | volume=29 | issue=4 |page=408 }}</ref> Some psychiatrists raise the concern that evolutionary psychologists seek to explain hidden adaptive advantages without engaging the rigorous empirical testing required to back up such claims.<ref name = "Brune 2006"/><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Douzenis|first1=A |last2=Seretis|first2=D |last3=Rizos|first3=E |last4=Michopoulos|first4=I |last5=Christodoulou|first5=C |last6=Lykouras|first6=L |year=2010 |journal=British Journal of Psychiatry |title=Evolution and Psychiatry |volume=196 |issue=3 |pages=246–47 |pmid=20194552 |doi=10.1192/bjp.196.3.246a |doi-access=free }}</ref> While there is strong research to suggest a genetic link to bipolar disorder and schizophrenia, there is significant debate within clinical psychology about the relative influence and the mediating role of cultural or environmental factors.<ref>Adams, H. and Sutker, P. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology, 3rd Ed. Springer. 2001. Chapter 3, pp. 53–84.</ref> For example, epidemiological research suggests that different cultural groups may have divergent rates of diagnosis, symptomatology, and expression of mental illnesses.<ref name="Adams, H 2001. pp. 105–27">Adams, H. and Sutker, P. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology, 3rd Ed. Springer. 2001. Chapter 5, pp. 105–27</ref> There has also been increasing acknowledgment of culture-bound disorders,<ref name="Adams, H 2001. pp. 105–27"/><ref>American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). 2000. Culture Bound Syndromes, pp. 897–903.</ref> which may be viewed as an argument for an environmental versus genetic psychological adaptation.<ref>Schumaker, J. The Age of Insanity: Modernity and Mental Health. Praeger. 2001. Chapter 3, pp. 29–49.</ref> While certain mental disorders may have psychological traits that can be explained as 'adaptive' on an evolutionary scale, these disorders cause individuals significant emotional and psychological distress and negatively influence the stability of interpersonal relationships and day-to-day adaptive functioning.<ref>Adams, H. and Sutker, P. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology, 3rd Ed. Springer. 2001</ref>
==Social risk hypothesis==
This hypothesis explains the evolutionary origin of depression in the ancestral context in which depression was an adaptation that enabled an individual to maintain social ties critical for survival and reproduction. During the Pleistocene period, for instance, such social ties were vital in food foraging and protection from predators.<ref name = "Allen and Badcock">{{cite journal | author = Allen, N.B.; Badcock, P.B. | year = 2006 | title = Darwinian models of depression: a review of evolutionary accounts of mood and mood disorders | journal = Progress in neuro-psychopharmacology & biological psychology | volume = 30 | pages = 815–826 | doi = 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.007 | pmid = 16647176 | issue = 5}}</ref>


== See also ==
Depression, resulting from one’s loss of “Social Attention Holding Power,” or SAHP, may have signalled the individual's submissiveness to the more dominant males of the social group through associated symptoms, such as low confidence and anxiety.<ref name = "Allen and Badcock"/> This, in turn, reduced tension among group members, and enabled bonds to be maintained. Secondly, depression may also have served to signal other members of an individual’s need for aid <ref name = "Allen and Badcock"/> and desire to reform lost bonds.<ref name = Averill1968>{{cite journal | author = Averill, J.R. | year = 1968 | title = Grief: its nature and significance | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 70 | pages= 721–748 | doi = 10.1037/h0026824 | pmid = 4889573 | issue = 6}}</ref>
* ]


'''General:'''
==Behavioral shutdown model==
* ]
If an organism faces more risk or expenditure than reward from activities, the best evolutionary strategy may be to withdraw from them. The behavioral shutdown model proposes that emotional pain, like physical pain, serves a useful adaptive purpose. Negative emotions like disappointment, sadness, grief, fear, anxiety, anger, and guilt are described as "evolved strategies that allow for the identification and avoidance of specific problems, especially in the social domain." Depression is characteristically associated with ] and lack of energy, and those experiencing it are risk-aversive and perceive more negative and pessimistic outcomes because they are focused on preventing further loss. Although the model views depression as an adaptive response, it does not suggest that it is beneficial by the standards of current society; but it does suggest that many approaches to depression treat symptoms rather than causes, and underlying social problems need to be addressed.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://psychweb.cisat.jmu.edu/ToKSystem/My%20ToK%20Papers/BSM%20Final.pdf|title=Depression: disease or behavioral shutdown mechanism|author=Gregg Henriques|publisher=Journal of Science and Health Policy}}</ref>
* ]


== Videos ==
==Possibilities of depression as a dysregulated adaptation==
*
Depression, especially in the modern context, may not necessarily be adaptive. The ability to feel pain and experience depression, are adaptive defense mechanisms,<ref name = Gilbert2006>{{cite journal|author=Gilbert, P. |year=2006|title= Evolution and depression: issues and implications|journal= Psychological Medicine |volume=36|pages=287–297|doi=10.1017/S0033291705006112|pmid=16236231|issue=3}}</ref> but when they are “too easily triggered, too intense, or long lasting, they can become “dysregulated.<ref name=Gilbert2006/> In such a case, defense mechanisms, too, can become diseases, such as “chronic pain or dehydration from diarrhea. Depression, which may be a similar kind of defense mechanism, may have become dysregulated as well.<ref name = Nesse2000>{{cite journal | author = Nesse, R.M. | year = 2000 | title = Is Depression an Adaptation? | journal = Archives of General Psychology | volume = 57 | issue = 1 | pages= 14–20 | doi = 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14 | pmid=10632228}}</ref>


==References== ==References==
{{Reflist}} {{reflist|30em}}


{{Evolutionary psychology|state=expanded}} {{Evolutionary psychology|state=expanded}}


] ]
] ]

Latest revision as of 06:43, 9 December 2024

Evolutionary approaches to depression are attempts by evolutionary psychologists and evolutionary psychiatrists to use the theory of evolution to further understand mood disorders. Depression is generally thought of as dysfunction or a mental disorder, but its prevalence does not increase with age the way dementia and other organic dysfunction commonly does. Some researchers have surmised that the disorder may have evolutionary roots, in the same way that others suggest evolutionary contributions to schizophrenia, sickle cell anemia, psychopathy and other disorders. The proposed explanations for the evolution of depression remain controversial.

Background

This section may require cleanup to meet Misplaced Pages's quality standards. The specific problem is: This reads too much like a textbook. Please help improve this section if you can. (November 2024) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

Major depression (also called "major depressive disorder", "clinical depression" or often simply "depression") is a leading cause of disability worldwide, and in 2000 was the fourth leading contributor to the global burden of disease (measured in DALYs); it is also an important risk factor for suicide. It is understandable, then, that clinical depression is thought to be a pathology—a major dysfunction of the brain.

In most cases, rates of organ dysfunction increase with age, with low rates in adolescents and young adults, and the highest rates in the elderly. These patterns are consistent with evolutionary theories of aging which posit that selection against dysfunctional traits decreases with age (because there is a decreasing probability of surviving to later ages).

In contrast to these patterns, prevalence of clinical depression is high in all age categories, including otherwise healthy adolescents and young adults. In one study of the US population, for example, the 12 month prevalence for a major depression episode was highest in the youngest age category (15- to 24-year-olds). The high prevalence of unipolar depression (excluding depression associated bipolar disorder) is also an outlier when compared to the prevalence of other mental disorders such as major intellectual disability, autism, schizophrenia and even the aforementioned bipolar disorder, all with prevalence rates about one tenth that of depression, or less. As of 2017, the only mental disorders with a higher prevalence than depression are anxiety disorders.

The common occurrence and persistence of a trait like clinical depression with such negative effects early in life is difficult to explain. (Rates of infectious disease are high in young people, of course, but clinical depression is not thought to be caused by an infection.) Evolutionary psychology and its application in evolutionary medicine suggest how behaviour and mental states, including seemingly harmful states such as depression, may have been beneficial adaptations of human ancestors which improved the fitness of individuals or their relatives. It has been argued, for example, that Abraham Lincoln's lifelong depression was a source of insight and strength. Some even suggest that "we aren't designed to have happiness as our natural default" and so a state of depression is the evolutionary norm.

The following hypotheses attempt to identify a benefit of depression that outweighs its obvious costs.

Such hypotheses are not necessarily incompatible with one another and may explain different aspects, causes, and symptoms of depression.

Psychic pain hypothesis

One reason depression is thought to be a pathology is that it causes so much psychic pain and distress. However, physical pain is also very distressful, yet it has an evolved function: to inform the organism that it is being damaged, to motivate it to withdraw from the source of damage, and to learn to avoid such damage-causing circumstances in the future. Sadness is also distressing, yet is widely believed to be an evolved adaptation. In fact, perhaps the most influential evolutionary view is that most cases of depression are simply particularly intense cases of sadness in response to adversity, such as the loss of a loved one.

According to the psychic pain hypothesis, depression is analogous to physical pain in that it informs them that current circumstances, such as the loss of a friend, are imposing a threat to biological fitness. It motivates them to cease activities that led to the costly situation, if possible, and it causes him or her to learn to avoid similar circumstances in the future. Proponents of this view tend to focus on low mood, and regard clinical depression as a dysfunctional extreme of low mood—and not as a unique set of characteristics that are physiologically distanced from regular depressed mood.

Alongside the absence of pleasure, other noticeable changes include psychomotor retardation, disrupted patterns of sleeping and feeding, a loss of sex drive and motivation—which are all also characteristics of the body's reaction to actual physical pain. In depressed people there is an increased activity in the regions of the cortex involved with the perception of pain, such as the anterior cingulate cortex and the left prefrontal cortex. This activity allows the cortex to manifest an abstract negative thought as a true physical stressor to the rest of the brain.

Behavioral shutdown model

The behavioral shutdown model states that if an organism faces more risk or expenditure than reward from activities, the best evolutionary strategy may be to withdraw from them. This model proposes that emotional pain, like physical pain, serves a useful adaptive purpose. Negative emotions like disappointment, sadness, grief, fear, anxiety, anger, and guilt are described as "evolved strategies that allow for the identification and avoidance of specific problems, especially in the social domain." Depression is characteristically associated with anhedonia and lack of energy, and those experiencing it are risk-aversive and perceive more negative and pessimistic outcomes because they are focused on preventing further loss. Although the model views depression as an adaptive response, it does not suggest that it is beneficial by the standards of current society; but it does suggest that many approaches to depression treat symptoms rather than causes, and underlying social problems need to be addressed.

A related phenomenon to the behavioral shutdown model is learned helplessness. In animal subjects, a loss of control or predictability in the subject's experiences results in a condition similar to clinical depression in humans. That is to say, if uncontrollable and unstoppable stressors are repeated for long enough, a rat subject will adopt a learned helplessness, which shares a number of behavioral and psychological features with human depression. The subject will not attempt to cope with problems, even when placed in a stressor-free novel environment. Should their rare attempts at coping prove successful in a new environment, a long lasting cognitive block prevents them from perceiving their action as useful and their coping strategy does not last long. From an evolutionary perspective, learned helplessness also allows a conservation of energy for an extended period of time should people find themselves in a predicament that is outside of their control, such as an illness or a dry season. However, for today's humans whose depression resembles learned helplessness, this phenomenon usually manifests as a loss of motivation and the distortion of one uncontrollable aspect of a person's life being viewed as representative of all aspects of their life – suggesting a mismatch between ultimate cause and modern manifestation.

Analytical rumination hypothesis

This hypothesis suggests that depression is an adaptation that causes the affected individual to concentrate his or her attention and focus on a complex problem in order to analyze and solve it.

One way depression increases the individual's focus on a problem is by inducing rumination. Depression activates the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which increases attention control and maintains problem-related information in an "active, accessible state" referred to as "working memory", or WM. As a result, depressed individuals have been shown to ruminate, reflecting on the reasons for their current problems. Feelings of regret associated with depression also cause individuals to reflect and analyze past events in order to determine why they happened and how they could have been prevented. The rumination hypothesis has come under criticism. Evolutionary fitness is increased by ruminating before rather than after bad outcomes. A situation that resulted in a child being in danger but unharmed should lead the parent to ruminate on how to avoid the dangerous situation in the future. Waiting until the child dies and then ruminating in a state of depression is too late.

Some cognitive psychologists argue that ruminative tendency itself increases the likelihood of the onset of depression.

Another way depression increases an individual's ability to concentrate on a problem is by reducing distraction from the problem. For example, anhedonia, which is often associated with depression, decreases an individual's desire to participate in activities that provide short-term rewards, and instead, allows the individual to concentrate on long-term goals. In addition, "psychomotoric changes", such as solitariness, decreased appetite, and insomnia also reduce distractions. For instance, insomnia enables conscious analysis of the problem to be maintained by preventing sleep from disrupting such processes. Likewise, solitariness, lack of physical activity, and lack of appetite all eliminate sources of distraction, such as social interactions, navigation through the environment, and "oral activity", which disrupt stimuli from being processed.

Possibilities of depression as a dysregulated adaptation

Depression, especially in the modern context, may not necessarily be adaptive. The ability to feel pain and experience depression, are adaptive defense mechanisms, but when they are "too easily triggered, too intense, or long lasting", they can become "dysregulated". In such a case, defense mechanisms, too, can become diseases, such as "chronic pain or dehydration from diarrhea". Depression, which may be a similar kind of defense mechanism, may have become dysregulated as well.

Thus, unlike other evolutionary theories this one sees depression as a maladaptive extreme of something that is beneficial in smaller amounts. In particular, one theory focuses on the personality trait neuroticism. Low amounts of neuroticism may increase a person's fitness through various processes, but too much may reduce fitness by, for example, recurring depressions. Thus, evolution will select for an optimal amount and most people will have neuroticism near this amount. However, genetic variation continually occurs, and some people will have high neuroticism which increases the risk of depressions.

Rank theory

Rank theory is the hypothesis that, if an individual is involved in a lengthy fight for dominance in a social group and is clearly losing, then depression causes the individual to back down and accept the submissive role. In doing so, the individual is protected from unnecessary harm. In this way, depression helps maintain a social hierarchy. This theory is a special case of a more general theory derived from the psychic pain hypothesis: that the cognitive response that produces modern-day depression evolved as a mechanism that allows people to assess whether they are in pursuit of an unreachable goal, and if they are, to motivate them to desist.

Social risk hypothesis

This hypothesis is similar to the social rank hypothesis but focuses more on the importance of avoiding exclusion from social groups, rather than direct dominance contests. The fitness benefits of forming cooperative bonds with others have long been recognised—during the Pleistocene period, for instance, social ties were vital for food foraging and finding protection from predators.

As such, depression is seen to represent an adaptive, risk-averse response to the threat of exclusion from social relationships that would have had a critical impact on the survival and reproductive success of our ancestors. Multiple lines of evidence on the mechanisms and phenomenology of depression suggest that mild to moderate (or "normative") depressed states preserve an individual's inclusion in key social contexts via three intersecting features: a cognitive sensitivity to social risks and situations (e.g., "depressive realism"); it inhibits confident and competitive behaviours that are likely to put the individual at further risk of conflict or exclusion (as indicated by symptoms such as low self-esteem and social withdrawal); and it results in signalling behaviours directed toward significant others to elicit more of their support (e.g., the so-called "cry for help"). According to this view, the severe cases of depression captured by clinical diagnoses reflect the maladaptive, dysregulation of this mechanism, which may partly be due to the uncertainty and competitiveness of the modern, globalised world.

Honest signaling theory

See also: Signalling theory

Another reason depression is thought to be a pathology is that key symptoms, such as loss of interest in virtually all activities, are extremely costly to them. Biologists and economists have proposed, however, that signals with inherent costs can credibly signal information when there are conflicts of interest. In the wake of a serious negative life event, such as those that have been implicated in depression (e.g., death, divorce), "cheap" signals of need, such as crying, might not be believed when social partners have conflicts of interest. The symptoms of major depression, such as loss of interest in virtually all activities and suicidality, are inherently costly, but, as costly signaling theory requires, the costs differ for individuals in different states. For individuals who are not genuinely in need, the fitness cost of major depression is very high because it threatens the flow of fitness benefits. For individuals who are in genuine need, however, the fitness cost of major depression is low, because the individual is not generating many fitness benefits. Thus, only an individual in genuine need can afford to have major depression. Major depression therefore serves as an honest, or credible, signal of need.

For example, individuals suffering a severe loss such as the death of a spouse are often in need of help and assistance from others. Such individuals who have few conflicts with their social partners are predicted to experience grief—a means, in part, to signal need to others. Such individuals who have many conflicts with their social partners, in contrast, are predicted to experience depression—a means, in part, to credibly signal need to others who might be skeptical that the need is genuine.

Bargaining theory

Depression is not only costly to the affected person, it also imposes a significant burden on family, friends, and society at large—yet another reason it is thought to be pathological. Yet if people with depression have real but unmet needs, they might have to provide an incentive to others to address those needs.

The bargaining theory of depression is similar to the honest signaling, niche change, and social navigation theories of depression described below. It draws on theories of labor strikes developed by economists to basically add one additional element to honest signaling theory: The fitness of social partners is generally correlated. When a wife has depression and reduces her investment in offspring, for example, the husband's fitness is also put at risk. Thus, not only do the symptoms of major depression serve as costly and therefore honest signals of need, they also compel reluctant social partners to respond to that need in order to prevent their own fitness from being reduced. This explanation for depression has been challenged. Depression decreases the joint product of the family or group as the husband or helper only partially compensates for the loss of productivity by the depressed person. Instead of being depressed the person could break their own leg and gain help from the social group, but this obviously is a counterproductive strategy. And the lack of a sex drive certainly does not improve marital relations or fitness.

Social navigation or niche change theory

The social navigation or niche change hypothesis proposes that depression is a social navigation adaptation of last resort, designed especially to help individuals overcome costly, complex contractual constraints on their social niche. The hypothesis combines the analytical rumination and bargaining hypotheses and suggests that depression, operationally defined as a combination of prolonged anhedonia and psychomotor retardation or agitation, provides a focused sober perspective on socially imposed constraints hindering a person's pursuit of major fitness enhancing projects. Simultaneously, publicly displayed symptoms, which reduce the depressive's ability to conduct basic life activities, serve as a social signal of need; the signal's costliness for the depressive certifies its honesty. Finally, for social partners who find it uneconomical to respond helpfully to an honest signal of need, the same depressive symptoms also have the potential to extort relevant concessions and compromises. Depression's extortionary power comes from the fact that it slows the flow of just those goods and services such partners have come to expect from the depressive under status quo socioeconomic arrangements.

Thus depression may be a social adaptation especially useful in motivating a variety of social partners, all at once, to help the depressive initiate major fitness-enhancing changes in their socioeconomic life. There are diverse circumstances under which this may become necessary in human social life, ranging from loss of rank or a key social ally which makes the current social niche uneconomic to having a set of creative new ideas about how to make a livelihood which begs for a new niche. The social navigation hypothesis emphasizes that an individual can become tightly ensnared in an overly restrictive matrix of social exchange contracts, and that this situation sometimes necessitates a radical contractual upheaval that is beyond conventional methods of negotiation. Regarding the treatment of depression, this hypothesis calls into question any assumptions by the clinician that the typical cause of depression is related to maladaptive perverted thinking processes or other purely endogenous sources. The social navigation hypothesis calls instead for analysis of the depressive's talents and dreams, identification of relevant social constraints (especially those with a relatively diffuse non-point source within the social network of the depressive), and practical social problem-solving therapy designed to relax those constraints enough to allow the depressive to move forward with their life under an improved set of social contracts. This theory has been the subject of criticism.

Depression as an incentive device

This approach argues that being in a depressed state is not adaptive (indeed quite the opposite), but the threat of depression for bad outcomes and the promise of pleasure for good outcomes are adaptive because they motivate the individual toward undertaking effort that increase fitness. The reason for not relying on pleasure alone as an incentive device is because happiness is costly in terms of fitness as the individual becomes less cautious. This is most readily seen when an individual is manic and undertakes very risky behavior. The physiological manifestation of the incentives are most noticeable when an individual is bipolar with bouts of extreme elation and extreme depression as anxiety which is about the (possibly immediate) future is highly correlated with being bipolar. As noted earlier, bipolar disorder and clinical depression, as opposed to event depression, are viewed as dysregulation just as persistently high (or low) blood pressure are viewed as dysregulation even though at times high or low blood pressure is fitness enhancing.

Prevention of infection

It has been hypothesized that depression is an evolutionary adaptation because it helps prevent infection in both the affected individual and their kin.

First, the associated symptoms of depression, such as inactivity and lethargy, encourage the affected individual to rest. Energy conserved through such methods is highly crucial, as immune activation against infections is relatively costly; there must be, for instance, a 10% increase in metabolic activity for even a 1°C change in body temperature. Therefore, depression allows one to conserve and allocate energy to the immune system more efficiently.

Depression further prevents infection by discouraging social interactions and activities that may result in exchange of infections. For example, the loss of interest discourages one from engaging in sexual activity, which, in turn, prevents the exchange of sexually transmitted diseases. Similarly, depressed mothers may interact less with their children, reducing the probability of the mother infecting her kin. Lastly, the lack of appetite associated with depression may also reduce exposure to food-borne parasites.

However, it should also be noted that chronic illness itself may be involved in causing depression. In animal models, the prolonged overreaction of the immune system, in response to the strain of chronic disease, results in an increased production of cytokines (a diverse group of hormonal regulators and signaling molecules). Cytokines interact with neurotransmitter systems—mainly norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin, and induce depressive characteristics. The onset of depression may help an individual recover from their illness by allowing them a more reserved, safe and energetically efficient lifestyle. The overproduction of these cytokines, beyond optimal levels due to the repeated demands of dealing with a chronic disease, may result in clinical depression and its accompanying behavioral manifestations that promote extreme energy reservation.

The third ventricle hypothesis

Third ventricle

The third ventricle hypothesis of depression proposes that the behavioural cluster associated with depression (hunched posture, avoidance of eye contact, reduced appetites for food and sex plus social withdrawal and sleep disturbance) serves to reduce an individual's attack-provoking stimuli within the context of a chronically hostile social environment. It further proposes that this response is mediated by the acute release of an unknown inflammatory agent (probably cytokine) into the third ventricular space. In support of this suggestion, imaging studies reveal that the third ventricle is enlarged in depressives.

Reception

Clinical psychology and psychiatry have historically been relatively isolated from the field of evolutionary psychology. Some psychiatrists raise the concern that evolutionary psychologists seek to explain hidden adaptive advantages without engaging the rigorous empirical testing required to back up such claims. While there is strong research to suggest a genetic link to bipolar disorder and schizophrenia, there is significant debate within clinical psychology about the relative influence and the mediating role of cultural or environmental factors. For example, epidemiological research suggests that different cultural groups may have divergent rates of diagnosis, symptomatology, and expression of mental illnesses. There has also been increasing acknowledgment of culture-bound disorders, which may be viewed as an argument for an environmental versus genetic psychological adaptation. While certain mental disorders may have psychological traits that can be explained as 'adaptive' on an evolutionary scale, these disorders cause individuals significant emotional and psychological distress and negatively influence the stability of interpersonal relationships and day-to-day adaptive functioning.

See also

General:

Videos

References

  1. "The World Health Report 2001". Health Systems: Improving Performance. Geneva: World Health Organization. 2001.
  2. "Prevalence of Heart Disease --- United States, 2005". Retrieved 2008-03-22."Prevalence of Chronic Kidney Disease and Associated Risk Factors --- United States, 1999--2004". Retrieved 2008-03-22.
  3. Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz M, Blazer DG, Nelson CB (1993). "Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. I: Lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence" (PDF). J Affect Disord. 29 (2–3): 85–96. doi:10.1016/0165-0327(93)90026-G. hdl:2027.42/30529. PMID 8300981.
  4. Epidemiologic Catchment Area, National Comorbidity Study
  5. Ritchie, Hannah; Roser, Max (2018). "Mental Health". Our World in Data. Global Change Data Lab.
  6. Neese, Randolph M. (October 2005). The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Mood Disorders - Chapter 10: Evolutionary Explanations for Mood and Mood Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. pp. 159–175. ISBN 978-1-58562-151-4. Retrieved 2007-10-23.
  7. Erica Goode (February 1, 2000). "Viewing Depression As Tool for Survival". New York Times.
  8. Natasha Mitchell (3 April 2004). "The Evolution of Depression – Does it Have a Role?". Australian Broadcasting Corporation. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  9. Julia M. Klein (12 February 2007). "Depression as a survival tool? Some new treatments assume so". LA Times.
  10. Joshua Wolf Shenk (2005). Lincoln's Melancholy: How Depression Challenged a President and Fueled His Greatness. Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 9780618551163.
  11. John Naish (January 19, 2008). "Why the happiness industry can only lead to misery". The Times. London. Archived from the original on June 15, 2011.
  12. ^ Allen, N.B.; Badcock, P.B. (2006). "Darwinian models of depression: a review of evolutionary accounts of mood and mood disorders". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 30 (5): 815–826. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.007. PMID 16647176. S2CID 5954047.
  13. Allan V. Horwitz; Jerome C. Wakefield (18 June 2007). The Loss of Sadness: How Psychiatry Transformed Normal Sorrow into Depressive Disorder. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-804269-3.
  14. Thornhill NW, Thornhill R (1991). "An evolutionary analysis of psychological pain following human (Homo sapiens) rape: IV. The effect of the nature of the sexual assault". J Comp Psychol. 105 (3): 243–52. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.105.3.243. PMID 1935004.
  15. Thornhill, R.; Thornhill, N.W. (1989). "The evolution of psychological pain". Sociobiology and the Social Sciences: 73–103.
  16. ^ Neese, Randolph M. (January 2005). "Is Depression an Adaptation?" (PDF). Archives of General Psychiatry. 57 (1): 14–20. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.318.2659. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14. PMID 10632228. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2020-11-09. Retrieved 2007-10-23.
  17. Suarez, S.D.; Gallup Jr, G.G. (1991). "Depression as a response to reproductive failure". Journal of Social and Biological Structures. 8 (3): 279–287. doi:10.1016/0140-1750(85)90071-5.
  18. Hagen, Edward H.; Barrett, Clark (2007). "Perinatal Sadness among Shuar Women: Support for an Evolutionary Theory of Psychic Pain" (PDF). Medical Anthropology Quarterly. 21 (1): 22–40. doi:10.1525/maq.2007.21.1.22. ISSN 0745-5194. PMID 17405696. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-09-26. Retrieved 2007-10-23.
  19. Keller, Matthew C.; Neese, Randolph M. (May 2005). "Is low mood an adaptation? Evidence for subtypes with symptoms that match precipitants" (PDF). Journal of Affective Disorders. 86 (1): 27–35. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.409.6927. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2004.12.005. PMID 15820268. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-08-10. Retrieved 2007-10-23.
  20. Tooby, J.; Cosmides, L. (1990). "The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments". Ethology and Sociobiology. 11 (4–5): 375–424. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z.
  21. ^ Hagen, E.H. (1999). "The Functions of Postpartum Depression". Evolution and Human Behavior. 20 (5): 325–359. Bibcode:1999EHumB..20..325H. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.335.7173. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00016-1.
  22. ^ Nettle, D. (2004). "Evolutionary origins of depression: a review and reformulation". Journal of Affective Disorders. 81 (2): 91–102. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2003.08.009. PMID 15306134.
  23. Sachar, E (1975). "Neuroendocrine abnormalities in depressive illness". Topics of Psychoendocrinology: 135.
  24. Gregg Henriques. "Depression: disease or behavioral shutdown mechanism" (PDF). Journal of Science and Health Policy. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-03-03. Retrieved 2009-12-14.
  25. Seligman, Martin (1975). Helplessness: On Depression, Development and Death. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.
  26. ^ Andrews, P.W.; Thompson, J.A. (2009). "The bright side of being blue: depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems". Psychological Review. 116 (3): 620–654. doi:10.1037/a0016242. PMC 2734449. PMID 19618990.
  27. ^ Wittman, D. (2014). "Darwinian Depression". Journal of Affective Disorders. 168 (2): 142–150. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.06.052. PMID 25046740.
  28. Nolen-Hoeksma, S (1987). "Sex differences in depression: theory and evidence". Psychological Bulletin. 101 (2): 259–82. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.259. PMID 3562707. S2CID 5026228.
  29. Jacobs, B.L.; Fornal, C.A. (1999). "Activity of serotonergic neurons in behaving animals". Neuropsychopharmacology. 21 (2 Suppl): 9S – 15S. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1395336. PMID 10432483.
  30. ^ Gilbert, P. (2006). "Evolution and depression: issues and implications". Psychological Medicine. 36 (3): 287–297. doi:10.1017/S0033291705006112. PMID 16236231. S2CID 15408068.
  31. Nesse, R.M. (2000). "Is Depression an Adaptation?". Archives of General Psychiatry. 57 (1): 14–20. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.318.2659. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14. PMID 10632228.
  32. Paul Gilbert (1992). Depression: The Evolution of Powerlessness. Psychology Press. ISBN 978-0-86377-221-4.
  33. Averill, J.R. (1968). "Grief: its nature and significance". Psychological Bulletin. 70 (6): 721–748. doi:10.1037/h0026824. PMID 4889573.
  34. Gaffney, Michael R.; Adams, Kai H.; Syme, Kristen L.; Hagen, Edward H. (2022-05-01). "Depression and suicidality as evolved credible signals of need in social conflicts". Evolution and Human Behavior. 43 (3): 242–256. Bibcode:2022EHumB..43..242G. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.02.004. ISSN 1090-5138.
  35. ^ Hagen, E.H. (2003). "The Bargaining Model of Depression". MIT Press. pp. 95–124. doi:10.7551/mitpress/3232.003.0008. ISBN 978-0-262-08326-3 http://cogprints.org/2471/1/depression.pdf. Retrieved 2008-02-28. {{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help); Missing or empty |title= (help)
  36. ^ Watson, P.J.; Andrews, P.W. (2002). "Toward a revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of depression: the social navigation hypothesis". Journal of Affective Disorders. 72 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00459-1. PMID 12204312.
  37. Hammerstein, Peter (2003). Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. MIT Press. pp. 95–97. ISBN 978-0-262-08326-3.
  38. Hagen, E.H. (2002). "Depression as bargaining The case postpartum". Evolution and Human Behavior. 23 (5): 323–336. Bibcode:2002EHumB..23..323H. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00102-7.
  39. ^ "Watson, Paul J. "An Evolutionary Adaptationist Theory of Unipolar Depression: Depression as an adaptation for social navigation, especially for overcoming costly, complex contractual constraints of the individual's social niche"". Archived from the original on 2019-12-31. Retrieved 2017-07-03.
  40. ^ Kinney, D.K.; Tanaka, M. (2009). "An evolutionary hypothesis of depression and its symptoms, adaptive values, and risk factors". Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 197 (8): 561–567. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181b05fa8. PMID 19684491. S2CID 205881413.
  41. Raison, C.L, Miller, A. N. (2012). The evolutionary significance of depression in Pathogen Host Defense (PATHOS-D) Molecular Psychiatry 1-23. PDF.
  42. Kluger, M.J. (1991). "Fever: Role of pyrogens and cryogens". Physiological Reviews. 71 (1): 93–127. doi:10.1152/physrev.1991.71.1.93. PMC 7191625. PMID 1986393.
  43. Dantzer, E (2002). "Cytokines and depression: an update". Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 16 (5): 501–502. doi:10.1016/s0889-1591(02)00002-8. PMID 12401463. S2CID 33116478.
  44. Hendrie CA, Pickles AR (2010). "Depression as an evolutionary adaptation: Implications for the development of new drug treatments". European Psychiatric Review. 3: 46.
  45. Hendrie CA, Pickles AR (2011) Depression: An evolutionary adaptation organised around the third ventricle In: Brinkworth M, Weinert F (eds) Darwinian Repercussions Darwinism in anInterdisciplinary Context Heidelberg, New York, London: Springer
  46. Hendrie CA, Pickles AR (2012). "The failure of the antidepressant drug discovery process is systemic". J Psychopharm. 27 (5): 407–13, discussion 413-6. doi:10.1177/0269881112466185. PMID 23222042. S2CID 24171469.
  47. Baumann B, Bornschlegl C, Krell D, Bogerts B (1997). "Changes in CSF spaces differ in endogenous and neurotic depression A planimetric CT scan study". J Affect Disord. 45 (3): 179–88. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00073-6. PMID 9298431.
  48. Cousins D. A.; Brian Moore P.; Watson S.; Harrison L.; Nicol Ferrier I.; Young A. H.; Lloyd A. J. (2010). "Pituitary volume and third ventricle width in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 35 (7): 1074–1081. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.01.008. PMID 20171783. S2CID 24962012.
  49. ^ Brüne, Martin (2006). "Evolutionary psychiatry is dead – Long liveth evolutionary psychopathology". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 29 (4): 408. doi:10.1017/S0140525X06259090.
  50. Douzenis, A; Seretis, D; Rizos, E; Michopoulos, I; Christodoulou, C; Lykouras, L (2010). "Evolution and Psychiatry". British Journal of Psychiatry. 196 (3): 246–47. doi:10.1192/bjp.196.3.246a. PMID 20194552.
  51. Adams, H. and Sutker, P. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology, 3rd Ed. Springer. 2001. Chapter 3, pp. 53–84.
  52. ^ Adams, H. and Sutker, P. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology, 3rd Ed. Springer. 2001. Chapter 5, pp. 105–27
  53. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). 2000. Culture Bound Syndromes, pp. 897–903.
  54. Schumaker, J. The Age of Insanity: Modernity and Mental Health. Praeger. 2001. Chapter 3, pp. 29–49.
  55. Adams, H. and Sutker, P. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology, 3rd Ed. Springer. 2001
Evolutionary psychology
Evolutionary
processes
Areas
Cognition /
Emotion
Culture
Development
Human factors /
Mental health
Sex
Sex differences
Related subjects
Academic disciplines
Research topics
Theoretical positions
Categories: