Revision as of 15:52, 23 April 2006 editAWeidman (talk | contribs)408 editsNo edit summary← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 01:45, 13 December 2024 edit undo2601:1c2:c003:db8:c109:3e2e:caec:6fdd (talk) →Diagnosis: InclusivityTags: Mobile edit Mobile web edit | ||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{ |
{{short description|Psychological disorder that can affect children}} | ||
{{Update|reason=DSM-5 was published in 2013, DSM-5-TR published in 2022. This article still speaks of DSM-5 as a proposal. Discussion of DSM 3&4 outside of history section should be removed and replaced.|date=September 2023}} | |||
{{DiseaseDisorder infobox | | |||
Name = Reactive Attachment Disorder | | |||
{{Use dmy dates|date=October 2019}} | |||
ICD10 = F94.1/2 | | |||
{{Infobox medical condition (new) | |||
ICD9 = {{ICD9|313.89}} | | |||
| name = Reactive attachment disorder | |||
| image = | |||
| caption = Children need sensitive and responsive caregivers to develop secure attachments. RAD arises from a failure to form normal attachments to primary caregivers in early childhood. | |||
| field = ], ] | |||
| symptoms = | |||
| complications = | |||
| onset = | |||
| duration = | |||
| types = | |||
| causes = | |||
| risks = | |||
| diagnosis = | |||
| differential = | |||
| prevention = | |||
| treatment = | |||
| medication = | |||
| prognosis = | |||
| frequency = | |||
| deaths = | |||
}} | }} | ||
{{intro length}} | |||
'''Reactive Attachment Disorder''' (sometimes called "RAD") (] 313.89) is a psychophysiologic condition (1) with markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate social relatedness in most contexts that begins before age five years and is associated with grossly pathological care. This pathological caregiving behaviour may consist of any form of neglect, ], mistreatment or ]. | |||
] | |||
In ], attachments to caregivers are consistent with the level of development. In ], attachments to caregivers either fail to develop or are highly deviant, but this usually occurs in a context of reasonably supportive care. | |||
'''Reactive attachment disorder''' ('''RAD''') is described in clinical literature as a severe ] that can affect children, although these issues do occasionally persist into adulthood.<ref name="DSM-IV-TR 2000 p. 129">DSM-IV-TR (2000) ] p. 129.</ref><ref>{{cite journal | author = ] DS, Willheim E | title = Disturbances of attachment and parental psychopathology in early childhood | journal = Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America | volume = 18 | issue = 3 | pages = 665–86 |date=July 2009 | pmid = 19486844 | doi = 10.1016/j.chc.2009.03.001 | pmc = 2690512}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author1=] van IJzendoorn MH |author2=Femmie Juffer | title = Behavior Problems and Mental Health Referrals of International Adoptees | journal = The Journal of the American Medical Association | volume = 293 | issue = 20 | pages = 2501–2515 |date= May 2005 | doi = 10.1001/jama.293.20.2501| pmid = 15914751 |s2cid=25576414 }}</ref> RAD is characterized by markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate ways of relating socially in most contexts. It can take the form of a persistent failure to initiate or respond to most social interactions in a developmentally appropriate way—known as the "inhibited form". In the ], the "disinhibited form" is considered a separate diagnosis named "]". | |||
RAD arises from a failure to form normal attachments to primary caregivers in early childhood. Such a failure could result from severe early experiences of ], ], abrupt separation from caregivers between the ages of six months and three years, frequent changes of caregivers, or a lack of caregiver responsiveness to a child's communicative efforts. It is differentiated from ] or developmental delay and from possibly ] conditions such as ], all of which can affect attachment behavior. The criteria for a diagnosis of a reactive attachment ''disorder'' are very different from the criteria used in assessment or categorization of ] such as insecure or disorganized attachment. | |||
Due to maltreatment by caregivers, RAD sufferers have difficulty forming healthy relationships with their caregivers, peers and families. | |||
Children with RAD are presumed to have grossly disturbed internal working models of relationships that may lead to interpersonal and behavioral difficulties in later life. There are few studies of long-term effects, and there is a lack of clarity about the presentation of the disorder beyond the age of five years.<ref name=AACAP-2005/><ref name="prior228">Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 228.</ref> However, the opening of orphanages in Eastern Europe following the end of the ] in the early 1990s provided opportunities for research on infants and toddlers brought up in very deprived conditions. Such research broadened the understanding of the prevalence, causes, mechanism and assessment of disorders of attachment and led to efforts from the late 1990s onwards to develop treatment and prevention programs and better methods of assessment. Mainstream theorists in the field have proposed that a broader range of conditions arising from problems with attachment should be defined beyond current classifications.<ref name="oconzean2003">{{cite journal |vauthors=O'Connor TG, Zeanah CH |title=Attachment disorders: assessment strategies and treatment approaches |journal=Attach Hum Dev |volume=5 |issue=3 |pages=223–44 |year=2003 |pmid=12944216 |doi=10.1080/14616730310001593974 |s2cid=21547653 }}</ref> | |||
RAD can reportedly be diagnosed as early as the first month of life, but critics have charged such diagnoses are often inaccurate. | |||
Mainstream treatment and prevention programs that target RAD and other problematic early attachment behaviors are based on ] and concentrate on increasing the responsiveness and sensitivity of the caregiver, or if that is not possible, placing the child with a different caregiver.<ref>Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 231.</ref> Most such strategies are in the process of being evaluated. Mainstream practitioners and theorists have presented significant criticism of the diagnosis and treatment of alleged reactive attachment disorder or the theoretically baseless "attachment disorder" within the controversial form of psychotherapy commonly known as ]. Attachment therapy has a scientifically unsupported theoretical base and uses diagnostic criteria or symptom lists markedly different from criteria under ICD-10 or DSM-IV-TR, or to attachment behaviors. A range of treatment approaches are used in attachment ''therapy'', some of which are physically and psychologically coercive, and considered to be ] to attachment ''theory''.<ref name="O'Connor & Nilsen">O'Connor TG, Nilsen WJ (2005). "Models versus Metaphors in Translating Attachment Theory to the Clinic and Community". In Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L and Greenberg MT (eds.). ''Enhancing Early Attachments: Theory, Research, Intervention, and Policy''. pp. 313–26. The Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. {{ISBN|1-59385-470-6}}.</ref> | |||
Some estimate that 10% (2) to 80% (3) of children and adolescents in ], and an unknown number of children who remain in their family of origin, suffer from RAD. (note some critics have questioned the accuracy of these percentages). There generally tend to be the same causes regardless of family setting. | |||
==Signs and symptoms== | |||
A crucial defining characteristic of Reactive Attachment Disorder--explicit in ] and ]--is that there be pathogenic caregiving. This can be very difficult to prove, but it makes lasting effects on the children concerned. | |||
{{See also|Attachment theory| attachment disorder}} | |||
] are often the first health professionals to assess and raise suspicions of RAD in children with the disorder. The initial presentation varies according to the child's developmental and chronological age, although it always involves a disturbance in social interaction. ]s up to about 18–24 months may present with ] and display abnormal responsiveness to stimuli. Laboratory investigations will be unremarkable barring possible findings consistent with ] or ], while serum ] levels will be normal or elevated.<ref name=Sad04>{{cite book |title=Kaplan & Sadock's Concise Textbook of Clinical Psychiatry |last=Sadock |first=BJ |author2=Sadock VA |year=2004 |publisher=Lippincott Williams and Wilkins |location=Philadelphia |isbn=978-0-7817-5033-2 |pages= |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/kaplansadockscon00sado/page/570 }}</ref> | |||
The core feature is severely inappropriate social relating by affected children. This can manifest itself in two ways: | |||
Critics charge that actual RAD is rather rare, and that diagnoses are often incorrect, too broadly applied and are made by unqualified persons. Some critics have further charged that RAD is a ] diagnosis for any number of unrelated behaviors that parents disapprove of in their children. | |||
# Indiscriminate and excessive attempts to receive comfort and affection from any available adult, even relative strangers (older children and adolescents may also aim attempts at peers). This may often appear as denial of comfort from anyone as well. | |||
==Classification== | |||
# Extreme reluctance to initiate or accept comfort and affection, even from familiar adults, especially when distressed.<ref name="Chaffin 2006, p. 80">Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 80. The APSAC Taskforce Report</ref> | |||
{{inappropriate tone}} | |||
# | |||
The ] specifically includes two forms of clinical presentation: | |||
While RAD occurs in relation to neglectful and abusive treatment, automatic diagnoses on this basis alone cannot be made, as children can form stable attachments and social relationships despite marked abuse and neglect. However, the instances of that ability are rare.<ref name="Rutter 2002">{{cite journal |author=Rutter M |title=Nature, nurture, and development: from evangelism through science toward policy and practice |journal=Child Dev |volume=73 |issue=1 |pages=1–21 |year=2002 |pmid=14717240 |doi=10.1111/1467-8624.00388 }}</ref> | |||
*"Inhibited" (Criterion A1), and | |||
*"disinhibited" (Criterion A2) | |||
The name of the disorder emphasizes problems with attachment but the criteria include symptoms such as failure to thrive, a lack of developmentally appropriate social responsiveness, apathy, and onset before 8 months.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=RICHTERS|first1=MARGOT MOSER|last2=VOLKMAR|first2=FRED R.|date=1 March 1994|title=Reactive Attachment Disorder of Infancy or Early Childhood|journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry|volume=33|issue=3|pages=328–332|doi=10.1097/00004583-199403000-00005|pmid=7513324|citeseerx=10.1.1.527.9988|s2cid=13058511 }}</ref> | |||
These are roughly equivalent to the ], in which 94.1 represents the "inhibited" form of the ], and 94.2 represents the "disinhibited" form. | |||
===Assessment tools=== | |||
When either classification system is used, the inhibited form tends to have more withdrawal behaviours towards a caregiver, (4) and the disinhibited more externalising behaviours. (5) | |||
There is as yet no universally accepted diagnostic protocol for reactive attachment disorder. Often a range of measures are used in research and diagnosis. Recognized assessment methods of attachment styles, difficulties or disorders include the ] (devised by ] ]),<ref name="Ainsworth et al. (1979)"/><ref name="Main & Solomon (1986)"/><ref name= "Main & Solomon 1990"/> the separation and reunion procedure and the Preschool Assessment of Attachment,<ref name= "Crittenden 1992">{{cite journal |author=Crittenden PM |title= Quality of attachment in the preschool years|journal= Development and Psychopathology |volume= 4 |issue=2 |pages= 209–41|year= 1992 |doi=10.1017/S0954579400000110|s2cid= 143894461}}</ref> the Observational Record of the Caregiving Environment,<ref name="NICHHD">{{cite journal |last1=National Institute of Child Health and Human Development |year=1996 |title=Characteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing to positive caregiving |journal=Early Childhood Research Quarterly |volume=11 |issue=3 |pages=269–306(38) |doi= 10.1016/S0885-2006(96)90009-5 |first1=D |url=https://zenodo.org/record/1260095 |author1-link=National Institute of Child Health and Human Development }}</ref> the ]<ref name="Waters & Deane 1985">Waters E, Deane K (1985). "Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood". In Bretherton I and Waters E (Eds.) ''Growing pains of attachment theory and research: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development'' 50, Serial No. 209 (1–2), pp. 41–65.</ref> and a variety of narrative techniques using ], puppets or pictures. For older children, actual interviews such as the ] and the Autobiographical Emotional Events Dialogue can be used. Caregivers may also be assessed using procedures such as the Working Model of the Child Interview.<ref name="Zeanah & Benoit 1995">{{cite journal |journal=Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America|vauthors=Zeanah CH, Benoit D | year= 1995 |title= Clinical applications of a parent perception interview in infant mental health |volume= 43|issue=3 | pages= 539–554 |doi=10.1016/S1056-4993(18)30418-8 }}</ref> | |||
More recent research also uses the ] (DAI) developed by Smyke and ] (1999).<ref name="Smyke & Zeanah (1999)">Smyke A, Zeanah CH (1999). "Disturbances of Attachment Interview". Available on the ''Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry'' website at www.jaacap.com via Article plus. Retrieved on 3 March 2008.</ref> The DAI is a semi-structured interview designed to be administered by clinicians to caregivers. It covers 12 items, namely "having a discriminated, preferred adult", "seeking comfort when distressed", "responding to comfort when offered", "social and emotional reciprocity", "emotional regulation", "checking back after venturing away from the care giver", "reticence with unfamiliar adults", "willingness to go off with relative strangers", "self-endangering behavior", "excessive clinging", "vigilance/hypercompliance" and "role reversal". This method is designed to pick up not only RAD but also the proposed new alternative categories of disorders of attachment. | |||
Many popular, informal classification systems, outside the ] and ], have been created out of ] and parental experience. Some critics have charged these informal classification systems are inaccurate, too broadly defined or applied by unqualified persons. | |||
==Causes== | |||
One popular classification system is the Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire. (6) The checklist includes 93 discrete behaviours, many of which overlap with other disorders, like ] and ]. | |||
Although increasing numbers of childhood mental health problems are being attributed to ]s,<ref>Mercer (2006), pp. 104–05.</ref> reactive attachment disorder is by definition based on a problematic history of care and social relationships. Abuse can occur alongside the required factors, but on its own does not explain attachment disorder.<ref>Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 218.</ref> It has been suggested that types of ], or constitutional response to the environment, may make some individuals susceptible to the stress of unpredictable or hostile relationships with caregivers in the early years.<ref name="Marshall & Fox (2005)">{{cite journal |year=2005 |title=Relationship between behavioral reactivity at 4 months and attachment classification at 14 months in a selected sample |journal=Infant Behavior and Development |volume=28 |issue=4 |pages=492–502 |doi= 10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.06.002 |vauthors=Marshall PJ, Fox NA }}</ref> In the absence of available and responsive caregivers it appears that most children are particularly vulnerable to developing attachment disorders.<ref name="prior">Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 219.</ref> | |||
While similar abnormal parenting may produce the two distinct forms of the disorder, inhibited and disinhibited, studies show that abuse and neglect were far more prominent and severe in the cases of RAD, disinhibited type. The issue of temperament and its influence on the development of attachment disorders has yet to be resolved. RAD has never been reported in the absence of serious environmental adversity yet outcomes for children raised in the same environment are the same.<ref name="Zeanah & Fox (2004)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Zeanah CH, Fox NA |title=Temperament and attachment disorders |journal=J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol |volume=33 |issue=1 |pages=32–41 |year=2004 |pmid=15028539 |doi=10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_4 |s2cid=9416146 }}</ref> | |||
Children who are adopted after the age of six months are at risk for attachment problems. Normal attachment develops during the child's first two to three years of life. Problems with the mother-child relationship during that time, orphanage experience, or breaks in the consistent caregiver-child relationship interfere with the normal development of a healthy and secure attachment. There are wide ranges of attachment difficulties that result in varying degrees of emotional disturbance in the child. One thing is certain; if an infant's needs are not met consistently, in a loving, nurturing way, attachment will not occur normally and this underlying problem will manifest itself in a variety of symptoms ]. | |||
When the first-year-of-life attachment-cycle is undermined, Basic Trust vs. Mistrust, in E. ]'s framework and the child’s needs are not met, and normal socializing shame is not resolved, mistrust begins to define the perspective of the child and attachment problems result. The cycle can become undermined or broken for many reasons: | |||
1. Multiple disruptions in care giving | |||
In discussing the ] basis for attachment and trauma symptoms in a seven-year ], it has been suggested that the roots of various forms of ], including RAD, ] (BPD), and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), can be found in disturbances in ] regulation. The subsequent development of higher-order ] is jeopardized and the formation of internal models is affected. Consequently, the "templates" in the mind that drive organized behavior in relationships may be impacted. The potential for "re-regulation" (modulation of emotional responses to within the normal range) in the presence of "corrective" experiences (normative caregiving) seems possible.<ref name="Heller et al. (2006)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Heller SS, Boris NW, Fuselier SH, Page T, Koren-Karie N, Miron D |title=Reactive attachment disorder in maltreated twins follow-up: from 18 months to 8 years |journal=Attach Hum Dev |volume=8 |issue=1 |pages=63–86 |year=2006 |pmid=16581624 |doi=10.1080/14616730600585177|s2cid=34947321 }}</ref> | |||
2. Post-partum depression causing an emotionally unavailable mother | |||
==Diagnosis== | |||
3. Hospitalization of the child causing separation from the parent and/or unrelieved pain. For example, stays in a NICU or repeated hospitalizations during infancy. | |||
{{See also|Attachment measures}} | |||
RAD is one of the least researched and most poorly understood disorders in the DSM. There is little systematic epidemiologic information on RAD, its course is not well established and it appears difficult to diagnose accurately.<ref name="Chaffin 2006, p. 80"/> There is a lack of clarity about the presentation of attachment disorders over the age of five years and difficulty in distinguishing between aspects of attachment disorders, disorganized attachment or the consequences of maltreatment.<ref name="prior228"/> | |||
According to the ] (AACAP), children who exhibit signs of reactive attachment disorder need a comprehensive psychiatric assessment and individualized treatment plan. The signs or symptoms of RAD may also be found in other psychiatric disorders and AACAP advises against giving a child this label or diagnosis without a comprehensive evaluation.<ref name="AACAP"> {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080203213050/http://www.aacap.org/cs/root/facts_for_families/reactive_attachment_disorder |date=3 February 2008 }} American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Facts for Families, No. 85; Updated December 2002. Retrieved on 13 February 2008.</ref> Their practice parameter states that the assessment of reactive attachment disorder requires evidence directly obtained from serial observations of the child interacting with their primary caregivers and history (as available) of the child's patterns of attachment behavior with these caregivers. It also requires observations of the child's behavior with unfamiliar adults and a comprehensive history of the child's early caregiving environment including, for example, pediatricians, teachers, or caseworkers.<ref name=AACAP-2005/> In the US, initial evaluations may be conducted by psychologists, psychiatrists, Licensed Marriage and Family Therapists, Licensed Professional Counselors, specialist Licensed Clinical Social Workers or psychiatric nurses.<ref>For examples see {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20071228050441/http://dcfswebresource.prairienet.org/resources/rad.php |date=28 December 2007 }}, DCFS, State of Illinois and (PDF), Arizona Department of Health Services, 2 October 2006. Retrieved on 23 February 2008.</ref> | |||
4. Parents who are attachment disordered, leading to neglect, abuse (physical/sexual/verbal), or inappropriate parental responses not leading to a secure/predictable relationship. | |||
In the UK, the ] (BAAF) advise that only a psychiatrist can diagnose an attachment disorder and that any assessment must include a comprehensive evaluation of the child's individual and family history.<ref name="BAAF"> {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20081002051146/http://www.baaf.org.uk/about/believes/ps4.pdf |date=2 October 2008 }} (PDF). British Association for Adoption and Fostering, Position Statement 4, 2006. Retrieved on 23 February 2008</ref> | |||
5. Genetic factors. | |||
According to the AACAP Practice Parameter (2005) the question of whether attachment disorders can reliably be diagnosed in older children and adults has not been resolved. Attachment behaviors used for the diagnosis of RAD change markedly with development and defining analogous behaviors in older children is difficult. There are no substantially validated measures of attachment in middle childhood or early adolescence.<ref name=AACAP-2005/> Assessments of RAD past school age may not be possible at all as by this time children have developed along individual lines to such an extent that early attachment experiences are only one factor among many that determine emotion and behavior.<ref>Mercer (2006), p. 116.</ref> | |||
6. Caregivers whose own needs are not met, leading to overload and lack of awareness of the infant's needs | |||
The child may develop mistrust, impeding effective attachment behavior. The developmental stages following these first three years continue to be distorted and/or retarded, and common symptoms emerge. Although I am listing several common symptoms it is very important to realize that when you are trying to parent a child with attachment difficulties you must focus on the cause of the behaviors and not on the symptoms or surface behaviors. It is the cause or motivation for the behaviors that must be your focus…otherwise you are like a doctor who treats a cough without figuring out whether the cough is caused by TB, an allergy, the flu, or lung cancer. | |||
What are the underlying causes of the various symptoms? The cause is some break in the early attachment relationship that results in difficulties trusting others. The child experiences a fear of close authentic emotional relationships because early maltreatment or other difficulties has “taught” the child that adults are not trust worthy and that the child is unloved and unlovable. Fundamentally, the cause is a developmental delay. The child may be chronologically six, ten, or fifteen, but developmentally these children are much younger. It is often useful to consider, “at what age would this behavior be normal?” Frequently you will find that the child’s behavior would be normal for a toddler. | |||
Chronic Maltreatment (abuse or neglect) or other disruptions to the normal attachment relationship cause: | |||
===Criteria=== | |||
1. Fear of intimacy | |||
ICD-10 describes reactive attachment disorder of childhood, known as RAD, and ], less well known as DAD. DSM-IV-TR also describes reactive attachment disorder of infancy or early childhood divided into two subtypes, inhibited type and disinhibited type, both known as RAD. The two classifications are similar and both include: | |||
* markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate social relatedness in most contexts (e.g., the child is avoidant or unresponsive to care when offered by caregivers or is indiscriminately affectionate with strangers);<ref name=DSMIV /> | |||
* the disturbance is not accounted for solely by ] delay and does not meet the criteria for ]; | |||
* onset before five years of age (there is no age specified before five years of age at which RAD cannot be diagnosed);<ref name=DSMIV>{{cite book|title=Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Text Revision|url=https://archive.org/details/diagnosticstatis0ed4unse_y1p2|url-access=registration|year=2000|publisher=American Psychiatric Association|isbn=978-0-89042-025-6|pages=}}</ref> | |||
* a history of significant neglect; | |||
* an implicit lack of identifiable, preferred attachment figure. | |||
ICD-10 states in relation to the inhibited form only that the syndrome probably occurs as a direct result of severe parental neglect, abuse, or serious mishandling. DSM states in relation to both forms there must be a history of "] care" defined as persistent disregard of the child's basic emotional or physical needs or repeated changes in primary caregiver that prevents the formation of a discriminatory or selective attachment that is presumed to account for the disorder. For this reason, part of the ] is the child's history of care rather than observation of symptoms. | |||
2. Overwhelming feelings of shame. (Not guilt…shame causes you to want to hide and not be seen. So, for example, some children’s chronic lying can be seen as a manifestation of this pervasive sense of shame. After all, what is a lie, but another way to hide?). | |||
In DSM-IV-TR the ''inhibited'' form is described as persistent failure to initiate or respond in a developmentally appropriate fashion to most social interactions, as manifest by excessively inhibited, hypervigilant, or highly ambivalent and contradictory responses (e.g., the child may respond to caregivers with a mixture of approach, avoidance, and resistance to comforting or may exhibit "frozen watchfulness", hypervigilance while keeping an impassive and still demeanour).<ref name= "DSM">{{cite book |title= Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders |edition=4th ed., text revision (]) |author= American Psychiatric Association |year=2000 |isbn=978-0-89042-025-6 |chapter=Diagnostic criteria for 313.89 Reactive attachment disorder of infancy or early childhood |publisher= AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC PRESS INC (DC) |location= United States}} | |||
3. Chronic feelings of being unloved | |||
</ref> Such infants do not seek or accept comfort at times of threat, alarm or distress, thus failing to maintain "proximity", an essential element of attachment behavior. The ''disinhibited'' form shows diffuse attachments as manifest by indiscriminate sociability with marked inability to exhibit appropriate selective attachments (e.g., excessive familiarity with relative strangers or lack of selectivity in choice of attachment figures).<ref name ="DSM"/> There is therefore a lack of "specificity" of attachment figure, the second basic element of attachment behavior. | |||
The ICD-10 descriptions are comparable save that ICD-10 includes in its description several elements not included in DSM-IV-TR as follows: | |||
4. Chronic feelings of being unlovable | |||
* ], (] or physical), in addition to neglect; | |||
* associated emotional disturbance; | |||
* poor social interaction with peers, aggression towards self and others, misery, and growth failure in some cases (inhibited form only); | |||
* evidence of capacity for social reciprocity and responsiveness as shown by elements of normal social relatedness in interactions with appropriately responsive, non-deviant adults (disinhibited form only). | |||
The first of these is somewhat controversial, being a commission rather than omission and because abuse in and of itself does not lead to attachment disorder. | |||
5. A distorted view of self, other, and relationships based on past maltreatment. | |||
The inhibited form has a greater tendency to ameliorate with an appropriate caregiver, while the disinhibited form is more enduring.<ref>Prior & Glaser (2006), pp. 220–21.</ref> ICD-10 states the disinhibited form "tends to persist despite marked changes in environmental circumstances". Disinhibited and inhibited are not opposites in terms of attachment disorder and can coexist in the same child.<ref name="Zeanah et al. (2004)"/> The question of whether there are two subtypes has been raised. The World Health Organization acknowledges that there is uncertainty regarding the diagnostic criteria and the appropriate subdivision.<ref name="WHO1992">World Health Organisation (1992) ''International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision (ICD-10)''. Geneva: World health Organization.</ref> One reviewer has commented on the difficulty of clarifying the core characteristics of and differences between atypical attachment styles and ways of categorizing more severe disorders of attachment.<ref>{{cite journal | author = Zilberstein K | year = 2006 | title = Clarifying core characteristics of attachment disorders | journal = ] | volume = 76 | issue = 1| pages = 55–64 | doi=10.1037/0002-9432.76.1.55| pmid = 16569127 | s2cid = 25416390 }}</ref> | |||
6. Lack of trust | |||
{{as of|2010}}, the American Psychiatric Association has proposed to redefine RAD into two distinct disorders in the DSM-V.<ref name=DSM-V>Proposed Revision Reactive Attachment Disorder, ''American Psychiatric Association'' (2012). Retrieved from http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevisions/Pages/proposedrevision.aspx?rid=120</ref> Corresponding with the inhibited type, one disorder will be reclassified as ''Reactive Attachment Disorder of Infancy and Early Childhood''.<ref name=DSMIV /> | |||
7. Feeling that nothing the child does can make a difference; hence, low motivation and poor academic performance. | |||
In regards to pathogenic care, or the type of care in which these behaviors are present, a new criterion for Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder now includes chronically harsh punishment or other types of severely inept caregiving. Relating to pathogenic care for both proposed disorders, a new criterion is rearing in atypical environments such as institutions with high child/caregiver ratios that cut down on opportunities to form attachments with a caregiver.<ref name=DSM-V /> | |||
8. A core sense of being Bad. | |||
===Differential diagnosis=== | |||
9. Difficulty asking for help | |||
The diagnostic complexities of RAD mean that careful diagnostic evaluation by a trained ] expert with particular expertise in ] is considered essential.<ref name="Hanson&Spratt2000">{{cite journal |vauthors=Hanson RF, Spratt EG |title=Reactive Attachment Disorder: what we know about the disorder and implications for treatment |journal=Child Maltreat |volume=5 |issue=2 |pages=137–45 |year=2000 |pmid=11232086 |doi=10.1177/1077559500005002005 |s2cid=21497329 }}</ref><ref name="Wilson (2001)">{{cite journal |doi=10.1080/00223980109603678 |author=Wilson SL |title=Attachment disorders: review and current status |journal=J Psychol |volume=135 |issue=1 |pages=37–51 |year=2001 |pmid=11235838 |s2cid=7226465 }}</ref><ref name="taskforce"/> Several other disorders, such as ]s, ], ]s, ] and ] share many symptoms and are often comorbid with or confused with RAD, leading to over and under diagnosis. RAD can also be confused with neuropsychiatric disorders such as ], ], ] and some genetic syndromes. Infants with this disorder can be distinguished from those with organic illness by their rapid physical improvement after hospitalization.<ref name=Sad04/> Autistic children are likely to be of normal size and weight and often exhibit a degree of intellectual disability. They are unlikely to improve upon being removed from the home.<ref name=Sad04/><ref name="Hanson&Spratt2000"/><ref name="Wilson (2001)"/><ref name="taskforce"/> | |||
===Alternative diagnosis=== | |||
10. Difficulty relying on others in a cooperative and collaborative manner. | |||
In the absence of a standardized diagnosis system, many popular, informal classification systems or checklists, outside the ] and ], were created out of clinical and parental experience within the field known as ]. These lists are unvalidated and critics state they are inaccurate, too broadly defined or applied by unqualified persons. Many are found on the websites of attachment therapists. Common elements of these lists such as lying, lack of remorse or conscience and cruelty do not form part of the diagnostic criteria under either DSM-IV-TR or ICD-10.<ref>Chaffin et al. (2006), pp. 82–83. The APSAC Taskforce Report</ref> Many children are being diagnosed with RAD because of behavioral problems that are outside the criteria.<ref name="Hanson&Spratt2000"/> There is an emphasis within attachment therapy on aggressive behavior as a symptom of what they describe as attachment disorder whereas mainstream theorists view these behaviors as comorbid, ] behaviors requiring appropriate assessment and treatment rather than attachment disorders. However, knowledge of attachment relationships can contribute to the cause, maintenance and treatment of externalizing disorders.<ref name="Gutman-Steinmetz & Crowell (2006)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Guttmann-Steinmetz S, Crowell JA |title=Attachment and externalizing disorders: a developmental psychopathology perspective |journal=J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry |volume=45 |issue=4 |pages=440–51 |year=2006 |pmid=16601649 |doi=10.1097/01.chi.0000196422.42599.63 }}</ref> | |||
The Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire or RADQ is one of the better known of these checklists and is used by attachment therapists and others.<ref name="Randolph 1996">Randolph, Elizabeth Marie. (1996). ''Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire''. Institute for Attachment, Evergreen CO.</ref> The checklist includes 93 discrete behaviours, many of which either overlap with other disorders, like conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder, or are not related to attachment difficulties. Critics assert that it is unvalidated<ref name="Mercer J.">{{cite journal |author=Mercer J |title=Coercive restraint therapies: a dangerous alternative mental health intervention |journal=MedGenMed |volume=7 |issue=3 |pages=6 |year=2005 |pmid=16369232 |url=http://www.medscape.com/viewarticle/508956 |pmc=1681667 }}</ref> and lacks ].<ref name="Cappelletty et al. (2005)">{{cite journal |title= Correlates of the Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire (RADQ) in a Sample of Children in Foster Placement |year=2005 |journal=Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal |volume=22 |issue=1 |pages=71–84 |doi=10.1007/s10560-005-2556-2 |quote=The findings showed that children in foster care have reported symptoms within the range typical of children not involved in foster care. The conclusion is that the RADQ has limited usefulness due to its lack of specificity with implications for treatment of children in foster care |vauthors=Cappelletty G, Brown M, Shumate S |s2cid=143743052 }}</ref> | |||
==Framework== | |||
The theoretical framework for Reactive Attachment Disorder is based on work by Bowlby, Ainsworth and Spitz, from the ] to the ]. Some critics charge later therapists have misused or misrepresented Ainsworth's or Bowlby's work. | |||
==Treatment== | |||
In contrast, the popular framework tends to be more eclectic, using many sources from birth/prenatal psychology (7), the ] (8) (where issues of ] and ] in ] are especially relevant) to transactional analysis and ethology. | |||
{{Main|Attachment-based therapy (children)|Attachment therapy}} | |||
Assessing the child's safety is an essential first step that determines whether future intervention can take place in the family unit or whether the child should be removed to a safe situation. Interventions may include psychosocial support services for the family unit (including financial or domestic aid, housing and social work support), psychotherapeutic interventions (including treating parents for mental illness, ], individual therapy), education (including training in basic parenting skills and child development), and monitoring of the child's safety within the family environment<ref name=Sad04/> | |||
In 2005 the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry laid down guidelines (devised by N.W. Boris and C.H. Zeanah) based on its published parameters for the diagnosis and treatment of RAD.<ref name=AACAP-2005>{{cite journal |journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry |date=November 2005 |volume=44 |issue=11 |pages=1206–19 |title= Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder of infancy and early childhood |last1=Boris |first1=Neil W. |last2=Zeanah |first2=Charles H. |author3=Work Group on Quality Issues |pmid=16239871 |url=http://www.jaacap.com/article/S0890-8567(09)62229-2/fulltext |doi=10.1097/01.chi.0000177056.41655.ce |doi-access=free }}</ref> Recommendations in the guidelines include the following: | |||
The development of diagnostic criteria was further operationalised by Zeanah and O’Connor throughout the ] and ]<code id="ref_9_back">]]</code>, and through greater awareness garnered from the adoption of institutionalised children from ], ] and ], and also ] in ] and other nations. | |||
# "The most important intervention for young children diagnosed with reactive attachment disorder and who lack an attachment to a discriminated caregiver is for the clinician to advocate for providing the child with an emotionally available attachment figure." | |||
# "Although the diagnosis of reactive attachment disorder is based on symptoms displayed by the child, assessing the caregiver's attitudes toward and perceptions about the child is important for treatment selection." | |||
# "Children with reactive attachment disorder are presumed to have grossly disturbed internal models for relating to others. After ensuring that the child is in a safe and stable placement, effective attachment treatment must focus on creating positive interactions with caregivers." | |||
# "Children who meet criteria for reactive attachment disorder and who display aggressive and oppositional behavior require adjunctive (additional) treatments." | |||
Mainstream prevention programs and treatment approaches for attachment difficulties or disorders for infants and younger children are based on attachment theory and concentrate on increasing the responsiveness and sensitivity of the caregiver, or if that is not possible, placing the child with a different caregiver.<ref name=AACAP-2005/><ref>Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 231.</ref><ref name="BakermansKranenburg et al. (2003)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Bakermans-Kranenburg M, van IJzendoorn M, Juffer F |year=2003 |title=Less Is More: Meta-Analyses of Sensitivity and Attachment Interventions in Early Childhood |journal=Psychological Bulletin |volume=129 |pages=195–215 |url=http://www.childandfamilystudies.leidenuniv.nl/content_docs/agp/Publicaties/baketal03pb.pdf |access-date=2 February 2008 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195 |pmid=12696839 |issue=2 |s2cid=7504386 }}</ref> These approaches are mostly in the process of being evaluated. The programs invariably include a detailed assessment of the attachment status or caregiving responses of the adult caregiver as attachment is a two-way process involving attachment behavior and caregiver response. Some of these treatment or prevention programs are specifically aimed at foster carers rather than parents, as the attachment behaviors of infants or children with attachment difficulties often do not elicit appropriate caregiver responses.<ref name="Stovall & Dozier (2000)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Stovall KC, Dozier M |title=The development of attachment in new relationships: single subject analyses for 10 foster infants |journal=Dev. Psychopathol. |volume=12 |issue=2 |pages=133–56 |year=2000 |pmid=10847621 |doi=10.1017/S0954579400002029 |s2cid=746807 }}</ref> Approaches include "Watch, wait and wonder,"<ref name="Cohen et al. 1999">{{cite journal |vauthors=Cohen N, Muir E, Lojkasek M, Muir R, Parker C, Barwick M, Brown M |year=1999 |title=Watch, wait and wonder: testing the effectiveness of a new approach to mother-infant psychotherapy |journal=Infant Mental Health Journal |volume=20 |issue= 4 |pages=429–51 |doi= 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199924)20:4<429::AID-IMHJ5>3.0.CO;2-Q}}</ref> manipulation of sensitive responsiveness,<ref name="van den Boom 1994">{{cite journal |author=van den Boom D |year=1994 |title=The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: an experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants |journal=Child Development |volume=65 |pages=1457–77 |doi=10.2307/1131277 |pmid=7982362 |issue=5 |jstor=1131277}}</ref><ref name="van den Boom 1995">{{cite journal |author=van den Boom D |title=Do first-year intervention effects endure? Follow-up during toddlerhood of a sample of Dutch irritable infants |journal=Child Dev |volume=66 |issue=6 |pages=1798–816 |year=1995 |pmid=8556900 |doi=10.2307/1131911 |jstor=1131911}}</ref> modified "Interaction Guidance",<ref name="Benoit et al.">{{cite journal |vauthors=Benoit D, Madigan S, Lecce S, Shea B, Goldberg S |year=2002 |title=Atypical maternal behaviour toward feeding disordered infants before and after intervention |journal=Infant Mental Health Journal |volume=22 |issue= 6|pages=611–26 |doi= 10.1002/imhj.1022 }}</ref> "Clinician-Assisted Videofeedback Exposure Sessions (CAVES)",<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Schechter DS, Myers MM, Brunelli SA, etal | title = Traumatized mothers can change their minds about their toddlers: Understanding how a novel use of videofeedback supports positive change of maternal attributions | journal = Infant Mental Health Journal | volume = 27 | issue = 5 | pages = 429–447 |date=September 2006 | pmid = 18007960 | pmc = 2078524 | doi = 10.1002/imhj.20101 }}</ref> "Preschool Parent Psychotherapy",<ref name="Toth et al. (2002)">{{cite journal |last=Toth |first=S |author2=Maughan A |author3=Manly J |author4=Spagnola M |author5=Cicchetti D |year=2002 |title=The relative efficacy of two in altering maltreated preschool children's representational models: implications for attachment theory |journal=Development and Psychopathology |volume=14 |pages=877–908 |doi= 10.1017/S095457940200411X |pmid=12549708 |issue=4 |s2cid=30792141 }}</ref> "Circle of Security",<ref name="Marvin et al. (2002)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Marvin R, Cooper G, Hoffman K, Powell B |date=April 2002 |title=The Circle of Security project: Attachment-based intervention with caregiver – pre-school child dyads |journal=Attachment & Human Development |volume=4 |issue=1 |pages=107–24 |url=http://www.circleofsecurity.org/docs/languages/08%20AHD%20final.pdf |access-date=2 February 2008 |doi=10.1080/14616730252982491 |pmid=12065033 |s2cid=25815919 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080227161855/http://www.circleofsecurity.org/docs/languages/08%20AHD%20final.pdf |archive-date=27 February 2008 }}</ref><ref name="Cooper et al. (2005)">Cooper G, Hoffman K, Powell B and Marvin R (2005). "The Circle of Security Intervention; differential diagnosis and differential treatment". In Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L and Greenberg MT (eds.) ''Enhancing Early Attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy''. pp. 127–51. The Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. (2005) {{ISBN|1-59385-470-6}}.</ref> "Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up" (ABC),<ref name="Dozier et al. (2005)">Dozier M, Lindheim O and Ackerman JP (2005) "Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-Up: An intervention targeting empirically identified needs of foster infants". In Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L and Greenberg MT (Eds.) ''Enhancing Early Attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy'' pp. 178–94. Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. (2005) {{ISBN|1-59385-470-6}} (pbk)</ref> the New Orleans Intervention,<ref name="Zeanah & Larrieu">{{cite journal |vauthors=Zeanah CH, Larrieu JA |title=Intensive intervention for maltreated infants and toddlers in foster care |journal=Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am |volume=7 |issue=2 |pages=357–71 |year=1998 |pmid=9894069|doi=10.1016/S1056-4993(18)30246-3 }}</ref><ref name="Larrieu & Zeanah (2004)">Larrieu JA, Zeanah CH (2004). "Treating infant-parent relationships in the context of maltreatment: An integrated, systems approach". In Saner A, McDonagh S and Roesenblaum K (Eds.) ''Treating parent-infant relationship problems '' pp. 243–64. New York. Guilford Press. {{ISBN|1-59385-245-2}}</ref><ref name="Zeanah & Smyke">Zeanah CH, Smyke AT (2005) "Building Attachment Relationships Following Maltreatment and Severe Deprivation". In Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L and Greenberg MT (Eds) ''Enhancing Early Attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy'' The Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. (2005) pp. 195–216. {{ISBN|1-59385-470-6}} (pbk)</ref> and parent–child psychotherapy.<ref name="Leiberman et al. 2000">Lieberman AF, Silverman R, Pawl JH (2000). "Infant-parent psychotherapy". In Zeanah CH (Ed.) ''Handbook of infant mental health'' (2nd ed.) p. 432. New York: Guilford Press. {{ISBN|1-59385-171-5}}</ref> Other treatment methods include Developmental, Individual-difference, and Relationship-based therapy (DIR, also referred to as Floor Time) by ], although DIR is primarily directed to treatment of pervasive developmental disorders.<ref name="ICD">{{cite web | publisher = Interdisciplinary Council on Developmental & Learning Disorders | year = 2007 | url = http://www.icdl.com/dirFloortime/overview/index.shtml | title = Dir/floortime model | access-date = 2 February 2008 | url-status = dead | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20080225132429/http://www.icdl.com/dirFloortime/overview/index.shtml | archive-date = 25 February 2008 | df = dmy-all }}</ref> | |||
Psychiatrist ] has done an outcome study, the largest of its kind, called the ]. ] has done another outcome study, and ] there are many in process. (10) | |||
The relevance of these approaches to intervention with fostered and adopted children with RAD or older children with significant histories of maltreatment is unclear.<ref name="Newman & Mares (2007)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Newman L, Mares S |title=Recent advances in the theories of and interventions with attachment disorders |journal=Current Opinion in Psychiatry |volume=20 |issue=4 |pages=343–8 |year=2007 |pmid=17551348 |doi=10.1097/YCO.0b013e3281bc0d08|s2cid=34000485 }}</ref> | |||
==Diagnosis== | |||
In mainstream medical practice, Reactive Attachment Disorder is most often diagnosed by ] or ]. ] may be called in when there is ] involved. | |||
===Attachment therapy=== | |||
It is important to note that there are various "attachment styles" that are not pathological, and attachment issues that may run anywhere across the continuum. "Reactive Attachment Disorder" has been traditionally used to describe a "severe disturbance in the attachment between caregiver and child that is of long standing and applicable/observable in all contexts in which the child interacts." | |||
{{main|Attachment therapy}} | |||
The terms ], attachment problems, and attachment therapy, although increasingly used, have no clear, specific, or consensus definitions. However, the terms and therapies often are applied to children who are maltreated, particularly those in the foster care, kinship care, or adoption systems, and related populations such as children adopted internationally from orphanages.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Chaffin|first1=Mark|last2=Hanson|first2=Rochelle|last3=Saunders|first3=Benjamin E.|last4=Nichols|first4=Todd|last5=Barnett|first5=Douglas|last6=Zeanah|first6=Charles|last7=Berliner|first7=Lucy|last8=Egeland|first8=Byron|last9=Newman|first9=Elana|date=1 February 2006|title=Report of the APSAC Task Force on Attachment Therapy, Reactive Attachment Disorder, and Attachment Problems|journal=Child Maltreatment|language=en|volume=11|issue=1|pages=76–89|doi=10.1177/1077559505283699|issn=1077-5595|pmid=16382093|s2cid=11443880}}</ref> | |||
Some of the "attachment styles" are named: "avoidant," "aggressive," "ambivalent" and "disorganised/mixed". There is often a blending of several "attachment styles" in an individual. | |||
Outside the mainstream programs is a ] generally known as attachment therapy, a subset of techniques (and accompanying novel diagnosis) for supposed attachment disorders including RAD. These "attachment disorders" use diagnostic criteria or symptom lists different from criteria under ICD-10 or DSM-IV-TR, or to attachment behaviors. Those with "attachment disorder" are said to lack empathy and remorse. | |||
Reactive Attachment Disorder affects the "basic working model." | |||
Many parents report that they do not understand what their child is thinking or feeling at any given time. Some diagnosticians of RAD argue these sensations are due to the child giving inconsistent, "low-level" or mixed signals. | |||
Treatments of this pseudoscientific disorder are called "Attachment therapy". In general, these therapies are aimed at adopted or fostered children with a view to creating attachment in these children to their new caregivers. The theoretical base is broadly a combination of ] and ], accompanied by parenting methods which emphasize ] and parental control.<ref name="Chaffin 2006 pp. 79">Chaffin et al. (2006), pp. 79–80. The APSAC Taskforce Report.</ref> There is considerable criticism of this form of treatment and diagnosis as it is largely unvalidated and has developed outside the scientific mainstream.<ref>Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 85. The APSAC Taskforce Report</ref> There is little or no ] and techniques vary from non-coercive ] work to more extreme forms of physical, confrontational and coercive techniques, of which the best known are ], ], ] and the ]. These forms of the therapy may well involve physical restraint, the deliberate provocation of rage and anger in the child by physical and verbal means including deep tissue massage, aversive tickling, enforced eye contact and verbal confrontation, and being pushed to revisit earlier trauma.<ref>Chaffin et al. (2006), pp. 78–83. The APSAC Taskforce Report.</ref><ref name="Speltz 2002">{{cite journal|author=Speltz ML |year=2002 |title=Description, History and Critique of Corrective Attachment Therapy |journal=The APSAC Advisor |volume=14 |issue=3 |pages=4–8 |url=http://www.kidscomefirst.info/Speltz.pdf |access-date=3 March 2008 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080414011820/http://www.kidscomefirst.info/Speltz.pdf |archive-date=14 April 2008 }}</ref> Critics maintain that these therapies are not within the attachment paradigm, are potentially abusive,<ref>Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 267.</ref> and are ] to attachment theory.<ref name="O'Connor & Nilsen"/> The APSAC Taskforce Report of 2006 notes that many of these therapies concentrate on changing the child rather than the caregiver.<ref>Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 79. The APSAC Taskforce Report.</ref> Children may be described as "RADs", "Radkids" or "Radishes" and dire predictions may be made as to their supposedly violent futures if they are not treated with attachment therapy.<ref name="Chaffin 2006 pp. 79"/> The Mayo Clinic, a well known U.S. non-profit medical practice and medical research group, cautions against consulting with mental health providers who promote these types of methods and offer evidence to support their techniques; to date, this evidence base is not published within reputable medical or mental health journals.<ref>{{cite web|title=Treatments and drugs|url=http://www.mayoclinic.com/health/reactive-attachment-disorder/DS00988/DSECTION=treatments-and-drugs|publisher=Mayo Clinic Staff}}</ref> | |||
==Intervention== | |||
Many caregivers and therapists, say, however, that "traditional therapies" do not always work on those who have Reactive Attachment Disorder. | |||
==Prognosis== | |||
Evidence based approaches do exist for the effective treatment of RAD. One important study found that "usual treatments" for RAD are ineffective, while the intervention under investigation, Dyadic Developmental Psychotherpay (10) (11), was effective. (see "Treatment for Children with Trauma-Attachment Disorders: Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy," ''Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal''. '''12'''(6), December 2005). | |||
The AACAP guidelines state that children with reactive attachment disorder are presumed to have grossly disturbed internal models for relating to others.<ref name=AACAP-2005/> However, the course of RAD is not well studied and there have been few efforts to examine symptom patterns over time. The few existing ] (dealing with developmental change with age over a period of time) involve only children from poorly run Eastern European institutions.<ref name=AACAP-2005/> | |||
Findings from the studies of children from Eastern European orphanages indicate that persistence of the inhibited pattern of RAD is rare in children adopted out of institutions into normative care-giving environments. However, there is a close association between duration of deprivation and severity of attachment disorder behaviors.<ref name="Connor & Rutter (2000)">{{cite journal |vauthors=O'Connor TG, Rutter M |title=Attachment disorder behavior following early severe deprivation: extension and longitudinal follow-up. English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team |journal=J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry |volume=39 |issue=6 |pages=703–12 |year=2000 |pmid=10846304 |doi=10.1097/00004583-200006000-00008}}</ref> The quality of attachments that these children form with subsequent care-givers may be compromised, but they probably no longer meet criteria for inhibited RAD.<ref name="O'Connor et al. (2003)">{{cite journal |vauthors=O'Connor TG, Marvin RS, Rutter M, Olrick JT, Britner PA |title=Child-parent attachment following early institutional deprivation|journal=Dev. Psychopathol. |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=19–38 |year=2003 |pmid=12848433 |doi=10.1017/S0954579403000026|s2cid=33936291}}</ref> The same group of studies suggests that a minority of adopted, institutionalized children exhibit persistent indiscriminate sociability even after more normative caregiving environments are provided.<ref name="Heller et al. (2006)"/> Indiscriminate sociability may persist for years, even among children who subsequently exhibit preferred attachment to their new caregivers. Some exhibit ] and attention problems as well as difficulties in peer relationships.<ref name="O'Connor et al.(1999)">{{cite journal |author=O'Connor TG, Bredenkamp D, Rutter M, & The English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team |year=1999 |title=Attachment disturbances and disorders in children exposed to early severe deprivation |journal=] |volume=20 |pages=10–29 |doi= 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199921)20:1<10::AID-IMHJ2>3.0.CO;2-S}}</ref> In the only longitudinal study that has followed children with indiscriminate behavior into adolescence, these children were significantly more likely to exhibit poor peer relationships.<ref name="Hodges & Tizard (1989)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Hodges J, Tizard B |title=Social and family relationships of ex-institutional adolescents |journal=J Child Psychol Psychiatry |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=77–97 |year=1989 |pmid=2925822 |doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00770.x}}</ref> | |||
===Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy=== | |||
] is an effective and evidence-based treatment developed by Daniel Hughes, Ph.D., (Hughes, 2005, Hughes, 2004, Hughes, 2003; Hughes, 1997)(13-15). Its basic principals are described by Hughes (2003) and summarized as follows: | |||
Studies of children who were reared in institutions have suggested that they are inattentive and overactive, no matter what quality of care they received. In one investigation, some institution-reared boys were reported to be inattentive, overactive, and markedly unselective in their social relationships, while girls, foster-reared children, and some institution-reared children were not. It is not yet clear whether these behaviors should be considered as part of disordered attachment.<ref name="Roy et al.(2004)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Roy P, Rutter M, Pickles A |year=2004 |title=Institutional care: Associations between overactivity and lack of selectivity in social relationships|journal=Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry |volume=45 |issue=4 |pages=866–73 |doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00278.x |pmid=15056316}}</ref> | |||
1. A focus on both the caregivers and therapists own attachment strategies. Previous research (Dozier, 2001,(16) Tyrell 1999 (17)) has shown the importance of the caregivers and therapists state of mind for the success of interventions. | |||
2. Therapist and caregiver are attuned to the child’s subjective experience and reflect this back to the child. In the process of maintaining an intersubjective attuned connection with the child, the therapist and caregiver help the child regulate affect and construct a coherent autobiographical narrative. | |||
3. Sharing of subjective experiences. | |||
4. Use of PACE and PLACE are essential to healing. | |||
5. Directly address the inevitable misattunements and conflicts that arise in interpersonal relationships. | |||
6. Caregivers use attachment-facilitating interventions. | |||
7. Use of a variety of interventions, including cognitive-behavioral strategies. | |||
There is one case study on maltreated twins published in 1999 with a follow-up in 2006. This study assessed the twins between the ages of 19 and 36 months, during which time they had multiple moves and placements.<ref name="Hinshaw-Fusilier et al. (1999)">{{cite journal|last1=Hinshaw-Fuselier|first1=Sarah|last2=Boris|first2=Neil W.|last3=Zeanah|first3=Charles H.|title=Reactive attachment disorder in maltreated twins|journal=Infant Mental Health Journal|volume=20|issue=1| year=1999| pages=42–59|doi=10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199921)20:1<42::AID-IMHJ4>3.0.CO;2-B}}</ref> The paper explores the similarities, differences and comorbidity of RAD, disorganized attachment and post traumatic stress disorder. The girl showed signs of the inhibited form of RAD while the boy showed signs of the indiscriminate form. It was noted that the diagnosis of RAD ameliorated with better care but symptoms of post traumatic stress disorder and signs of disorganized attachment came and went as the infants progressed through multiple placement changes. At age three, some lasting relationship disturbance was evident. | |||
PACE refers to the therapist setting a healing pace by being playful, accepting, curious, and empathic. PLACE refers to the parent creating a healing environment by being playful, loving accepting, curious, and empathic. These ideas are described more fully below. | |||
In the follow-up case study when the twins were aged three and eight years, the lack of longitudinal research on maltreated as opposed to institutionalized children was again highlighted. The girl's symptoms of disorganized attachment had developed into controlling behaviors—a well-documented outcome. The boy still exhibited self-endangering behaviors, not within RAD criteria but possibly within "secure base distortion", (where the child has a preferred familiar caregiver, but the relationship is such that the child cannot use the adult for safety while gradually exploring the environment). At age eight the children were assessed with a variety of measures including those designed to access representational systems, or the child's "internal working models". The twins' symptoms were indicative of different trajectories. The girl showed externalizing symptoms (particularly deceit), contradictory reports of current functioning, chaotic personal narratives, struggles with friendships, and emotional disengagement with her caregiver, resulting in a clinical picture described as "quite concerning". The boy still evidenced self-endangering behaviors as well as avoidance in relationships and emotional expression, separation anxiety and impulsivity and attention difficulties. It was apparent that life stressors had impacted each child differently. The ] used were considered helpful in tracking how early attachment disruption is associated with later expectations about relationships.<ref name="Heller et al. (2006)"/> | |||
] interventions flow from several theoretical and empirical lines (Becker-Weidman & Shell, 2005). Attachment theory (Bowlby, 1980, Bowlby, 1988) provides the theoretical foundation for Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy. | |||
One paper using questionnaires found that children aged three to six, diagnosed with RAD, scored lower on empathy but higher on ] (regulating your behavior to "look good"). These differences were especially pronounced based on ratings by parents, and suggested that children with RAD may systematically report their personality traits in overly positive ways. Their scores also indicated considerably more behavioral problems than scores of the control children.<ref name="Hall & Geher (2003)">{{cite journal |doi=10.1080/00223980309600605 |vauthors=Hall SE, Geher G |title=Behavioral and personality characteristics of children with reactive attachment disorder|journal=J Psychol |volume=137 |issue=2 |pages=145–62 |year=2003 |pmid=12735525|s2cid=32015193 }}</ref> | |||
This treatment has been found to produce meaurable and sustained improvement in children diagnosed with Reactive Attachment Disorder (Becker-Weidman, 2005)(12). In that study it was found that other forms of treatment, such as individual therapy or play therapy did not produce any improvement; thus indicating that Dyadic Developmental Psychotheray is effective while other forms of treatment are not effective for this disorder. | |||
== |
==Epidemiology== | ||
Epidemiological data are limited, but reactive attachment disorder appears to be very uncommon.<ref name="DSM-IV-TR 2000 p. 129"/> The ] of RAD is unclear but it is probably quite rare, other than in populations of children being reared in the most extreme, deprived settings such as some orphanages.<ref name="prior" /> There is little systematically gathered ] information on RAD.<ref name="Hanson&Spratt2000"/> A cohort study of 211 Copenhagen children to the age of 18 months found a prevalence of 0.9%.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Skovgaard AM, Houmann T, Christiansen E, etal |year=2007 |title= The prevalence of mental health problems in children 1½ years of age – the Copenhagen Child Cohort 2000 |journal= J Child Psychol Psychiatry |volume=48 |issue=1 |pages=62–70 |doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01659.x |pmid= 17244271}}</ref> | |||
Critcs charge that some treatments for RAD are abusive and improper. As is true for nearly all therapies, there are practitioners who are not properly trained or who use unproven interventions. Medical malpractice is a significant problem. Any coercive intervention should be considered unacceptable. | |||
Attachment disorders tend to occur in a definable set of contexts such as within some types of institutions, in the presence of repeated changes of primary caregiver or of extremely neglectful identifiable primary caregivers who show persistent disregard for the child's basic attachment needs, but not all children raised in these conditions develop an attachment disorder.<ref>Prior & Glaser (2006), pp. 218–19.</ref> Studies undertaken on children from Eastern European orphanages from the mid-1990s showed significantly higher levels of both forms of RAD and of insecure patterns of attachment in the institutionalized children, regardless of how long they had been there.<ref name="Chisholm et al. (1995)"/><ref name=" Smyke et al.(2002)"/><ref name="Zeanah & Smyke et al.(2005)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Koga SF, Carlson E |title=Attachment in institutionalized and community children in Romania|journal=Child Dev |volume=76 |issue=5 |pages=1015–28 |year=2005 |pmid=16149999 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00894.x|citeseerx=10.1.1.417.6482}}</ref> It would appear that children in institutions like these are unable to form selective attachments to their caregivers. The difference between the institutionalized children and the control group had lessened in the follow-up study three years later, although the institutionalized children continued to show significantly higher levels of indiscriminate friendliness.<ref name="Chisholm et al. (1995)">{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1017/S0954579400006507 | last1 = Chisholm | first1 = K | last2 = Carter | first2 = M | last3 = Ames | first3 = E | last4 = Morison | first4 = S | year = 1995 | title = Attachment Security and indiscriminately friendly behavior in children adopted from Romanian orphanages | url = http://summit.sfu.ca/item/6999| journal = Development and Psychopathology | volume = 7 | issue = 2| pages = 283–94 | s2cid = 145524717 }}</ref><ref name=" Chisholm (1998)">{{cite journal |author=Chisholm K |title=A three-year follow-up of attachment and indiscriminate friendliness in children adopted from Romanian orphanages |journal=Child Dev |volume=69 |issue=4 |pages=1092–106 |year=1998 |pmid=9768488 |doi=10.2307/1132364 |jstor=1132364}}</ref> However, even among children raised in the most deprived institutional conditions the majority did not show symptoms of this disorder.<ref name="Connor & Rutter (2000)"/> | |||
While it is true that certain radical and unsupervised therapies have resulted in horrific tragedy, what is considered standard treatment for disorders of attachment, such as Theraplay or ] today is quite different. (12) These treatments focus on addressing the underlying trauma and resulting distortions in internal working models that cause the disorder of attachment. The focus is on developing an emotionally sensitive and responseive relationship with the child within which the attachment relationship can be remediated. Current research in brain physiology has led to the introduction of new techniques and tools, such as neurofeedback and ], as a means of understanding physical abnormalities in brain functionality and offering treatment solutions to these severely hurting children. | |||
==References== | |||
A 2002 study of children in residential nurseries in ], in which the DAI was used, challenged the current DSM and ICD conceptualizations of disordered attachment and showed that inhibited and disinhibited disorders could coexist in the same child.<ref name="Smyke et al.(2002)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Smyke AT, Dumitrescu A, Zeanah CH |title=Attachment disturbances in young children. I: The continuum of caretaking casualty|journal=J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry |volume=41 |issue=8 |pages=972–82 |year=2002 |pmid=12162633 |doi=10.1097/00004583-200208000-00016|s2cid=7359043}}</ref> | |||
#Alston, John. (2000) ''Characteristics of Attention Deficit Disorder, Bipolar I Disorder and Reactive Attachment Disorder''. | |||
#Alston, John. (2000) op cit. | |||
#Support for the lower estimate given here: Boris N. W, Zeanah C. et al (1998) ''Attachment Disorders in Infancy and Early Childhood: A Preliminary Investigation of Diagnostic Criteria''. ''American Journal of Psychiatry'' February 1998. (The actual figure for their preliminary report was 42%). | |||
#Support for the higher estimate given here: Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, Greenberg MT, & Marvin RS: ''An organizational perspective on attachment beyond infancy''. In: ''Attachment in the Preschool Years''. Ed. Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, & Cummings EM, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1990. (Cited: Becker-Weidman , ''Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy: An Effective Treatment for Children with Trauma-Attachment Disorders'') | |||
#Ames, Elinor ''Recommendations from the Final Report: The Development of Romanian Orphanage Children Adopted to Canada'' (1997) cited in Hanlon L, Tepper T and Sanstrom S (Eds) ''International Adoption-Challenges and Opportunities'' (1999) | |||
#Ames, Elinor op cit. | |||
#Randolph, Elizabeth Marie. (1996) ''Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire'':Institute for Attachment, Evergreen CO. | |||
#See especially Speltz (2002) ''Description, History, and Critique of Corrective Attachment Therapy''. ''The APSAC Advisor'' '''14'''(3), 4-8 | |||
#The given reference is not to demean the good work of subcommittees from the DSM-III onwards; nor their counterparts in ICD or the many others who have done clinical and research work. O'Connor and Zeanah (2003) "Attachment disorders and assessment approaches ''Attachment and Human Development'' '''5'''(3)223-244:Taylor and Francis | |||
#"Treatment for Children with Trauma-Attachment Disorders: Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy" ''Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal''. '''12'''(6), December 2005. | |||
#''Creating Capacity For Attachment'', (Eds.) Arthur Becker-Weidman, Ph.D., & Deborah Shell, MA, Wood 'N' Barnes, OK: 2005. | |||
#As of 2004, these US states have forbidden coercive treatments. These states permit the use of non-coercive therapies for the treatment of Disorders of Attachment, such as Theraplay or Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy: ], ] (State), ], ], ] and ], as indicated by action by the respective ], including ]. | |||
#Hughes, D. (1997). Facilitating developmental attachment. NJ: Jason Aronson. | |||
#Hughes, D. (2004). An attachment-based treatment of maltreated children and young people. Attachment & Human Development, 3, 263–278. | |||
#Hughes, D. (2003). Psychological intervention for the spectrum of attachment disorders and intrafamilial trauma. Attachment & Human Development, 5, 271–279.Hughes, D., “The Development of Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy.” In Becker-Weidman, A., & Shell, D., (Eds.), Creating Capacity for Attachment. OK: Wood ‘N’ Barnes, 2005, pp vii – xvii. | |||
#Dozier, M. Stovall, K.C., Albus, K.E., & Bates, B. (2001) Attachment for Infants in Foster Care: The Role of Caregiver State of Mind. Child Development, 70, 1467-1477. | |||
#Tyrell, C., Dozier, M., Teague, G.B. & Fallot, R. (1999). Effective treatment relationships or persons with serious psychiatric disorders: the importance of attachment states of mind. | |||
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 725-733. | |||
There are two studies on the incidence of RAD relating to high risk and maltreated children in the U.S. Both used ICD, DSM and the DAI. The first, in 2004, reported that children from the maltreatment sample were significantly more likely to meet criteria for one or more attachment disorders than children from the other groups, however this was mainly the proposed new classification of disrupted attachment disorder rather than the DSM or ICD classified RAD or DAD.<ref name="Boris et al.(2004)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Boris NW, Hinshaw-Fuselier SS, Smyke AT, Scheeringa MS, Heller SS, Zeanah CH |title=Comparing criteria for attachment disorders: establishing reliability and validity in high-risk samples |journal=J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry |volume=43 |issue=5 |pages=568–77 |year=2004 |pmid=15100563 |doi=10.1097/00004583-200405000-00010 }}</ref> The second study, also in 2004, attempted to ascertain the prevalence of RAD and whether it could be reliably identified in ''maltreated'' rather than ''neglected'' toddlers. Of the 94 maltreated toddlers in foster care, 35% were identified as having ICD RAD and 22% as having ICD DAD, and 38% fulfilled the DSM criteria for RAD.<ref name="Zeanah et al. (2004)"/> This study found that RAD could be reliably identified and also that the inhibited and disinhibited forms were not independent. However, there are some methodological concerns with this study. A number of the children identified as fulfilling the criteria for RAD did in fact have a preferred attachment figure.<ref>Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 215.</ref> | |||
==External links == | |||
* | |||
* | |||
* | |||
It has been suggested by some within the field of attachment therapy that RAD may be quite prevalent because severe child maltreatment, which is known to increase risk for RAD, is prevalent and because children who are severely abused may exhibit behaviors similar to RAD behaviors.<ref name="taskforce">Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 81. The APSAC Taskforce Report</ref> The APSAC Taskforce consider this inference to be flawed and questionable.<ref name="taskforce"/> Severely abused children may exhibit similar behaviors to RAD behaviors but there are several far more common and demonstrably treatable diagnoses which may better account for these difficulties.<ref name="dsm">] ] 1994, as discussed in Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 81.</ref> Further, many children experience severe maltreatment and do not develop clinical disorders.<ref name="dsm"/> ] is a common and normal human characteristic.<ref name="Bonanno">{{cite journal |author=Bonanno GA|year=2004 |title=Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? |journal=American Psychologist |volume=59 |issue=1 |pages=20–28 |url=http://www.nh.gov/safety/divisions/bem/behavhealth/documents/loss_trauma.pdf|access-date=26 January 2008 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.20 |pmid=14736317 |s2cid=6296189 }}</ref> RAD does not underlie all or even most of the behavioral and emotional problems seen in ], ], or children who are maltreated and rates of child abuse and/or neglect or problem behaviors are not a benchmark for estimates of RAD.<ref name="taskforce"/> | |||
] | |||
There are few data on comorbid conditions, but there are some conditions that arise in the same circumstances in which RAD arises, such as institutionalization or maltreatment. These are principally developmental delays and language disorders associated with neglect.<ref name=AACAP-2005/> Conduct disorders, oppositional defiant disorder, anxiety disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder and social phobia share many symptoms and are often comorbid with or confused with RAD.<ref name="taskforce"/><ref name="Hinshaw-Fusilier et al. (1999)"/> Attachment disorder behaviors amongst institutionalized children are correlated with attentional and conduct problems and cognitive levels but nonetheless appear to index a distinct set of symptoms and behaviors.<ref name="Connor & Rutter (2000)"/> | |||
==History== | |||
{{Main|Attachment theory|Attachment in children}} | |||
Reactive attachment disorder first made its appearance in standard ] of psychological disorders in DSM-III, 1980, following an accumulation of evidence on institutionalized children. The criteria included a requirement of onset before the age of 8 months and was equated with ]. Both these features were dropped in DSM-III-R, 1987. Instead, onset was changed to being within the first 5 years of life and the disorder itself was divided into two subcategories, inhibited and disinhibited. These changes resulted from further research on maltreated and institutionalized children and remain in the current version, DSM-IV, 1994, and its 2000 text revision, DSM-IV-TR, as well as in ICD-10, 1992. Both nosologies focus on young children who are not merely at increased risk for subsequent disorders but are already exhibiting clinical disturbance.<ref name="Zean96">{{cite journal |author=Zeanah CH |title=Beyond insecurity: a reconceptualization of attachment disorders of infancy |journal=J Consult Clin Psychol |volume=64 |issue=1 |pages=42–52 |year=1996 |pmid=8907083 |doi=10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.42 }}</ref> | |||
The broad theoretical framework for current versions of RAD is ], based on work conducted from the 1940s to the 1980s by ], ] and ]. Attachment theory is a framework that employs ], ] and ] concepts to explain social behaviors typical of young children. Attachment theory focuses on the tendency of infants or children to ''seek proximity'' to a ''particular attachment figure'' (familiar caregiver), in situations of alarm or distress, behavior which appears to have survival value.<ref>Bowlby (1997 edition) pp. 224–27.</ref> This is known as a discriminatory or selective attachment. Subsequently, the child begins to use the caregiver as a base of security from which to explore the environment, returning periodically to the familiar person. Attachment is not the same as love and/or affection although they are often associated. Attachment and attachment ] tend to develop between the ages of six months and three years. ] who are sensitive and responsive in social interactions with the infant, and who remain as consistent caregivers for some time.<ref>Bowlby (1997 edition) pp. 313–17.</ref> Caregiver responses lead to the development of patterns of attachment, that in turn lead to internal working models which will guide the individual's feelings, thoughts, and expectations in later relationships.<ref name=" Bretherton & Munholland 1999">Bretherton I, Munholland KA (1999). "Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships: A Construct Revisited". In Cassidy J and Shaver PR (eds.) ''Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications''. pp. 89–111. Guilford Press {{ISBN|1-57230-087-6}}.</ref><ref>Bowlby (1997 edition) p. 354.</ref> For a diagnosis of reactive attachment disorder, the child's history and atypical social behavior must suggest the absence of formation of a discriminatory or selective attachment. | |||
The pathological absence of a discriminatory or selective attachment needs to be differentiated from the existence of attachments with either typical or somewhat atypical behavior patterns, known as ''styles'' or ''patterns''. There are four attachment ] ascertained and used within developmental attachment research. These are known as ''secure'', ''anxious-ambivalent'', ''anxious-avoidant'', (all ''organized'')<ref name="Ainsworth et al. (1979)">Ainsworth MD, Blehar M, Waters E, Wall S (1979). ''Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation''. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. {{ISBN|0-89859-461-8}}</ref> and ''disorganized''.<ref name="Main & Solomon (1986)">Main M, Solomon J (1986). "Discovery of an insecure disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern: procedures, findings and implications for the classification of behavior". In Brazelton TB and Yogman M (Eds.) ''Affective development in infancy'', pp. 95–124. Norwood, NJ: Ablex {{ISBN|0-89391-345-6}}</ref><ref name= "Main & Solomon 1990">Main M, Solomon J (1990). "Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation". In Greenberg M, Cicchetti D and Cummings E (Eds.) ''Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention'', pp. 121–60. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. {{ISBN|0-226-30630-5}}.</ref> The latter three are characterised as ''insecure''. These are assessed using the ], designed to assess the quality of attachments rather than whether an attachment exists at all.<ref name=AACAP-2005/> | |||
A securely attached toddler will explore freely while the caregiver is present, engage with strangers, be visibly upset when the caregiver departs, and happy to see the caregiver return. The anxious-ambivalent toddler is anxious of exploration, extremely distressed when the caregiver departs but ambivalent when the caregiver returns. The anxious-avoidant toddler will not explore much, avoid or ignore the parent—showing little emotion when the parent departs or returns—and treat strangers much the same as caregivers with little emotional range shown. The disorganized/disoriented toddler shows a lack of a coherent style or pattern for coping. Evidence suggests this occurs when the caregiving figure is also an object of fear, thus putting the child in an irresolvable situation regarding approach and avoidance. On reunion with the caregiver, these children can look dazed or frightened, freezing in place, backing toward the caregiver or approaching with head sharply averted, or showing other behaviors implying fear of the person who is being sought. It is thought to represent a breakdown of an inchoate attachment strategy and it appears to affect the capacity to regulate emotions.<ref name="Van Ijzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg (2003)"/> | |||
Although there are a wide range of attachment difficulties within the styles which may result in emotional disturbance and increase the risk of later psychopathologies, particularly the disorganized style, none of the styles constitute a disorder in themselves and none equate to criteria for RAD as such.<ref name="Thompson (2000)">{{cite journal |author=Thompson RA |title=The legacy of early attachments |journal=Child Dev |volume=71 |issue=1 |pages=145–52 |year=2000 |pmid=10836568 |doi=10.1111/1467-8624.00128 }} | |||
</ref> A ] in the ] sense is a condition requiring treatment, as opposed to risk factors for subsequent disorders.<ref name=AACAP-2005/> Reactive attachment disorder denotes a lack of typical attachment behaviors rather than an attachment style, however problematic that style may be, in that there is an unusual lack of discrimination between familiar and unfamiliar people in both forms of the disorder. Such discrimination does exist as a feature of the social behavior of children with atypical attachment styles. Both DSM-IV and ICD-10 depict the disorder in terms of socially aberrant behavior in general rather than focusing more specifically on attachment behaviors as such. DSM-IV emphasizes a failure to initiate or respond to social interactions across a range of relationships and ICD-10 similarly focuses on contradictory or ambivalent social responses that extend across social situations.<ref name="Zean96"/> The relationship between patterns of attachment in the Strange Situation and RAD is not yet clear.<ref name="O'Con '02">O'Connor TG (2002), "Attachment disorders in infancy and childhood". In Rutter M, Taylor E, (Eds.) ''Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: Modern Approaches'' (4th ed.) Blackwell Scientific publications. pp. 776–792. {{ISBN|0-632-01229-3}}</ref> | |||
There is a lack of consensus about the precise meaning of the term "attachment disorder".<ref>Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 77. The APSAC Taskforce Report</ref> The term is frequently used both as an alternative to reactive attachment disorder and in discussions about different proposed classifications for disorders of attachment beyond the limitations of the ICD and DSM classifications.<ref name="Zean96"/> It is also used within the field of attachment therapy, as is the term reactive attachment disorder, to describe a range of problematic behaviors not within the ICD or DSM criteria or not related directly to attachment styles or difficulties at all.<ref>Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 82–83. The APSAC Taskforce Report</ref> | |||
==Research== | |||
Research from the late 1990s indicated there were disorders of attachment not captured by DSM or ICD and showed that RAD could be diagnosed reliably without evidence of pathogenic care, thus illustrating some of the conceptual difficulties with the rigid structure of the current definition of RAD.<ref name="Boris et al. 1996">{{cite journal |vauthors=Boris NW, Zeanah CH, Larrieu JA, Scheeringa MS, Heller SS |title=Attachment disorders in infancy and early childhood: a preliminary investigation of diagnostic criteria|journal=Am J Psychiatry |volume=155 |issue=2 |pages=295–97 |date=1 February 1998|pmid=9464217|doi=10.1176/ajp.155.2.295|s2cid=23232613 |url=http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=9464217 |access-date=31 January 2008 }}</ref> Research published in 2004 showed that the disinhibited form can endure alongside structured attachment behavior (of any style) towards the child's permanent caregivers.<ref name="Zeanah et al. (2004)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Zeanah CH, Scheeringa M, Boris N, Heller S, Smyke A, Trapani J |date=August 2004 |title=Reactive Attachment Disorder in Maltreated Toddlers |journal=Child Abuse & Neglect |volume=28 |issue=8 |doi=10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.010 |pages=877–88 |pmid=15350771 }}</ref> | |||
Some authors have proposed a broader continuum of definitions of attachment disorders ranging from RAD through various attachment difficulties to the more problematic attachment styles. There is as yet no consensus, on this issue but a new set of practice parameters containing three categories of attachment disorder has been proposed by ] and N. Boris. The first of these is ''disorder of attachment,'' in which a young child has no preferred adult caregiver. The proposed category of disordered attachment is parallel to RAD in its inhibited and disinhibited forms, as defined in DSM and ICD. The second category is ''secure base distortion'', where the child has a preferred familiar caregiver, but the relationship is such that the child cannot use the adult for safety while gradually exploring the environment. Such children may endanger themselves, cling to the adult, be excessively compliant, or show role reversals in which they care for or punish the adult. The third type is ''disrupted attachment''. Disrupted attachment is not covered under ICD-10 and DSM criteria, and results from an abrupt separation or loss of a familiar caregiver to whom attachment has developed.<ref name="Boris & Zeanah (1999)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Boris NW, Zeanah CH |year=1999 |title= Disturbance and disorders of attachment in infancy: An overview|journal=Infant Mental Health Journal |volume=20|pages=1–9 |doi= 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199921)20:1<1::AID-IMHJ1>3.0.CO;2-V}}</ref> This form of categorisation may demonstrate more clinical accuracy overall than the current DSM-IV-TR classification, but further research is required.<ref name="oconzean2003" /><ref name="Zeanah 2000">Zeanah CH (2000). "Disturbances and disorders of attachment in early childhood". In Zeanah CH (Ed.) ''Handbook of infant mental health'' (2nd ed.) pp. 358–62. New York: Guilford Press. {{ISBN|1-59385-171-5}}</ref> The practice parameters would also provide the framework for a diagnostic protocol. Most recently, ] and Erica Willheim have shown a relationship between some maternal violence-related ] and secure base distortion (see above) which is characterized by child recklessness, separation anxiety, hypervigilance, and role-reversal.<ref>Schechter DS, Willheim E (2009). Disturbances of attachment and parental psychopathology in early childhood. Infant and Early Childhood Mental Health Issue. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North America, 18(3), 665–687.</ref> | |||
Some research indicates there may be a significant overlap between behaviors of the inhibited form of RAD or DAD and aspects of disorganized attachment where there is an identified attachment figure.<ref name="Van Ijzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg (2003)">{{cite journal |vauthors=Van Ijzendoorn M, Bakermans-Kranenburg M |date=September 2003 |title=Attachment disorders and disorganized attachment: Similar and different |journal=Attachment & Human Development |volume=5 |issue=3 |pages=313–20(8) |doi= 10.1080/14616730310001593938 |pmid=12944229|s2cid=10644822 }}</ref> | |||
An ongoing question is whether RAD should be thought of as a disorder of the child's personality or a distortion of the relationship between the child and a specific other person. It has been noted that as attachment disorders are by their very nature relational disorders, they do not fit comfortably into nosologies that characterize the disorder as centered on the person.<ref name= "Greenberg 1999">Greenberg MT (1999). ''Attachment and Psychopathology in Childhood''. In Cassidy J and Shaver PR (Eds.) ''Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications''. pp. 469–96. Guilford Press {{ISBN|1-57230-087-6}}</ref> Work by C.H. Zeanah<ref name="Zeanah et al. (2004)"/> indicates that atypical attachment-related behaviors may occur with one caregiver but not with another. This is similar to the situation reported for attachment styles, in which a particular parent's frightened expression has been considered as possibly responsible for disorganized/disoriented reunion behavior during the Strange Situation Procedure.<ref name= "Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990)">Main M, Hesse E (1990) "Parents' unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infants' insecure-disorganized/disoriented attachment status: Is frightened or frightening behavior the linking mechanism?" In Greenberg M, Cicchetti D and Cummings E (Eds.) ''Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention'', pp. 161–182 Chicago: University of Chicago Press. {{ISBN|0-226-30630-5}}.</ref> | |||
The draft of the proposed DSM-V suggests dividing RAD into two disorders, Reactive Attachment Disorder for the current inhibited form of RAD, and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder for what is currently the disinhibited form of RAD, with some alterations in the proposed DSM definition.<ref>DSM-V Proposed Draft. . American Psychiatric Association. Charles H. Zeanah.</ref> | |||
==See also== | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
==Notes== | |||
{{Reflist|30em}} | |||
==References== | |||
{{refbegin}} | |||
* American Psychiatric Association (1994). ''DSM-IV-TR: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edition. Text Revision''. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association. {{ISBN|0-89042-025-4}}. | |||
* ] (1997). ''Attachment and Loss: Attachment Vol. 1 (Attachment and Loss)''. Pimlico; New Ed. {{ISBN|0-7126-7471-3}}. | |||
* Bowlby J (1973). ''Attachment and Loss: Separation—Anxiety and Anger v. 2'' (International Psycho-Analysis Library). London: Hogarth Press. {{ISBN|0-7012-0301-3}}. | |||
* Bowlby J (1980). ''Attachment and Loss: Loss—Sadness and Depression v. 3'' (International Psycho-Analysis Library). London: Hogarth Press. {{ISBN|0-7012-0350-1}}. | |||
* {{cite journal |vauthors=Chaffin M, Hanson R, Saunders BE, etal |title=Report of the APSAC task force on attachment therapy, reactive attachment disorder, and attachment problems|journal=Child Maltreat |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=76–89 |year=2006 |pmid=16382093 |doi=10.1177/1077559505283699|s2cid=11443880}} | |||
* Mercer J (2006). ''Understanding Attachment: Parenting, child care and emotional development''. Westport, CT: Praeger. {{ISBN|0-275-98217-3}}. | |||
* Prior V, Glaser D (2006). ''Understanding Attachment and Attachment Disorders: Theory, Evidence and Practice''. Child and Adolescent Mental Health series, RCPRTU, Jessica Kingsley Publishers. {{ISBN|978-1-84310-245-8}} (pbk). | |||
{{refend}} | |||
==Further reading== | |||
* {{cite journal |last1=Zeanah |first1=Charles H. |last2=Chesher |first2=Tessa |last3=Boris |first3=Neil W. |author4=AACAP Committee on Quality Issues |date=November 2016 |title=Practice Parameter for the Assessment and Treatment of Children and Adolescents With Reactive Attachment Disorder and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder |journal=Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry |volume=55 |issue=11 |pages=990–1003 |doi=10.1016/j.jaac.2016.08.004 |doi-access=free |pmid=27806867 |url=http://www.jaacap.com/article/S0890-8567(16)31183-2/fulltext }} | |||
{{Medical resources | |||
| ICD10 = {{ICD10|F|94|1|f|90}}, {{ICD10|F|94|2|f|90}} | |||
| ICD9 = 313.89 | |||
| ICDO = | |||
| OMIM = | |||
| MedlinePlus = | |||
| eMedicineSubj = ped | |||
| eMedicineTopic = 2646 | |||
| MeshID = D019962 | |||
}} | |||
{{Emotional and behavioral disorders}} | |||
{{Attachment theory}} | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] |
Latest revision as of 01:45, 13 December 2024
Psychological disorder that can affect childrenThis article needs to be updated. The reason given is: DSM-5 was published in 2013, DSM-5-TR published in 2022. This article still speaks of DSM-5 as a proposal. Discussion of DSM 3&4 outside of history section should be removed and replaced.. Please help update this article to reflect recent events or newly available information. (September 2023) |
Medical condition
Reactive attachment disorder | |
---|---|
Specialty | Psychiatry, pediatrics |
Reactive attachment disorder (RAD) is described in clinical literature as a severe disorder that can affect children, although these issues do occasionally persist into adulthood. RAD is characterized by markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate ways of relating socially in most contexts. It can take the form of a persistent failure to initiate or respond to most social interactions in a developmentally appropriate way—known as the "inhibited form". In the DSM-5, the "disinhibited form" is considered a separate diagnosis named "disinhibited attachment disorder".
RAD arises from a failure to form normal attachments to primary caregivers in early childhood. Such a failure could result from severe early experiences of neglect, abuse, abrupt separation from caregivers between the ages of six months and three years, frequent changes of caregivers, or a lack of caregiver responsiveness to a child's communicative efforts. It is differentiated from pervasive developmental disorder or developmental delay and from possibly comorbid conditions such as intellectual disability, all of which can affect attachment behavior. The criteria for a diagnosis of a reactive attachment disorder are very different from the criteria used in assessment or categorization of attachment styles such as insecure or disorganized attachment.
Children with RAD are presumed to have grossly disturbed internal working models of relationships that may lead to interpersonal and behavioral difficulties in later life. There are few studies of long-term effects, and there is a lack of clarity about the presentation of the disorder beyond the age of five years. However, the opening of orphanages in Eastern Europe following the end of the Cold War in the early 1990s provided opportunities for research on infants and toddlers brought up in very deprived conditions. Such research broadened the understanding of the prevalence, causes, mechanism and assessment of disorders of attachment and led to efforts from the late 1990s onwards to develop treatment and prevention programs and better methods of assessment. Mainstream theorists in the field have proposed that a broader range of conditions arising from problems with attachment should be defined beyond current classifications.
Mainstream treatment and prevention programs that target RAD and other problematic early attachment behaviors are based on attachment theory and concentrate on increasing the responsiveness and sensitivity of the caregiver, or if that is not possible, placing the child with a different caregiver. Most such strategies are in the process of being evaluated. Mainstream practitioners and theorists have presented significant criticism of the diagnosis and treatment of alleged reactive attachment disorder or the theoretically baseless "attachment disorder" within the controversial form of psychotherapy commonly known as attachment therapy. Attachment therapy has a scientifically unsupported theoretical base and uses diagnostic criteria or symptom lists markedly different from criteria under ICD-10 or DSM-IV-TR, or to attachment behaviors. A range of treatment approaches are used in attachment therapy, some of which are physically and psychologically coercive, and considered to be antithetical to attachment theory.
Signs and symptoms
See also: Attachment theory and attachment disorderPediatricians are often the first health professionals to assess and raise suspicions of RAD in children with the disorder. The initial presentation varies according to the child's developmental and chronological age, although it always involves a disturbance in social interaction. Infants up to about 18–24 months may present with non-organic failure to thrive and display abnormal responsiveness to stimuli. Laboratory investigations will be unremarkable barring possible findings consistent with malnutrition or dehydration, while serum growth hormone levels will be normal or elevated.
The core feature is severely inappropriate social relating by affected children. This can manifest itself in two ways:
- Indiscriminate and excessive attempts to receive comfort and affection from any available adult, even relative strangers (older children and adolescents may also aim attempts at peers). This may often appear as denial of comfort from anyone as well.
- Extreme reluctance to initiate or accept comfort and affection, even from familiar adults, especially when distressed.
While RAD occurs in relation to neglectful and abusive treatment, automatic diagnoses on this basis alone cannot be made, as children can form stable attachments and social relationships despite marked abuse and neglect. However, the instances of that ability are rare.
The name of the disorder emphasizes problems with attachment but the criteria include symptoms such as failure to thrive, a lack of developmentally appropriate social responsiveness, apathy, and onset before 8 months.
Assessment tools
There is as yet no universally accepted diagnostic protocol for reactive attachment disorder. Often a range of measures are used in research and diagnosis. Recognized assessment methods of attachment styles, difficulties or disorders include the Strange Situation Procedure (devised by developmental psychologist Mary Ainsworth), the separation and reunion procedure and the Preschool Assessment of Attachment, the Observational Record of the Caregiving Environment, the Attachment Q-sort and a variety of narrative techniques using stem stories, puppets or pictures. For older children, actual interviews such as the Child Attachment Interview and the Autobiographical Emotional Events Dialogue can be used. Caregivers may also be assessed using procedures such as the Working Model of the Child Interview.
More recent research also uses the Disturbances of Attachment Interview (DAI) developed by Smyke and Zeanah (1999). The DAI is a semi-structured interview designed to be administered by clinicians to caregivers. It covers 12 items, namely "having a discriminated, preferred adult", "seeking comfort when distressed", "responding to comfort when offered", "social and emotional reciprocity", "emotional regulation", "checking back after venturing away from the care giver", "reticence with unfamiliar adults", "willingness to go off with relative strangers", "self-endangering behavior", "excessive clinging", "vigilance/hypercompliance" and "role reversal". This method is designed to pick up not only RAD but also the proposed new alternative categories of disorders of attachment.
Causes
Although increasing numbers of childhood mental health problems are being attributed to genetic defects, reactive attachment disorder is by definition based on a problematic history of care and social relationships. Abuse can occur alongside the required factors, but on its own does not explain attachment disorder. It has been suggested that types of temperament, or constitutional response to the environment, may make some individuals susceptible to the stress of unpredictable or hostile relationships with caregivers in the early years. In the absence of available and responsive caregivers it appears that most children are particularly vulnerable to developing attachment disorders.
While similar abnormal parenting may produce the two distinct forms of the disorder, inhibited and disinhibited, studies show that abuse and neglect were far more prominent and severe in the cases of RAD, disinhibited type. The issue of temperament and its influence on the development of attachment disorders has yet to be resolved. RAD has never been reported in the absence of serious environmental adversity yet outcomes for children raised in the same environment are the same.
In discussing the neurobiological basis for attachment and trauma symptoms in a seven-year twin study, it has been suggested that the roots of various forms of psychopathology, including RAD, borderline personality disorder (BPD), and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), can be found in disturbances in affect regulation. The subsequent development of higher-order self-regulation is jeopardized and the formation of internal models is affected. Consequently, the "templates" in the mind that drive organized behavior in relationships may be impacted. The potential for "re-regulation" (modulation of emotional responses to within the normal range) in the presence of "corrective" experiences (normative caregiving) seems possible.
Diagnosis
See also: Attachment measuresRAD is one of the least researched and most poorly understood disorders in the DSM. There is little systematic epidemiologic information on RAD, its course is not well established and it appears difficult to diagnose accurately. There is a lack of clarity about the presentation of attachment disorders over the age of five years and difficulty in distinguishing between aspects of attachment disorders, disorganized attachment or the consequences of maltreatment.
According to the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (AACAP), children who exhibit signs of reactive attachment disorder need a comprehensive psychiatric assessment and individualized treatment plan. The signs or symptoms of RAD may also be found in other psychiatric disorders and AACAP advises against giving a child this label or diagnosis without a comprehensive evaluation. Their practice parameter states that the assessment of reactive attachment disorder requires evidence directly obtained from serial observations of the child interacting with their primary caregivers and history (as available) of the child's patterns of attachment behavior with these caregivers. It also requires observations of the child's behavior with unfamiliar adults and a comprehensive history of the child's early caregiving environment including, for example, pediatricians, teachers, or caseworkers. In the US, initial evaluations may be conducted by psychologists, psychiatrists, Licensed Marriage and Family Therapists, Licensed Professional Counselors, specialist Licensed Clinical Social Workers or psychiatric nurses.
In the UK, the British Association for Adoption and Fostering (BAAF) advise that only a psychiatrist can diagnose an attachment disorder and that any assessment must include a comprehensive evaluation of the child's individual and family history.
According to the AACAP Practice Parameter (2005) the question of whether attachment disorders can reliably be diagnosed in older children and adults has not been resolved. Attachment behaviors used for the diagnosis of RAD change markedly with development and defining analogous behaviors in older children is difficult. There are no substantially validated measures of attachment in middle childhood or early adolescence. Assessments of RAD past school age may not be possible at all as by this time children have developed along individual lines to such an extent that early attachment experiences are only one factor among many that determine emotion and behavior.
Criteria
ICD-10 describes reactive attachment disorder of childhood, known as RAD, and disinhibited attachment disorder, less well known as DAD. DSM-IV-TR also describes reactive attachment disorder of infancy or early childhood divided into two subtypes, inhibited type and disinhibited type, both known as RAD. The two classifications are similar and both include:
- markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate social relatedness in most contexts (e.g., the child is avoidant or unresponsive to care when offered by caregivers or is indiscriminately affectionate with strangers);
- the disturbance is not accounted for solely by developmental delay and does not meet the criteria for pervasive developmental disorder;
- onset before five years of age (there is no age specified before five years of age at which RAD cannot be diagnosed);
- a history of significant neglect;
- an implicit lack of identifiable, preferred attachment figure.
ICD-10 states in relation to the inhibited form only that the syndrome probably occurs as a direct result of severe parental neglect, abuse, or serious mishandling. DSM states in relation to both forms there must be a history of "pathogenic care" defined as persistent disregard of the child's basic emotional or physical needs or repeated changes in primary caregiver that prevents the formation of a discriminatory or selective attachment that is presumed to account for the disorder. For this reason, part of the diagnosis is the child's history of care rather than observation of symptoms.
In DSM-IV-TR the inhibited form is described as persistent failure to initiate or respond in a developmentally appropriate fashion to most social interactions, as manifest by excessively inhibited, hypervigilant, or highly ambivalent and contradictory responses (e.g., the child may respond to caregivers with a mixture of approach, avoidance, and resistance to comforting or may exhibit "frozen watchfulness", hypervigilance while keeping an impassive and still demeanour). Such infants do not seek or accept comfort at times of threat, alarm or distress, thus failing to maintain "proximity", an essential element of attachment behavior. The disinhibited form shows diffuse attachments as manifest by indiscriminate sociability with marked inability to exhibit appropriate selective attachments (e.g., excessive familiarity with relative strangers or lack of selectivity in choice of attachment figures). There is therefore a lack of "specificity" of attachment figure, the second basic element of attachment behavior.
The ICD-10 descriptions are comparable save that ICD-10 includes in its description several elements not included in DSM-IV-TR as follows:
- abuse, (psychological or physical), in addition to neglect;
- associated emotional disturbance;
- poor social interaction with peers, aggression towards self and others, misery, and growth failure in some cases (inhibited form only);
- evidence of capacity for social reciprocity and responsiveness as shown by elements of normal social relatedness in interactions with appropriately responsive, non-deviant adults (disinhibited form only).
The first of these is somewhat controversial, being a commission rather than omission and because abuse in and of itself does not lead to attachment disorder.
The inhibited form has a greater tendency to ameliorate with an appropriate caregiver, while the disinhibited form is more enduring. ICD-10 states the disinhibited form "tends to persist despite marked changes in environmental circumstances". Disinhibited and inhibited are not opposites in terms of attachment disorder and can coexist in the same child. The question of whether there are two subtypes has been raised. The World Health Organization acknowledges that there is uncertainty regarding the diagnostic criteria and the appropriate subdivision. One reviewer has commented on the difficulty of clarifying the core characteristics of and differences between atypical attachment styles and ways of categorizing more severe disorders of attachment.
As of 2010, the American Psychiatric Association has proposed to redefine RAD into two distinct disorders in the DSM-V. Corresponding with the inhibited type, one disorder will be reclassified as Reactive Attachment Disorder of Infancy and Early Childhood.
In regards to pathogenic care, or the type of care in which these behaviors are present, a new criterion for Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder now includes chronically harsh punishment or other types of severely inept caregiving. Relating to pathogenic care for both proposed disorders, a new criterion is rearing in atypical environments such as institutions with high child/caregiver ratios that cut down on opportunities to form attachments with a caregiver.
Differential diagnosis
The diagnostic complexities of RAD mean that careful diagnostic evaluation by a trained mental health expert with particular expertise in differential diagnosis is considered essential. Several other disorders, such as conduct disorders, oppositional defiant disorder, anxiety disorders, post traumatic stress disorder and social phobia share many symptoms and are often comorbid with or confused with RAD, leading to over and under diagnosis. RAD can also be confused with neuropsychiatric disorders such as autism, pervasive developmental disorder, childhood schizophrenia and some genetic syndromes. Infants with this disorder can be distinguished from those with organic illness by their rapid physical improvement after hospitalization. Autistic children are likely to be of normal size and weight and often exhibit a degree of intellectual disability. They are unlikely to improve upon being removed from the home.
Alternative diagnosis
In the absence of a standardized diagnosis system, many popular, informal classification systems or checklists, outside the DSM and ICD, were created out of clinical and parental experience within the field known as attachment therapy. These lists are unvalidated and critics state they are inaccurate, too broadly defined or applied by unqualified persons. Many are found on the websites of attachment therapists. Common elements of these lists such as lying, lack of remorse or conscience and cruelty do not form part of the diagnostic criteria under either DSM-IV-TR or ICD-10. Many children are being diagnosed with RAD because of behavioral problems that are outside the criteria. There is an emphasis within attachment therapy on aggressive behavior as a symptom of what they describe as attachment disorder whereas mainstream theorists view these behaviors as comorbid, externalizing behaviors requiring appropriate assessment and treatment rather than attachment disorders. However, knowledge of attachment relationships can contribute to the cause, maintenance and treatment of externalizing disorders.
The Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire or RADQ is one of the better known of these checklists and is used by attachment therapists and others. The checklist includes 93 discrete behaviours, many of which either overlap with other disorders, like conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder, or are not related to attachment difficulties. Critics assert that it is unvalidated and lacks specificity.
Treatment
Main articles: Attachment-based therapy (children) and Attachment therapyAssessing the child's safety is an essential first step that determines whether future intervention can take place in the family unit or whether the child should be removed to a safe situation. Interventions may include psychosocial support services for the family unit (including financial or domestic aid, housing and social work support), psychotherapeutic interventions (including treating parents for mental illness, family therapy, individual therapy), education (including training in basic parenting skills and child development), and monitoring of the child's safety within the family environment
In 2005 the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry laid down guidelines (devised by N.W. Boris and C.H. Zeanah) based on its published parameters for the diagnosis and treatment of RAD. Recommendations in the guidelines include the following:
- "The most important intervention for young children diagnosed with reactive attachment disorder and who lack an attachment to a discriminated caregiver is for the clinician to advocate for providing the child with an emotionally available attachment figure."
- "Although the diagnosis of reactive attachment disorder is based on symptoms displayed by the child, assessing the caregiver's attitudes toward and perceptions about the child is important for treatment selection."
- "Children with reactive attachment disorder are presumed to have grossly disturbed internal models for relating to others. After ensuring that the child is in a safe and stable placement, effective attachment treatment must focus on creating positive interactions with caregivers."
- "Children who meet criteria for reactive attachment disorder and who display aggressive and oppositional behavior require adjunctive (additional) treatments."
Mainstream prevention programs and treatment approaches for attachment difficulties or disorders for infants and younger children are based on attachment theory and concentrate on increasing the responsiveness and sensitivity of the caregiver, or if that is not possible, placing the child with a different caregiver. These approaches are mostly in the process of being evaluated. The programs invariably include a detailed assessment of the attachment status or caregiving responses of the adult caregiver as attachment is a two-way process involving attachment behavior and caregiver response. Some of these treatment or prevention programs are specifically aimed at foster carers rather than parents, as the attachment behaviors of infants or children with attachment difficulties often do not elicit appropriate caregiver responses. Approaches include "Watch, wait and wonder," manipulation of sensitive responsiveness, modified "Interaction Guidance", "Clinician-Assisted Videofeedback Exposure Sessions (CAVES)", "Preschool Parent Psychotherapy", "Circle of Security", "Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up" (ABC), the New Orleans Intervention, and parent–child psychotherapy. Other treatment methods include Developmental, Individual-difference, and Relationship-based therapy (DIR, also referred to as Floor Time) by Stanley Greenspan, although DIR is primarily directed to treatment of pervasive developmental disorders.
The relevance of these approaches to intervention with fostered and adopted children with RAD or older children with significant histories of maltreatment is unclear.
Attachment therapy
Main article: Attachment therapyThe terms attachment disorder, attachment problems, and attachment therapy, although increasingly used, have no clear, specific, or consensus definitions. However, the terms and therapies often are applied to children who are maltreated, particularly those in the foster care, kinship care, or adoption systems, and related populations such as children adopted internationally from orphanages.
Outside the mainstream programs is a form of treatment generally known as attachment therapy, a subset of techniques (and accompanying novel diagnosis) for supposed attachment disorders including RAD. These "attachment disorders" use diagnostic criteria or symptom lists different from criteria under ICD-10 or DSM-IV-TR, or to attachment behaviors. Those with "attachment disorder" are said to lack empathy and remorse.
Treatments of this pseudoscientific disorder are called "Attachment therapy". In general, these therapies are aimed at adopted or fostered children with a view to creating attachment in these children to their new caregivers. The theoretical base is broadly a combination of regression and catharsis, accompanied by parenting methods which emphasize obedience and parental control. There is considerable criticism of this form of treatment and diagnosis as it is largely unvalidated and has developed outside the scientific mainstream. There is little or no evidence base and techniques vary from non-coercive therapeutic work to more extreme forms of physical, confrontational and coercive techniques, of which the best known are holding therapy, rebirthing, rage-reduction and the Evergreen model. These forms of the therapy may well involve physical restraint, the deliberate provocation of rage and anger in the child by physical and verbal means including deep tissue massage, aversive tickling, enforced eye contact and verbal confrontation, and being pushed to revisit earlier trauma. Critics maintain that these therapies are not within the attachment paradigm, are potentially abusive, and are antithetical to attachment theory. The APSAC Taskforce Report of 2006 notes that many of these therapies concentrate on changing the child rather than the caregiver. Children may be described as "RADs", "Radkids" or "Radishes" and dire predictions may be made as to their supposedly violent futures if they are not treated with attachment therapy. The Mayo Clinic, a well known U.S. non-profit medical practice and medical research group, cautions against consulting with mental health providers who promote these types of methods and offer evidence to support their techniques; to date, this evidence base is not published within reputable medical or mental health journals.
Prognosis
The AACAP guidelines state that children with reactive attachment disorder are presumed to have grossly disturbed internal models for relating to others. However, the course of RAD is not well studied and there have been few efforts to examine symptom patterns over time. The few existing longitudinal studies (dealing with developmental change with age over a period of time) involve only children from poorly run Eastern European institutions.
Findings from the studies of children from Eastern European orphanages indicate that persistence of the inhibited pattern of RAD is rare in children adopted out of institutions into normative care-giving environments. However, there is a close association between duration of deprivation and severity of attachment disorder behaviors. The quality of attachments that these children form with subsequent care-givers may be compromised, but they probably no longer meet criteria for inhibited RAD. The same group of studies suggests that a minority of adopted, institutionalized children exhibit persistent indiscriminate sociability even after more normative caregiving environments are provided. Indiscriminate sociability may persist for years, even among children who subsequently exhibit preferred attachment to their new caregivers. Some exhibit hyperactivity and attention problems as well as difficulties in peer relationships. In the only longitudinal study that has followed children with indiscriminate behavior into adolescence, these children were significantly more likely to exhibit poor peer relationships.
Studies of children who were reared in institutions have suggested that they are inattentive and overactive, no matter what quality of care they received. In one investigation, some institution-reared boys were reported to be inattentive, overactive, and markedly unselective in their social relationships, while girls, foster-reared children, and some institution-reared children were not. It is not yet clear whether these behaviors should be considered as part of disordered attachment.
There is one case study on maltreated twins published in 1999 with a follow-up in 2006. This study assessed the twins between the ages of 19 and 36 months, during which time they had multiple moves and placements. The paper explores the similarities, differences and comorbidity of RAD, disorganized attachment and post traumatic stress disorder. The girl showed signs of the inhibited form of RAD while the boy showed signs of the indiscriminate form. It was noted that the diagnosis of RAD ameliorated with better care but symptoms of post traumatic stress disorder and signs of disorganized attachment came and went as the infants progressed through multiple placement changes. At age three, some lasting relationship disturbance was evident.
In the follow-up case study when the twins were aged three and eight years, the lack of longitudinal research on maltreated as opposed to institutionalized children was again highlighted. The girl's symptoms of disorganized attachment had developed into controlling behaviors—a well-documented outcome. The boy still exhibited self-endangering behaviors, not within RAD criteria but possibly within "secure base distortion", (where the child has a preferred familiar caregiver, but the relationship is such that the child cannot use the adult for safety while gradually exploring the environment). At age eight the children were assessed with a variety of measures including those designed to access representational systems, or the child's "internal working models". The twins' symptoms were indicative of different trajectories. The girl showed externalizing symptoms (particularly deceit), contradictory reports of current functioning, chaotic personal narratives, struggles with friendships, and emotional disengagement with her caregiver, resulting in a clinical picture described as "quite concerning". The boy still evidenced self-endangering behaviors as well as avoidance in relationships and emotional expression, separation anxiety and impulsivity and attention difficulties. It was apparent that life stressors had impacted each child differently. The narrative measures used were considered helpful in tracking how early attachment disruption is associated with later expectations about relationships.
One paper using questionnaires found that children aged three to six, diagnosed with RAD, scored lower on empathy but higher on self-monitoring (regulating your behavior to "look good"). These differences were especially pronounced based on ratings by parents, and suggested that children with RAD may systematically report their personality traits in overly positive ways. Their scores also indicated considerably more behavioral problems than scores of the control children.
Epidemiology
Epidemiological data are limited, but reactive attachment disorder appears to be very uncommon. The prevalence of RAD is unclear but it is probably quite rare, other than in populations of children being reared in the most extreme, deprived settings such as some orphanages. There is little systematically gathered epidemiologic information on RAD. A cohort study of 211 Copenhagen children to the age of 18 months found a prevalence of 0.9%.
Attachment disorders tend to occur in a definable set of contexts such as within some types of institutions, in the presence of repeated changes of primary caregiver or of extremely neglectful identifiable primary caregivers who show persistent disregard for the child's basic attachment needs, but not all children raised in these conditions develop an attachment disorder. Studies undertaken on children from Eastern European orphanages from the mid-1990s showed significantly higher levels of both forms of RAD and of insecure patterns of attachment in the institutionalized children, regardless of how long they had been there. It would appear that children in institutions like these are unable to form selective attachments to their caregivers. The difference between the institutionalized children and the control group had lessened in the follow-up study three years later, although the institutionalized children continued to show significantly higher levels of indiscriminate friendliness. However, even among children raised in the most deprived institutional conditions the majority did not show symptoms of this disorder.
A 2002 study of children in residential nurseries in Bucharest, in which the DAI was used, challenged the current DSM and ICD conceptualizations of disordered attachment and showed that inhibited and disinhibited disorders could coexist in the same child.
There are two studies on the incidence of RAD relating to high risk and maltreated children in the U.S. Both used ICD, DSM and the DAI. The first, in 2004, reported that children from the maltreatment sample were significantly more likely to meet criteria for one or more attachment disorders than children from the other groups, however this was mainly the proposed new classification of disrupted attachment disorder rather than the DSM or ICD classified RAD or DAD. The second study, also in 2004, attempted to ascertain the prevalence of RAD and whether it could be reliably identified in maltreated rather than neglected toddlers. Of the 94 maltreated toddlers in foster care, 35% were identified as having ICD RAD and 22% as having ICD DAD, and 38% fulfilled the DSM criteria for RAD. This study found that RAD could be reliably identified and also that the inhibited and disinhibited forms were not independent. However, there are some methodological concerns with this study. A number of the children identified as fulfilling the criteria for RAD did in fact have a preferred attachment figure.
It has been suggested by some within the field of attachment therapy that RAD may be quite prevalent because severe child maltreatment, which is known to increase risk for RAD, is prevalent and because children who are severely abused may exhibit behaviors similar to RAD behaviors. The APSAC Taskforce consider this inference to be flawed and questionable. Severely abused children may exhibit similar behaviors to RAD behaviors but there are several far more common and demonstrably treatable diagnoses which may better account for these difficulties. Further, many children experience severe maltreatment and do not develop clinical disorders. Resilience is a common and normal human characteristic. RAD does not underlie all or even most of the behavioral and emotional problems seen in foster children, adoptive children, or children who are maltreated and rates of child abuse and/or neglect or problem behaviors are not a benchmark for estimates of RAD.
There are few data on comorbid conditions, but there are some conditions that arise in the same circumstances in which RAD arises, such as institutionalization or maltreatment. These are principally developmental delays and language disorders associated with neglect. Conduct disorders, oppositional defiant disorder, anxiety disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder and social phobia share many symptoms and are often comorbid with or confused with RAD. Attachment disorder behaviors amongst institutionalized children are correlated with attentional and conduct problems and cognitive levels but nonetheless appear to index a distinct set of symptoms and behaviors.
History
Main articles: Attachment theory and Attachment in childrenReactive attachment disorder first made its appearance in standard nosologies of psychological disorders in DSM-III, 1980, following an accumulation of evidence on institutionalized children. The criteria included a requirement of onset before the age of 8 months and was equated with failure to thrive. Both these features were dropped in DSM-III-R, 1987. Instead, onset was changed to being within the first 5 years of life and the disorder itself was divided into two subcategories, inhibited and disinhibited. These changes resulted from further research on maltreated and institutionalized children and remain in the current version, DSM-IV, 1994, and its 2000 text revision, DSM-IV-TR, as well as in ICD-10, 1992. Both nosologies focus on young children who are not merely at increased risk for subsequent disorders but are already exhibiting clinical disturbance.
The broad theoretical framework for current versions of RAD is attachment theory, based on work conducted from the 1940s to the 1980s by John Bowlby, Mary Ainsworth and René Spitz. Attachment theory is a framework that employs psychological, ethological and evolutionary concepts to explain social behaviors typical of young children. Attachment theory focuses on the tendency of infants or children to seek proximity to a particular attachment figure (familiar caregiver), in situations of alarm or distress, behavior which appears to have survival value. This is known as a discriminatory or selective attachment. Subsequently, the child begins to use the caregiver as a base of security from which to explore the environment, returning periodically to the familiar person. Attachment is not the same as love and/or affection although they are often associated. Attachment and attachment behaviors tend to develop between the ages of six months and three years. Infants become attached to adults who are sensitive and responsive in social interactions with the infant, and who remain as consistent caregivers for some time. Caregiver responses lead to the development of patterns of attachment, that in turn lead to internal working models which will guide the individual's feelings, thoughts, and expectations in later relationships. For a diagnosis of reactive attachment disorder, the child's history and atypical social behavior must suggest the absence of formation of a discriminatory or selective attachment.
The pathological absence of a discriminatory or selective attachment needs to be differentiated from the existence of attachments with either typical or somewhat atypical behavior patterns, known as styles or patterns. There are four attachment styles ascertained and used within developmental attachment research. These are known as secure, anxious-ambivalent, anxious-avoidant, (all organized) and disorganized. The latter three are characterised as insecure. These are assessed using the Strange Situation Procedure, designed to assess the quality of attachments rather than whether an attachment exists at all.
A securely attached toddler will explore freely while the caregiver is present, engage with strangers, be visibly upset when the caregiver departs, and happy to see the caregiver return. The anxious-ambivalent toddler is anxious of exploration, extremely distressed when the caregiver departs but ambivalent when the caregiver returns. The anxious-avoidant toddler will not explore much, avoid or ignore the parent—showing little emotion when the parent departs or returns—and treat strangers much the same as caregivers with little emotional range shown. The disorganized/disoriented toddler shows a lack of a coherent style or pattern for coping. Evidence suggests this occurs when the caregiving figure is also an object of fear, thus putting the child in an irresolvable situation regarding approach and avoidance. On reunion with the caregiver, these children can look dazed or frightened, freezing in place, backing toward the caregiver or approaching with head sharply averted, or showing other behaviors implying fear of the person who is being sought. It is thought to represent a breakdown of an inchoate attachment strategy and it appears to affect the capacity to regulate emotions.
Although there are a wide range of attachment difficulties within the styles which may result in emotional disturbance and increase the risk of later psychopathologies, particularly the disorganized style, none of the styles constitute a disorder in themselves and none equate to criteria for RAD as such. A disorder in the clinical sense is a condition requiring treatment, as opposed to risk factors for subsequent disorders. Reactive attachment disorder denotes a lack of typical attachment behaviors rather than an attachment style, however problematic that style may be, in that there is an unusual lack of discrimination between familiar and unfamiliar people in both forms of the disorder. Such discrimination does exist as a feature of the social behavior of children with atypical attachment styles. Both DSM-IV and ICD-10 depict the disorder in terms of socially aberrant behavior in general rather than focusing more specifically on attachment behaviors as such. DSM-IV emphasizes a failure to initiate or respond to social interactions across a range of relationships and ICD-10 similarly focuses on contradictory or ambivalent social responses that extend across social situations. The relationship between patterns of attachment in the Strange Situation and RAD is not yet clear.
There is a lack of consensus about the precise meaning of the term "attachment disorder". The term is frequently used both as an alternative to reactive attachment disorder and in discussions about different proposed classifications for disorders of attachment beyond the limitations of the ICD and DSM classifications. It is also used within the field of attachment therapy, as is the term reactive attachment disorder, to describe a range of problematic behaviors not within the ICD or DSM criteria or not related directly to attachment styles or difficulties at all.
Research
Research from the late 1990s indicated there were disorders of attachment not captured by DSM or ICD and showed that RAD could be diagnosed reliably without evidence of pathogenic care, thus illustrating some of the conceptual difficulties with the rigid structure of the current definition of RAD. Research published in 2004 showed that the disinhibited form can endure alongside structured attachment behavior (of any style) towards the child's permanent caregivers.
Some authors have proposed a broader continuum of definitions of attachment disorders ranging from RAD through various attachment difficulties to the more problematic attachment styles. There is as yet no consensus, on this issue but a new set of practice parameters containing three categories of attachment disorder has been proposed by C.H. Zeanah and N. Boris. The first of these is disorder of attachment, in which a young child has no preferred adult caregiver. The proposed category of disordered attachment is parallel to RAD in its inhibited and disinhibited forms, as defined in DSM and ICD. The second category is secure base distortion, where the child has a preferred familiar caregiver, but the relationship is such that the child cannot use the adult for safety while gradually exploring the environment. Such children may endanger themselves, cling to the adult, be excessively compliant, or show role reversals in which they care for or punish the adult. The third type is disrupted attachment. Disrupted attachment is not covered under ICD-10 and DSM criteria, and results from an abrupt separation or loss of a familiar caregiver to whom attachment has developed. This form of categorisation may demonstrate more clinical accuracy overall than the current DSM-IV-TR classification, but further research is required. The practice parameters would also provide the framework for a diagnostic protocol. Most recently, Daniel Schechter and Erica Willheim have shown a relationship between some maternal violence-related posttraumatic stress disorder and secure base distortion (see above) which is characterized by child recklessness, separation anxiety, hypervigilance, and role-reversal.
Some research indicates there may be a significant overlap between behaviors of the inhibited form of RAD or DAD and aspects of disorganized attachment where there is an identified attachment figure.
An ongoing question is whether RAD should be thought of as a disorder of the child's personality or a distortion of the relationship between the child and a specific other person. It has been noted that as attachment disorders are by their very nature relational disorders, they do not fit comfortably into nosologies that characterize the disorder as centered on the person. Work by C.H. Zeanah indicates that atypical attachment-related behaviors may occur with one caregiver but not with another. This is similar to the situation reported for attachment styles, in which a particular parent's frightened expression has been considered as possibly responsible for disorganized/disoriented reunion behavior during the Strange Situation Procedure.
The draft of the proposed DSM-V suggests dividing RAD into two disorders, Reactive Attachment Disorder for the current inhibited form of RAD, and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder for what is currently the disinhibited form of RAD, with some alterations in the proposed DSM definition.
See also
Notes
- ^ DSM-IV-TR (2000) American Psychiatric Association p. 129.
- Schechter DS, Willheim E (July 2009). "Disturbances of attachment and parental psychopathology in early childhood". Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 18 (3): 665–86. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2009.03.001. PMC 2690512. PMID 19486844.
- Marinus van IJzendoorn MH; Femmie Juffer (May 2005). "Behavior Problems and Mental Health Referrals of International Adoptees". The Journal of the American Medical Association. 293 (20): 2501–2515. doi:10.1001/jama.293.20.2501. PMID 15914751. S2CID 25576414.
- ^ Boris, Neil W.; Zeanah, Charles H.; Work Group on Quality Issues (November 2005). "Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder of infancy and early childhood". Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 44 (11): 1206–19. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000177056.41655.ce. PMID 16239871.
- ^ Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 228.
- ^ O'Connor TG, Zeanah CH (2003). "Attachment disorders: assessment strategies and treatment approaches". Attach Hum Dev. 5 (3): 223–44. doi:10.1080/14616730310001593974. PMID 12944216. S2CID 21547653.
- Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 231.
- ^ O'Connor TG, Nilsen WJ (2005). "Models versus Metaphors in Translating Attachment Theory to the Clinic and Community". In Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L and Greenberg MT (eds.). Enhancing Early Attachments: Theory, Research, Intervention, and Policy. pp. 313–26. The Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. ISBN 1-59385-470-6.
- ^ Sadock, BJ; Sadock VA (2004). Kaplan & Sadock's Concise Textbook of Clinical Psychiatry. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. pp. 570–72. ISBN 978-0-7817-5033-2.
- ^ Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 80. The APSAC Taskforce Report
- Rutter M (2002). "Nature, nurture, and development: from evangelism through science toward policy and practice". Child Dev. 73 (1): 1–21. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00388. PMID 14717240.
- RICHTERS, MARGOT MOSER; VOLKMAR, FRED R. (1 March 1994). "Reactive Attachment Disorder of Infancy or Early Childhood". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 33 (3): 328–332. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.527.9988. doi:10.1097/00004583-199403000-00005. PMID 7513324. S2CID 13058511.
- ^ Ainsworth MD, Blehar M, Waters E, Wall S (1979). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-89859-461-8
- ^ Main M, Solomon J (1986). "Discovery of an insecure disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern: procedures, findings and implications for the classification of behavior". In Brazelton TB and Yogman M (Eds.) Affective development in infancy, pp. 95–124. Norwood, NJ: Ablex ISBN 0-89391-345-6
- ^ Main M, Solomon J (1990). "Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation". In Greenberg M, Cicchetti D and Cummings E (Eds.) Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention, pp. 121–60. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0-226-30630-5.
- Crittenden PM (1992). "Quality of attachment in the preschool years". Development and Psychopathology. 4 (2): 209–41. doi:10.1017/S0954579400000110. S2CID 143894461.
- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, D (1996). "Characteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing to positive caregiving". Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 11 (3): 269–306(38). doi:10.1016/S0885-2006(96)90009-5.
- Waters E, Deane K (1985). "Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood". In Bretherton I and Waters E (Eds.) Growing pains of attachment theory and research: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 50, Serial No. 209 (1–2), pp. 41–65.
- Zeanah CH, Benoit D (1995). "Clinical applications of a parent perception interview in infant mental health". Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 43 (3): 539–554. doi:10.1016/S1056-4993(18)30418-8.
- Smyke A, Zeanah CH (1999). "Disturbances of Attachment Interview". Available on the Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry website at www.jaacap.com via Article plus. Retrieved on 3 March 2008.
- Mercer (2006), pp. 104–05.
- Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 218.
- Marshall PJ, Fox NA (2005). "Relationship between behavioral reactivity at 4 months and attachment classification at 14 months in a selected sample". Infant Behavior and Development. 28 (4): 492–502. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.06.002.
- ^ Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 219.
- Zeanah CH, Fox NA (2004). "Temperament and attachment disorders". J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 33 (1): 32–41. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_4. PMID 15028539. S2CID 9416146.
- ^ Heller SS, Boris NW, Fuselier SH, Page T, Koren-Karie N, Miron D (2006). "Reactive attachment disorder in maltreated twins follow-up: from 18 months to 8 years". Attach Hum Dev. 8 (1): 63–86. doi:10.1080/14616730600585177. PMID 16581624. S2CID 34947321.
- Reactive Attachment Disorder. Archived 3 February 2008 at the Wayback Machine American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Facts for Families, No. 85; Updated December 2002. Retrieved on 13 February 2008.
- For examples see Reactive Attachment Disorder Archived 28 December 2007 at the Wayback Machine, DCFS, State of Illinois and DBHS Practice Protocol: Disturbances and Disorders of Attachment (PDF), Arizona Department of Health Services, 2 October 2006. Retrieved on 23 February 2008.
- Attachment Disorders, their Assessment and Intervention/Treatment Archived 2 October 2008 at the Wayback Machine (PDF). British Association for Adoption and Fostering, Position Statement 4, 2006. Retrieved on 23 February 2008
- Mercer (2006), p. 116.
- ^ Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Text Revision. American Psychiatric Association. 2000. pp. 943. ISBN 978-0-89042-025-6.
- ^ American Psychiatric Association (2000). "Diagnostic criteria for 313.89 Reactive attachment disorder of infancy or early childhood". Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text revision (DSM-IV-TR) ed.). United States: AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC PRESS INC (DC). ISBN 978-0-89042-025-6.
- Prior & Glaser (2006), pp. 220–21.
- ^ Zeanah CH, Scheeringa M, Boris N, Heller S, Smyke A, Trapani J (August 2004). "Reactive Attachment Disorder in Maltreated Toddlers". Child Abuse & Neglect. 28 (8): 877–88. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.010. PMID 15350771.
- World Health Organisation (1992) International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision (ICD-10). Geneva: World health Organization.
- Zilberstein K (2006). "Clarifying core characteristics of attachment disorders". American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 76 (1): 55–64. doi:10.1037/0002-9432.76.1.55. PMID 16569127. S2CID 25416390.
- ^ Proposed Revision Reactive Attachment Disorder, American Psychiatric Association (2012). Retrieved from http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevisions/Pages/proposedrevision.aspx?rid=120
- ^ Hanson RF, Spratt EG (2000). "Reactive Attachment Disorder: what we know about the disorder and implications for treatment". Child Maltreat. 5 (2): 137–45. doi:10.1177/1077559500005002005. PMID 11232086. S2CID 21497329.
- ^ Wilson SL (2001). "Attachment disorders: review and current status". J Psychol. 135 (1): 37–51. doi:10.1080/00223980109603678. PMID 11235838. S2CID 7226465.
- ^ Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 81. The APSAC Taskforce Report
- Chaffin et al. (2006), pp. 82–83. The APSAC Taskforce Report
- Guttmann-Steinmetz S, Crowell JA (2006). "Attachment and externalizing disorders: a developmental psychopathology perspective". J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 45 (4): 440–51. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000196422.42599.63. PMID 16601649.
- Randolph, Elizabeth Marie. (1996). Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire. Institute for Attachment, Evergreen CO.
- Mercer J (2005). "Coercive restraint therapies: a dangerous alternative mental health intervention". MedGenMed. 7 (3): 6. PMC 1681667. PMID 16369232.
- Cappelletty G, Brown M, Shumate S (2005). "Correlates of the Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire (RADQ) in a Sample of Children in Foster Placement". Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal. 22 (1): 71–84. doi:10.1007/s10560-005-2556-2. S2CID 143743052.
The findings showed that children in foster care have reported symptoms within the range typical of children not involved in foster care. The conclusion is that the RADQ has limited usefulness due to its lack of specificity with implications for treatment of children in foster care
- Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 231.
- Bakermans-Kranenburg M, van IJzendoorn M, Juffer F (2003). "Less Is More: Meta-Analyses of Sensitivity and Attachment Interventions in Early Childhood" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 129 (2): 195–215. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195. PMID 12696839. S2CID 7504386. Retrieved 2 February 2008.
- Stovall KC, Dozier M (2000). "The development of attachment in new relationships: single subject analyses for 10 foster infants". Dev. Psychopathol. 12 (2): 133–56. doi:10.1017/S0954579400002029. PMID 10847621. S2CID 746807.
- Cohen N, Muir E, Lojkasek M, Muir R, Parker C, Barwick M, Brown M (1999). "Watch, wait and wonder: testing the effectiveness of a new approach to mother-infant psychotherapy". Infant Mental Health Journal. 20 (4): 429–51. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199924)20:4<429::AID-IMHJ5>3.0.CO;2-Q.
- van den Boom D (1994). "The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: an experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants". Child Development. 65 (5): 1457–77. doi:10.2307/1131277. JSTOR 1131277. PMID 7982362.
- van den Boom D (1995). "Do first-year intervention effects endure? Follow-up during toddlerhood of a sample of Dutch irritable infants". Child Dev. 66 (6): 1798–816. doi:10.2307/1131911. JSTOR 1131911. PMID 8556900.
- Benoit D, Madigan S, Lecce S, Shea B, Goldberg S (2002). "Atypical maternal behaviour toward feeding disordered infants before and after intervention". Infant Mental Health Journal. 22 (6): 611–26. doi:10.1002/imhj.1022.
- Schechter DS, Myers MM, Brunelli SA, et al. (September 2006). "Traumatized mothers can change their minds about their toddlers: Understanding how a novel use of videofeedback supports positive change of maternal attributions". Infant Mental Health Journal. 27 (5): 429–447. doi:10.1002/imhj.20101. PMC 2078524. PMID 18007960.
- Toth, S; Maughan A; Manly J; Spagnola M; Cicchetti D (2002). "The relative efficacy of two in altering maltreated preschool children's representational models: implications for attachment theory". Development and Psychopathology. 14 (4): 877–908. doi:10.1017/S095457940200411X. PMID 12549708. S2CID 30792141.
- Marvin R, Cooper G, Hoffman K, Powell B (April 2002). "The Circle of Security project: Attachment-based intervention with caregiver – pre-school child dyads" (PDF). Attachment & Human Development. 4 (1): 107–24. doi:10.1080/14616730252982491. PMID 12065033. S2CID 25815919. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 February 2008. Retrieved 2 February 2008.
- Cooper G, Hoffman K, Powell B and Marvin R (2005). "The Circle of Security Intervention; differential diagnosis and differential treatment". In Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L and Greenberg MT (eds.) Enhancing Early Attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy. pp. 127–51. The Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. (2005) ISBN 1-59385-470-6.
- Dozier M, Lindheim O and Ackerman JP (2005) "Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-Up: An intervention targeting empirically identified needs of foster infants". In Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L and Greenberg MT (Eds.) Enhancing Early Attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy pp. 178–94. Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. (2005) ISBN 1-59385-470-6 (pbk)
- Zeanah CH, Larrieu JA (1998). "Intensive intervention for maltreated infants and toddlers in foster care". Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 7 (2): 357–71. doi:10.1016/S1056-4993(18)30246-3. PMID 9894069.
- Larrieu JA, Zeanah CH (2004). "Treating infant-parent relationships in the context of maltreatment: An integrated, systems approach". In Saner A, McDonagh S and Roesenblaum K (Eds.) Treating parent-infant relationship problems pp. 243–64. New York. Guilford Press. ISBN 1-59385-245-2
- Zeanah CH, Smyke AT (2005) "Building Attachment Relationships Following Maltreatment and Severe Deprivation". In Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L and Greenberg MT (Eds) Enhancing Early Attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy The Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. (2005) pp. 195–216. ISBN 1-59385-470-6 (pbk)
- Lieberman AF, Silverman R, Pawl JH (2000). "Infant-parent psychotherapy". In Zeanah CH (Ed.) Handbook of infant mental health (2nd ed.) p. 432. New York: Guilford Press. ISBN 1-59385-171-5
- "Dir/floortime model". Interdisciplinary Council on Developmental & Learning Disorders. 2007. Archived from the original on 25 February 2008. Retrieved 2 February 2008.
- Newman L, Mares S (2007). "Recent advances in the theories of and interventions with attachment disorders". Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 20 (4): 343–8. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3281bc0d08. PMID 17551348. S2CID 34000485.
- Chaffin, Mark; Hanson, Rochelle; Saunders, Benjamin E.; Nichols, Todd; Barnett, Douglas; Zeanah, Charles; Berliner, Lucy; Egeland, Byron; Newman, Elana (1 February 2006). "Report of the APSAC Task Force on Attachment Therapy, Reactive Attachment Disorder, and Attachment Problems". Child Maltreatment. 11 (1): 76–89. doi:10.1177/1077559505283699. ISSN 1077-5595. PMID 16382093. S2CID 11443880.
- ^ Chaffin et al. (2006), pp. 79–80. The APSAC Taskforce Report.
- Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 85. The APSAC Taskforce Report
- Chaffin et al. (2006), pp. 78–83. The APSAC Taskforce Report.
- Speltz ML (2002). "Description, History and Critique of Corrective Attachment Therapy" (PDF). The APSAC Advisor. 14 (3): 4–8. Archived from the original (PDF) on 14 April 2008. Retrieved 3 March 2008.
- Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 267.
- Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 79. The APSAC Taskforce Report.
- "Treatments and drugs". Mayo Clinic Staff.
- ^ O'Connor TG, Rutter M (2000). "Attachment disorder behavior following early severe deprivation: extension and longitudinal follow-up. English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team". J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 39 (6): 703–12. doi:10.1097/00004583-200006000-00008. PMID 10846304.
- O'Connor TG, Marvin RS, Rutter M, Olrick JT, Britner PA (2003). "Child-parent attachment following early institutional deprivation". Dev. Psychopathol. 15 (1): 19–38. doi:10.1017/S0954579403000026. PMID 12848433. S2CID 33936291.
- O'Connor TG, Bredenkamp D, Rutter M, & The English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team (1999). "Attachment disturbances and disorders in children exposed to early severe deprivation". Infant Mental Health Journal. 20: 10–29. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199921)20:1<10::AID-IMHJ2>3.0.CO;2-S.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Hodges J, Tizard B (1989). "Social and family relationships of ex-institutional adolescents". J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 30 (1): 77–97. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00770.x. PMID 2925822.
- Roy P, Rutter M, Pickles A (2004). "Institutional care: Associations between overactivity and lack of selectivity in social relationships". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 45 (4): 866–73. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00278.x. PMID 15056316.
- ^ Hinshaw-Fuselier, Sarah; Boris, Neil W.; Zeanah, Charles H. (1999). "Reactive attachment disorder in maltreated twins". Infant Mental Health Journal. 20 (1): 42–59. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199921)20:1<42::AID-IMHJ4>3.0.CO;2-B.
- Hall SE, Geher G (2003). "Behavioral and personality characteristics of children with reactive attachment disorder". J Psychol. 137 (2): 145–62. doi:10.1080/00223980309600605. PMID 12735525. S2CID 32015193.
- Skovgaard AM, Houmann T, Christiansen E, et al. (2007). "The prevalence of mental health problems in children 1½ years of age – the Copenhagen Child Cohort 2000". J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 48 (1): 62–70. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01659.x. PMID 17244271.
- Prior & Glaser (2006), pp. 218–19.
- ^ Chisholm, K; Carter, M; Ames, E; Morison, S (1995). "Attachment Security and indiscriminately friendly behavior in children adopted from Romanian orphanages". Development and Psychopathology. 7 (2): 283–94. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006507. S2CID 145524717.
- ^ Smyke AT, Dumitrescu A, Zeanah CH (2002). "Attachment disturbances in young children. I: The continuum of caretaking casualty". J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 41 (8): 972–82. doi:10.1097/00004583-200208000-00016. PMID 12162633. S2CID 7359043.
- Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Koga SF, Carlson E (2005). "Attachment in institutionalized and community children in Romania". Child Dev. 76 (5): 1015–28. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.417.6482. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00894.x. PMID 16149999.
- Chisholm K (1998). "A three-year follow-up of attachment and indiscriminate friendliness in children adopted from Romanian orphanages". Child Dev. 69 (4): 1092–106. doi:10.2307/1132364. JSTOR 1132364. PMID 9768488.
- Boris NW, Hinshaw-Fuselier SS, Smyke AT, Scheeringa MS, Heller SS, Zeanah CH (2004). "Comparing criteria for attachment disorders: establishing reliability and validity in high-risk samples". J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 43 (5): 568–77. doi:10.1097/00004583-200405000-00010. PMID 15100563.
- Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 215.
- ^ DSM-IV American Psychiatric Association 1994, as discussed in Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 81.
- Bonanno GA (2004). "Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events?" (PDF). American Psychologist. 59 (1): 20–28. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.20. PMID 14736317. S2CID 6296189. Retrieved 26 January 2008.
- ^ Zeanah CH (1996). "Beyond insecurity: a reconceptualization of attachment disorders of infancy". J Consult Clin Psychol. 64 (1): 42–52. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.42. PMID 8907083.
- Bowlby (1997 edition) pp. 224–27.
- Bowlby (1997 edition) pp. 313–17.
- Bretherton I, Munholland KA (1999). "Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships: A Construct Revisited". In Cassidy J and Shaver PR (eds.) Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. pp. 89–111. Guilford Press ISBN 1-57230-087-6.
- Bowlby (1997 edition) p. 354.
- ^ Van Ijzendoorn M, Bakermans-Kranenburg M (September 2003). "Attachment disorders and disorganized attachment: Similar and different". Attachment & Human Development. 5 (3): 313–20(8). doi:10.1080/14616730310001593938. PMID 12944229. S2CID 10644822.
- Thompson RA (2000). "The legacy of early attachments". Child Dev. 71 (1): 145–52. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00128. PMID 10836568.
- O'Connor TG (2002), "Attachment disorders in infancy and childhood". In Rutter M, Taylor E, (Eds.) Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: Modern Approaches (4th ed.) Blackwell Scientific publications. pp. 776–792. ISBN 0-632-01229-3
- Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 77. The APSAC Taskforce Report
- Chaffin et al. (2006), p. 82–83. The APSAC Taskforce Report
- Boris NW, Zeanah CH, Larrieu JA, Scheeringa MS, Heller SS (1 February 1998). "Attachment disorders in infancy and early childhood: a preliminary investigation of diagnostic criteria". Am J Psychiatry. 155 (2): 295–97. doi:10.1176/ajp.155.2.295. PMID 9464217. S2CID 23232613. Retrieved 31 January 2008.
- Boris NW, Zeanah CH (1999). "Disturbance and disorders of attachment in infancy: An overview". Infant Mental Health Journal. 20: 1–9. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199921)20:1<1::AID-IMHJ1>3.0.CO;2-V.
- Zeanah CH (2000). "Disturbances and disorders of attachment in early childhood". In Zeanah CH (Ed.) Handbook of infant mental health (2nd ed.) pp. 358–62. New York: Guilford Press. ISBN 1-59385-171-5
- Schechter DS, Willheim E (2009). Disturbances of attachment and parental psychopathology in early childhood. Infant and Early Childhood Mental Health Issue. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North America, 18(3), 665–687.
- Greenberg MT (1999). Attachment and Psychopathology in Childhood. In Cassidy J and Shaver PR (Eds.) Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. pp. 469–96. Guilford Press ISBN 1-57230-087-6
- Main M, Hesse E (1990) "Parents' unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infants' insecure-disorganized/disoriented attachment status: Is frightened or frightening behavior the linking mechanism?" In Greenberg M, Cicchetti D and Cummings E (Eds.) Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention, pp. 161–182 Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0-226-30630-5.
- DSM-V Proposed Draft. Reactive attachment disorder. American Psychiatric Association. Charles H. Zeanah.
References
- American Psychiatric Association (1994). DSM-IV-TR: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edition. Text Revision. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association. ISBN 0-89042-025-4.
- Bowlby J (1997). Attachment and Loss: Attachment Vol. 1 (Attachment and Loss). Pimlico; New Ed. ISBN 0-7126-7471-3.
- Bowlby J (1973). Attachment and Loss: Separation—Anxiety and Anger v. 2 (International Psycho-Analysis Library). London: Hogarth Press. ISBN 0-7012-0301-3.
- Bowlby J (1980). Attachment and Loss: Loss—Sadness and Depression v. 3 (International Psycho-Analysis Library). London: Hogarth Press. ISBN 0-7012-0350-1.
- Chaffin M, Hanson R, Saunders BE, et al. (2006). "Report of the APSAC task force on attachment therapy, reactive attachment disorder, and attachment problems". Child Maltreat. 11 (1): 76–89. doi:10.1177/1077559505283699. PMID 16382093. S2CID 11443880.
- Mercer J (2006). Understanding Attachment: Parenting, child care and emotional development. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-98217-3.
- Prior V, Glaser D (2006). Understanding Attachment and Attachment Disorders: Theory, Evidence and Practice. Child and Adolescent Mental Health series, RCPRTU, Jessica Kingsley Publishers. ISBN 978-1-84310-245-8 (pbk).
Further reading
- Zeanah, Charles H.; Chesher, Tessa; Boris, Neil W.; AACAP Committee on Quality Issues (November 2016). "Practice Parameter for the Assessment and Treatment of Children and Adolescents With Reactive Attachment Disorder and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder". Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 55 (11): 990–1003. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2016.08.004. PMID 27806867.
Classification | D |
---|---|
External resources |
Emotional and behavioral disorders | |
---|---|
Emotional/behavioral |
|