Revision as of 18:35, 1 May 2012 editAcadēmica Orientālis (talk | contribs)4,651 edits →Approaches: link← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 06:47, 5 November 2024 edit undoLWG (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users3,514 edits rm dead pov tag: no extant dispute | ||
(193 intermediate revisions by 63 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Short description|Psychosocial examination of crime}} | |||
⚫ | {{ |
||
{{Use dmy dates|date=May 2017}} | |||
⚫ | {{weasel|date=June 2012}} | ||
{{Criminology and penology |theory}} | |||
⚫ | '''Biosocial criminology''' is an |
||
{{Evolutionary biology}} | |||
⚫ | '''Biosocial criminology''' is an interdisciplinary field that aims to explain crime and ] by exploring ] factors. While contemporary ] has been dominated by ] theories, biosocial criminology also recognizes the potential contributions of fields such as ], ], and ].<ref name="Asghate2011">Kevin M. Beaver and Anthony Walsh. 2011. Biosocial Criminology. Chapter 1 in The Ashgate Research Companion to Biosocial Theories of Crime. 2011. Ashgate.</ref> | ||
==History== | |||
Historically, the role of biology was seen as much more important, and early, 19th-century crimonological theories were heavily influenced by biological notions; for example ] argued that criminals are ], "]" humans who can be identified by shared, "primitive", physical characteristics. With the passage of time and the advent of the 20th century, the ] argument came to be dominated by an absolutist nurture faction, and theories related to the importance of biology on criminal behavior were seen as obsolete. In the second half of the twentieth century, however, such theories began to be revisited. While no longer doubting the significant impact of environmental factors, arguments were brought forward once more focusing on the possible importance of certain biological factors, such as specific ]. | |||
==Approaches== | ==Approaches== | ||
===Environment=== | |||
One approach to studying the role of genetics for crime is to calculate the ] coefficient. It describes the proportion of the ] for some characteristic that differs between individuals that is due to genetic factors. The non-heritability proportion can be further divided into the "shared environment" which is the non-genetic factors which make siblings similar while the "non-shared environment" is the non-genetic factors which makes siblings different from another. Thousands of studies on antisocial behaviors studying hundreds of thousands of persons have typically found that the "shared environment" account for 10% of less of the variance while "non-shared environment" account for around 40% of the variance. A more recent approach with a large potential is to directly study DNA and examine if specific genetic markers are associated with criminal behavior. A number of such associations have already been found, especially between ]s genes and criminal behaviors. Each individual association when studied in isolation has a relatively small effect. However, also single genes can have a much larger effect in specific environments which is an example of ].<ref name=Asghate2011/> | |||
Environment has a significant effect on genetic expression. Disadvantaged environments enhance antisocial gene expression, suppress prosocial gene action and prevent the realization of genetic potential.<ref name=Asghate2011/> | |||
{{Blockquote|Genes and environments operating in tandem (interacting) were required to produce significant antisocial behavior, while neither was powerful enough to produce it independent of the other. That is, children genetically at risk for antisocial behavior reared in positive family environments did not display antisocial behavior, and children not at genetic risk did not become antisocial in adverse family environments.<ref name=Asghate2011/>}} | |||
⚫ | Another approach is to examine the relationship between neurophysiology and criminality. One example is that measured levels of neurotransmitters such as ] and ] have been associated with criminal behavior. Another is that ] studies give strong evidence |
||
===Behavioral genetics=== | |||
Biosocial criminology has sometimes been criticized for ignoring environmental influences. Biosocial researchers argues that this is incorrect but that on the other hand many sociologically influenced criminological approaches completely ignores the potential role of genetic which means that the results is likely ]ed by genetic factors. For example, if a study finds an association between parental abusive and antisocial behavior when the children become adults this is often assumed to be evidence for a causation between parental abuse and antisocial behavior. However, another possibility is that shared genes may cause both behaviors. Furthermore, the environment is often influenced by genes which is an example of ]. One example being that genes may influence which environment a person prefers and selects to be in. Thus, a strong association between exposure to delinquent peers and a person's own delinquent behavior has been taken as evidence for causal process where a person learns to become a criminal. However, research on the role of genetic factors have found that such exposure is largely caused by genetic factors.<ref name=Asghate2011/> | |||
One approach to studying the role of genetics for crime is to calculate the ] coefficient, which describes the proportion of the ] that is due to actualized genetic effects for some trait in a given population in a specific environment at a specific time. According to ] and ], the heritability coefficient for antisocial behavior is estimated to be between 0.40 and 0.58.<ref name=Asghate2011/> | |||
The methodology often used in biosocial criminology (that of ]) has been criticized for producing inflated heritability estimates,<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Burt |first1=Callie H. |last2=Simons |first2=Ronald L.|author-link2=Ronald L. Simons |date=2014-05-01 |title=Pulling Back the Curtain on Heritability Studies: Biosocial Criminology in the Postgenomic Era |journal=Criminology |language=en |volume=52 |issue=2 |pages=223–262 |doi=10.1111/1745-9125.12036 |issn=1745-9125}}</ref> though biosocial criminologists maintain that these criticisms are baseless.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Barnes |first1=J. C. |last2=Wright |first2=John Paul|author-link2=John Paul Wright |last3=Boutwell |first3=Brian B. |author-link3=Brian Boutwell|last4=Schwartz |first4=Joseph A. |last5=Connolly |first5=Eric J. |last6=Nedelec |first6=Joseph L. |last7=Beaver |first7=Kevin M. |author-link7=Kevin Beaver|date=2014-11-01 |title=Demonstrating the Validity of Twin Research in Criminology |journal=Criminology |language=en |volume=52 |issue=4 |pages=588–626 |doi=10.1111/1745-9125.12049 |issn=1745-9125}}</ref> Criminal justice researchers Brian Boutwell and J.C. Barnes argue that many sociological studies that do not control for genetic inheritance of risk factors have misleading or unreliable results.<ref></ref> | |||
Sociological approaches to criminality, while recognizing the importance of environmental factors through an individual's life, have mostly studied factors influential during adolescence or adulthood. Biological approaches have emphasized the importance of early development when the brain is rapidly growing and developing. ]s can cause brain damage and are associated with later criminality. Also the environment during first few years after birth are critical for the growing brain. This early environment has been lined with child temperaments that are associated with later problematic behaviors.<ref name=Asghate2011/> | |||
=== Neurophysiology === | |||
Two traditions in biosocial criminology focus on analyzing evolutionary forces related to criminal behavior. They link criminal behavior with specific biological processes such as ] secretions or ]. | |||
⚫ | Another approach is to examine the relationship between neurophysiology and criminality. One example is that measured levels of neurotransmitters such as ] and ] have been associated with criminal behavior. Another is that ] studies give strong evidence that both brain structure and function are involved in criminal behaviors. The ] creates emotions such as anger and jealousy that ultimately may cause criminal behavior. The ] is involved in delaying gratification and impulse control and moderates the impulses from the limbic system. If this balance is shifted in favor of the limbic system this may contribute to criminal behavior. ] argues that "life-course-persistent offenders" make up only 6% of the population but commits more than 50% of all crimes and that this is due to a combination neurophysiological deficits and an adverse environment that creates a criminal path that is very difficult to break once started.<ref name=Asghate2011/> | ||
=== Evolutionary psychology === | |||
], a representative of the "evolutionary forces" school, suggested that some human males - ] - display alternative ]; which can be described as engaging in various lawless acts throughout their life, particularly in their youth, that would usually, if known, lessen their chances of finding a reproductive partner. However, by the use of deception (creating false images of themselves towards their mates and society), and success in assaulting rivals and accumulating property and status, they manage to reproduce successfully. | |||
Men can potentially have many children with little effort; women only a few with great effort. One argued consequence of this is that males are more aggressive, and more violently aggressive, than females, since they face higher reproductive competition from their own sex than females. In particular, low-status males may be more likely to remain completely childless. Under such circumstances, it may have been evolutionarily useful to take very high risks and use violent aggression in order to try to increase status and reproductive success rather than become genetically extinct. This may explain why males have higher crime rates than females and why low status and being unmarried is associated with criminality. It may also explain why the degree of income inequality of a society is a better predictor than the absolute income level of the society for male-male homicides; income inequality creates social disparity, while differing average income levels may not do so. Furthermore, competition over females is argued to have been particularly intensive in late adolescence and young adulthood, which is theorized to explain why crime rates are particularly high during this period.<ref name=AEP>Aurelio José Figueredo, Paul Robert Gladden, Zachary Hohman. The evolutionary psychology of criminal behaviour. In {{Cite book | last1 = Roberts | first1 = S. C. | editor1-last = Roberts | doi = 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001 | editor1-first = S. Craig | title = Applied Evolutionary Psychology | year = 2011 | publisher = Oxford University Press| isbn = 9780199586073 }}</ref> | |||
The "]" focuses on the hormone ] as a factor influencing aggression and criminality and being beneficial during certain forms of competition.<ref name="psychology-criminalbehavior-law.com">{{cite web|title=Should We Be More Concerned With Prenatal Exposure in the Prevention Of Crime?|url=http://www.psychology-criminalbehavior-law.com/2015/04/should-we-be-more-concerned-with-prenatal-exposure-in-the-prevention-of-crime/|access-date=23 April 2015}}</ref> In most species, males are more aggressive than females. Castration of males usually has a pacifying effect on aggressive behavior in males.<ref name="crimetimes.org">{{cite web|title=MILD TESTOSTERONE REDUCTION EFFECTIVE AGAINST AGGRESSION?|url=http://www.crimetimes.org/95d/w95dp6.htm|access-date=23 April 2015|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150504184242/http://www.crimetimes.org/95d/w95dp6.htm|archive-date=4 May 2015|url-status=dead}}</ref> In humans, males engage in crime and especially violent crime more than females. The involvement in crime usually rises in the early teens to mid teens in correlation with the rise of testosterone levels. Research on the relationship between testosterone and aggression is difficult since the only reliable measurement of brain testosterone is by ], which is not done for research purposes. Studies therefore have often instead used less reliable measurements from blood or saliva. Some studies support a link between adult criminality and testosterone, although the relationship is modest if examined separately for each sex. A significant link between juvenile delinquency and testosterone levels has not been established. Some studies have also found testosterone to be associated with behaviors or personality traits linked with criminality such as ] and alcoholism. Many studies have also been done on the relationship between more general aggressive behavior/feelings and testosterone. About half the studies have found a relationship and about half no relationship.<ref name="EllisBeaver2009">{{cite book |author1=Lee Ellis| author2=Kevin M. Beaver|author3=John Wright| title=Handbook of Crime Correlates| url=https://books.google.com/books?id=eD0ttBXoMvQC| date=1 April 2009| publisher=Academic Press| isbn=978-0-12-373612-3}}</ref> | |||
], a representative of the "neurologically specific theories" school, proposed the ] ], which argues that people with an underdeveloped left hemisphere of the brain (linked with more linguistic skills and social behaviors) are more likely to engage in crime. As the hemispheres control opposite sides of the body, the theory also predicts that ] individuals are more likely to engage in criminal behavior than ] ones. | |||
Many conflicts causing homicides involve status conflicts, protecting reputation, and seemingly trivial insults.<ref name=AEP/> ] in his book '']'' argues that in non-state societies without a police it was very important to have a credible ] against aggression. Therefore, it was important to have a reputation for retaliation, causing humans to develop instincts for ] as well as for protecting reputation ("honor"). Pinker argues that the development of the state and the police have dramatically reduced the level of violence compared to the ancestral environment. Whenever the state breaks down, which can be very locally such as in poor areas of a city, humans again organize in groups for protection and aggression and concepts such as violent revenge and protecting honor again become extremely important. | |||
The ] (ENA theory) takes from both the evolutionary and neurological theories, focusing on the hormone ]. It asserts that males have evolved so that testosterone will make them more likely to display competing behavior and victimize others than females. More competitive males will be seen by females as more able to provide for the family, and thus more likely to be chosen as mates and so more able to pass their genes to the next generation. Testosterone is a key component of the process, as it tends to shift brain functions from the left to the right hemisphere. | |||
Some cultures place greater emphasis on protecting honor than other cultures. One explanation is that protecting honor was in the ancestral past relatively more important for herders than for farmers. The livestock of herders were easily and quickly stolen. As a result, it was important to constantly show toughness as a deterrence, which may cause a higher level of violence. The predictions of the theory was confirmed in a cross-cultural examination of traditional farming and herding Spanish-American societies. However, the prediction that sedentary fishing societies would place a low emphasis on honor was not confirmed.<ref name=AEP/> | |||
The degree of cultural ] is strongly associated with the burden of ]. It has been argued that this is due to collectivism and associated characteristics such as ] avoidance limiting the spread of infectious diseases. Other characteristics such as strong ] and willingness to defend the ]'s honor may promote violence. A study found strong associations between several forms of violent criminal behavior and both infectious disease rates across U.S states and degree of cultural collectivism across U.S. states. The associations remained strong after controlling for income inequality.<ref name=AEP/> | |||
==== Specific forms ==== | |||
Evolutionary psychology researchers have proposed several evolutionary explanations for ]. One is that psychopathy represents a frequency-dependent, socially ] strategy. This may benefit the psychopath as long as there are few other psychopaths in the community since more psychopaths means increasing the risk of encountering another psychopath as well as non-psychopaths likely adapting more countermeasures against cheaters.<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Glenn | first1 = A. L. | last2 = Kurzban | first2 = R. | last3 = Raine | first3 = A. | doi = 10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009 | title = Evolutionary theory and psychopathy | journal = Aggression and Violent Behavior | volume = 16 | issue = 5 | pages = 371 | year = 2011 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Buss | first1 = D. M. | title = How Can Evolutionary Psychology Successfully Explain Personality and Individual Differences? | doi = 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x | journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science | volume = 4 | issue = 4 | pages = 359–366 | year = 2009 | pmid = 26158983| s2cid = 2565416 }}</ref><ref>Harpending, H. C., & Sobus, J. (1987). Sociopathy as an adaptation. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, 63-72.</ref> | |||
] are theories that explore to what degree, if any, evolutionary adaptations influence the psychology of rapists. Such theories are highly controversial, as traditional theories typically do not consider rape to be a behavioral adaptation. Some object to such theories on ethical, religious, political, as well as scientific grounds. Others argue that a correct knowledge of the causes of rape is necessary in order to develop effective preventive measures. | |||
The ] is the alleged higher rate of ] being abused by ] as compared to genetic parents, observed in some, but not all, studies. An explanation of this affect has been attempted by application of ] theories. There have also been various criticisms of these theories.<ref name=DalyWilson2007>Daly & Wilson (2007) {{webarchive |url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110516102016/http://psych.mcmaster.ca/dalywilson/Cinderella_Effect.pdf |date=16 May 2011 }} In Crawford & Krebs (Eds) Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology, pp. 383-400. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.</ref> | |||
] is one of the few forms of violence more often done by women than men. Cross-cultural research has found that this is more likely to occur when the child has deformities or illnesses as well as when there are lacking resources due to factors such as poverty, other children requiring resources, and no male support. Such a child may have a low chance of reproductive success, in which case it would decrease the mother's ] to spend resources on the child, in particular since women generally have a greater ] than men.<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Liddle | first1 = J. R. | last2 = Shackelford | first2 = T. K. | last3 = Weekes–Shackelford | first3 = V. A. | title = Why can't we all just get along? Evolutionary perspectives on violence, homicide, and war | doi = 10.1037/a0026610 | journal = Review of General Psychology | volume = 16 | pages = 24–36 | year = 2012 | s2cid = 142984456 }}</ref> | |||
==== Criminal justice ==== | |||
Punishment of exploitative behaviors harmful to the group was likely a recurring problem in the ancestral environment. As such humans are argued to have developed a range of psychological mechanisms for handling this. Punishment can be a deterrent to undesired behaviors but excessive punishment can also be harmful to the group. Thus, humans are argued to favor a proportional response based on how severe the offence is. Cross-cultural research have a found a high agreement regarding how relatively harmful different crimes are perceived to be. On the other hand, evolutionary novel factors that may be rational to consider from a deterrent perspective, such as how difficult it is for the modern police to detect the crime, do not seem to affect people's perceptions of appropriate punishments.<ref name=AEP2>Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In {{Cite book | last1 = Roberts | first1 = S. C. | editor1-last = Roberts | doi = 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001 | editor1-first = S. Craig | title = Applied Evolutionary Psychology | year = 2011 | publisher = Oxford University Press| isbn = 9780199586073 }}</ref> | |||
Once a crime's severity has been judged, there is a choice regarding how to respond. In some cases in the ancestral environment there may have been benefits from future interactions with the offender which some forms of punishment may have prevented as compared to responses such as reparations or rehabilitation. Research suggests that individuals may modify what they think are appropriate forms of response to offenders based on factors that once in the past small-group environment may have indicated that they could personally benefit from continued interactions with the offender such as kinship, in-group or out-group membership, possession of resources, sexual attractiveness, expressed remorse, intentionality, and prior history of cooperation and exploitation.<ref name=AEP2/> | |||
==See also== | ==See also== | ||
{{Portal|Biology|Society}} | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
==References== | ==References== | ||
Line 35: | Line 60: | ||
==Further reading== | ==Further reading== | ||
* |
*Walsh, A., & Beaver, K.M. (2008). ''Biosocial criminology: new directions in theory and research.'' New York City: Taylor & Francis, {{ISBN|0-415-98944-2}}. | ||
* |
*Anthony Walsh, Lee Ellis, ''Biosocial criminology: challenging environmentalism's supremacy'', Nova Science Publishers, 2003, {{ISBN|1-59033-774-3}} | ||
*Matt DeLisi, Michael G Vaughn, ''The Routledge International Handbook of Biosocial Criminology'', Routledge, 2015, {{ISBN|9781317936749}} | |||
* |
*Kevin Beaver. ''Biosocial Criminology: A Primer'' Ken Hunt Publishing Company. 2009. | ||
*The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture | |||
*Jerome H. Barkow (Editor), Leda Cosmides (Editor), John Tooby (Editor) | *The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture . Jerome H. Barkow (Editor), Leda Cosmides (Editor), John Tooby (Editor) | ||
*Homicide (Foundations of Human Behavior) | *Homicide (Foundations of Human Behavior) , Margo Wilson (Author), Martin Daly (Author) | ||
*Margo Wilson (Author), Martin Daly (Author) | |||
*How the Mind Works , Steven Pinker (Author) | *How the Mind Works , Steven Pinker (Author) | ||
*Demonic Males by Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson | *Demonic Males by Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson | ||
*Before the Dawn: Recovering the Lost History of Our Ancestors | |||
*Human Morality and Sociality: Evolutionary and Comparative Perspectives by Henrik Hogh-Olesen, Christophe Boesch, Leda Cosmides and Azar Gat (Jan 19, 2010) | *Human Morality and Sociality: Evolutionary and Comparative Perspectives by Henrik Hogh-Olesen, Christophe Boesch, Leda Cosmides and Azar Gat (Jan 19, 2010) | ||
*Sex, Evolution and Behavior by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson |
*Sex, Evolution and Behavior by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson | ||
*Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (4th Edition) by David M. Buss (Feb 28, 2011) | *Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (4th Edition) by David M. Buss (Feb 28, 2011) | ||
{{Evolutionary psychology}} | |||
{{Authority control}} | |||
] | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
] |
Latest revision as of 06:47, 5 November 2024
Psychosocial examination of crime
This article contains weasel words: vague phrasing that often accompanies biased or unverifiable information. Such statements should be clarified or removed. (June 2012) |
Criminology and penology | |||
---|---|---|---|
Theory
|
|||
Major theorists | |||
Types of crime | |||
Methods | |||
Penology
|
|||
Schools | |||
Subfields | |||
Browse | |||
Part of a series on |
Evolutionary biology |
---|
Darwin's finches by John Gould |
Processes and outcomes |
Natural history |
History of evolutionary theory |
Fields and applications
|
Social implications |
Biosocial criminology is an interdisciplinary field that aims to explain crime and antisocial behavior by exploring biocultural factors. While contemporary criminology has been dominated by sociological theories, biosocial criminology also recognizes the potential contributions of fields such as behavioral genetics, neuropsychology, and evolutionary psychology.
Approaches
Environment
Environment has a significant effect on genetic expression. Disadvantaged environments enhance antisocial gene expression, suppress prosocial gene action and prevent the realization of genetic potential.
Genes and environments operating in tandem (interacting) were required to produce significant antisocial behavior, while neither was powerful enough to produce it independent of the other. That is, children genetically at risk for antisocial behavior reared in positive family environments did not display antisocial behavior, and children not at genetic risk did not become antisocial in adverse family environments.
Behavioral genetics
One approach to studying the role of genetics for crime is to calculate the heritability coefficient, which describes the proportion of the variance that is due to actualized genetic effects for some trait in a given population in a specific environment at a specific time. According to Kevin Beaver and Anthony Walsh, the heritability coefficient for antisocial behavior is estimated to be between 0.40 and 0.58.
The methodology often used in biosocial criminology (that of twin studies) has been criticized for producing inflated heritability estimates, though biosocial criminologists maintain that these criticisms are baseless. Criminal justice researchers Brian Boutwell and J.C. Barnes argue that many sociological studies that do not control for genetic inheritance of risk factors have misleading or unreliable results.
Neurophysiology
Another approach is to examine the relationship between neurophysiology and criminality. One example is that measured levels of neurotransmitters such as serotonin and dopamine have been associated with criminal behavior. Another is that neuroimaging studies give strong evidence that both brain structure and function are involved in criminal behaviors. The limbic system creates emotions such as anger and jealousy that ultimately may cause criminal behavior. The prefrontal cortex is involved in delaying gratification and impulse control and moderates the impulses from the limbic system. If this balance is shifted in favor of the limbic system this may contribute to criminal behavior. Terrie Moffitt's developmental theory of crime argues that "life-course-persistent offenders" make up only 6% of the population but commits more than 50% of all crimes and that this is due to a combination neurophysiological deficits and an adverse environment that creates a criminal path that is very difficult to break once started.
Evolutionary psychology
Men can potentially have many children with little effort; women only a few with great effort. One argued consequence of this is that males are more aggressive, and more violently aggressive, than females, since they face higher reproductive competition from their own sex than females. In particular, low-status males may be more likely to remain completely childless. Under such circumstances, it may have been evolutionarily useful to take very high risks and use violent aggression in order to try to increase status and reproductive success rather than become genetically extinct. This may explain why males have higher crime rates than females and why low status and being unmarried is associated with criminality. It may also explain why the degree of income inequality of a society is a better predictor than the absolute income level of the society for male-male homicides; income inequality creates social disparity, while differing average income levels may not do so. Furthermore, competition over females is argued to have been particularly intensive in late adolescence and young adulthood, which is theorized to explain why crime rates are particularly high during this period.
The "evolutionary neuroandrogenic theory" focuses on the hormone testosterone as a factor influencing aggression and criminality and being beneficial during certain forms of competition. In most species, males are more aggressive than females. Castration of males usually has a pacifying effect on aggressive behavior in males. In humans, males engage in crime and especially violent crime more than females. The involvement in crime usually rises in the early teens to mid teens in correlation with the rise of testosterone levels. Research on the relationship between testosterone and aggression is difficult since the only reliable measurement of brain testosterone is by lumbar puncture, which is not done for research purposes. Studies therefore have often instead used less reliable measurements from blood or saliva. Some studies support a link between adult criminality and testosterone, although the relationship is modest if examined separately for each sex. A significant link between juvenile delinquency and testosterone levels has not been established. Some studies have also found testosterone to be associated with behaviors or personality traits linked with criminality such as antisocial behavior and alcoholism. Many studies have also been done on the relationship between more general aggressive behavior/feelings and testosterone. About half the studies have found a relationship and about half no relationship.
Many conflicts causing homicides involve status conflicts, protecting reputation, and seemingly trivial insults. Steven Pinker in his book The Blank Slate argues that in non-state societies without a police it was very important to have a credible deterrence against aggression. Therefore, it was important to have a reputation for retaliation, causing humans to develop instincts for revenge as well as for protecting reputation ("honor"). Pinker argues that the development of the state and the police have dramatically reduced the level of violence compared to the ancestral environment. Whenever the state breaks down, which can be very locally such as in poor areas of a city, humans again organize in groups for protection and aggression and concepts such as violent revenge and protecting honor again become extremely important.
Some cultures place greater emphasis on protecting honor than other cultures. One explanation is that protecting honor was in the ancestral past relatively more important for herders than for farmers. The livestock of herders were easily and quickly stolen. As a result, it was important to constantly show toughness as a deterrence, which may cause a higher level of violence. The predictions of the theory was confirmed in a cross-cultural examination of traditional farming and herding Spanish-American societies. However, the prediction that sedentary fishing societies would place a low emphasis on honor was not confirmed.
The degree of cultural collectivism is strongly associated with the burden of infectious disease. It has been argued that this is due to collectivism and associated characteristics such as out-group avoidance limiting the spread of infectious diseases. Other characteristics such as strong in-group–out-group bias and willingness to defend the ingroup's honor may promote violence. A study found strong associations between several forms of violent criminal behavior and both infectious disease rates across U.S states and degree of cultural collectivism across U.S. states. The associations remained strong after controlling for income inequality.
Specific forms
Evolutionary psychology researchers have proposed several evolutionary explanations for psychopathy. One is that psychopathy represents a frequency-dependent, socially parasitic strategy. This may benefit the psychopath as long as there are few other psychopaths in the community since more psychopaths means increasing the risk of encountering another psychopath as well as non-psychopaths likely adapting more countermeasures against cheaters.
Sociobiological theories of rape are theories that explore to what degree, if any, evolutionary adaptations influence the psychology of rapists. Such theories are highly controversial, as traditional theories typically do not consider rape to be a behavioral adaptation. Some object to such theories on ethical, religious, political, as well as scientific grounds. Others argue that a correct knowledge of the causes of rape is necessary in order to develop effective preventive measures.
The Cinderella effect is the alleged higher rate of stepchildren being abused by stepparents as compared to genetic parents, observed in some, but not all, studies. An explanation of this affect has been attempted by application of evolutionary psychology theories. There have also been various criticisms of these theories.
Infanticide is one of the few forms of violence more often done by women than men. Cross-cultural research has found that this is more likely to occur when the child has deformities or illnesses as well as when there are lacking resources due to factors such as poverty, other children requiring resources, and no male support. Such a child may have a low chance of reproductive success, in which case it would decrease the mother's inclusive fitness to spend resources on the child, in particular since women generally have a greater parental investment than men.
Criminal justice
Punishment of exploitative behaviors harmful to the group was likely a recurring problem in the ancestral environment. As such humans are argued to have developed a range of psychological mechanisms for handling this. Punishment can be a deterrent to undesired behaviors but excessive punishment can also be harmful to the group. Thus, humans are argued to favor a proportional response based on how severe the offence is. Cross-cultural research have a found a high agreement regarding how relatively harmful different crimes are perceived to be. On the other hand, evolutionary novel factors that may be rational to consider from a deterrent perspective, such as how difficult it is for the modern police to detect the crime, do not seem to affect people's perceptions of appropriate punishments.
Once a crime's severity has been judged, there is a choice regarding how to respond. In some cases in the ancestral environment there may have been benefits from future interactions with the offender which some forms of punishment may have prevented as compared to responses such as reparations or rehabilitation. Research suggests that individuals may modify what they think are appropriate forms of response to offenders based on factors that once in the past small-group environment may have indicated that they could personally benefit from continued interactions with the offender such as kinship, in-group or out-group membership, possession of resources, sexual attractiveness, expressed remorse, intentionality, and prior history of cooperation and exploitation.
See also
- Anthropological criminology
- Behavioral genetics
- Biosocial theory
- Evolutionary psychology
- Scientific racism
- Sociobiology
- Statistical correlations of criminal behavior
References
- ^ Kevin M. Beaver and Anthony Walsh. 2011. Biosocial Criminology. Chapter 1 in The Ashgate Research Companion to Biosocial Theories of Crime. 2011. Ashgate.
- Burt, Callie H.; Simons, Ronald L. (1 May 2014). "Pulling Back the Curtain on Heritability Studies: Biosocial Criminology in the Postgenomic Era". Criminology. 52 (2): 223–262. doi:10.1111/1745-9125.12036. ISSN 1745-9125.
- Barnes, J. C.; Wright, John Paul; Boutwell, Brian B.; Schwartz, Joseph A.; Connolly, Eric J.; Nedelec, Joseph L.; Beaver, Kevin M. (1 November 2014). "Demonstrating the Validity of Twin Research in Criminology". Criminology. 52 (4): 588–626. doi:10.1111/1745-9125.12049. ISSN 1745-9125.
- Is crime genetic? Scientists don't know because they're afraid to ask
- ^ Aurelio José Figueredo, Paul Robert Gladden, Zachary Hohman. The evolutionary psychology of criminal behaviour. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
- "Should We Be More Concerned With Prenatal Exposure in the Prevention Of Crime?". Retrieved 23 April 2015.
- "MILD TESTOSTERONE REDUCTION EFFECTIVE AGAINST AGGRESSION?". Archived from the original on 4 May 2015. Retrieved 23 April 2015.
- Lee Ellis; Kevin M. Beaver; John Wright (1 April 2009). Handbook of Crime Correlates. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-373612-3.
- Glenn, A. L.; Kurzban, R.; Raine, A. (2011). "Evolutionary theory and psychopathy". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 16 (5): 371. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009.
- Buss, D. M. (2009). "How Can Evolutionary Psychology Successfully Explain Personality and Individual Differences?". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 4 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x. PMID 26158983. S2CID 2565416.
- Harpending, H. C., & Sobus, J. (1987). Sociopathy as an adaptation. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, 63-72.
- Daly & Wilson (2007) Is the "Cinderella Effect" controversial? Archived 16 May 2011 at the Wayback Machine In Crawford & Krebs (Eds) Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology, pp. 383-400. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Liddle, J. R.; Shackelford, T. K.; Weekes–Shackelford, V. A. (2012). "Why can't we all just get along? Evolutionary perspectives on violence, homicide, and war". Review of General Psychology. 16: 24–36. doi:10.1037/a0026610. S2CID 142984456.
- ^ Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
Further reading
- Walsh, A., & Beaver, K.M. (2008). Biosocial criminology: new directions in theory and research. New York City: Taylor & Francis, ISBN 0-415-98944-2.
- Anthony Walsh, Lee Ellis, Biosocial criminology: challenging environmentalism's supremacy, Nova Science Publishers, 2003, ISBN 1-59033-774-3
- Matt DeLisi, Michael G Vaughn, The Routledge International Handbook of Biosocial Criminology, Routledge, 2015, ISBN 9781317936749
- Kevin Beaver. Biosocial Criminology: A Primer Ken Hunt Publishing Company. 2009.
- The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture . Jerome H. Barkow (Editor), Leda Cosmides (Editor), John Tooby (Editor)
- Homicide (Foundations of Human Behavior) , Margo Wilson (Author), Martin Daly (Author)
- How the Mind Works , Steven Pinker (Author)
- Demonic Males by Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson
- Human Morality and Sociality: Evolutionary and Comparative Perspectives by Henrik Hogh-Olesen, Christophe Boesch, Leda Cosmides and Azar Gat (Jan 19, 2010)
- Sex, Evolution and Behavior by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson
- Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (4th Edition) by David M. Buss (Feb 28, 2011)