Misplaced Pages

Biosocial criminology: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 14:48, 30 June 2012 editArtifexMayhem (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users3,625 edits Genetics: not supported by the source (assuming the source is used properly)← Previous edit Latest revision as of 06:47, 5 November 2024 edit undoLWG (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users3,508 edits rm dead pov tag: no extant dispute 
(114 intermediate revisions by 59 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Psychosocial examination of crime}}
{{CriminTheo}}
{{NPOV|date=June 2012}} {{Use dmy dates|date=May 2017}}
{{weasel|date=June 2012}} {{weasel|date=June 2012}}

'''Biosocial criminology''' is an ] field that aims to explain crime and antisocial behavior by exploring both biological factors and environmental factors. While contemporary ] has been dominated by ] theories, biosocial criminology also recognizes the potential contributions of fields such as ], ], and ].<ref name=Asghate2011>Kevin M. Beaver and Anthony Walsh. 2011. Biosocial Criminology. Chapter 1 in The Ashgate Research Companion to Biosocial Theories of Crime. 2011. Ashgate.</ref>
{{Criminology and penology |theory}}
{{Evolutionary biology}}
'''Biosocial criminology''' is an interdisciplinary field that aims to explain crime and ] by exploring ] factors. While contemporary ] has been dominated by ] theories, biosocial criminology also recognizes the potential contributions of fields such as ], ], and ].<ref name="Asghate2011">Kevin M. Beaver and Anthony Walsh. 2011. Biosocial Criminology. Chapter 1 in The Ashgate Research Companion to Biosocial Theories of Crime. 2011. Ashgate.</ref>


==Approaches== ==Approaches==
Line 8: Line 11:
Environment has a significant effect on genetic expression. Disadvantaged environments enhance antisocial gene expression, suppress prosocial gene action and prevent the realization of genetic potential.<ref name=Asghate2011/> Environment has a significant effect on genetic expression. Disadvantaged environments enhance antisocial gene expression, suppress prosocial gene action and prevent the realization of genetic potential.<ref name=Asghate2011/>


{{quotation|Genes and environments operating in tandem (interacting) were required to produce significant antisocial behavior, while neither was powerful enough to produce it independent of the other. That is, children genetically at risk for antisocial behavior reared in positive family environments did not display antisocial behavior, and children not at genetic risk did not become antisocial in adverse family environments.<ref name=Asghate2011/>}} {{Blockquote|Genes and environments operating in tandem (interacting) were required to produce significant antisocial behavior, while neither was powerful enough to produce it independent of the other. That is, children genetically at risk for antisocial behavior reared in positive family environments did not display antisocial behavior, and children not at genetic risk did not become antisocial in adverse family environments.<ref name=Asghate2011/>}}


===Genetics=== ===Behavioral genetics===
One approach to studying the role of genetics for crime is to calculate the ] coefficient. It describes the proportion of the ] that is due to actualized genetic effects for some trait in a given population in a specific environment at a specific time. The heritability coefficient for antisocial behavior is estimated to be between 0.40 and 0.58.<ref name=Asghate2011/> One approach to studying the role of genetics for crime is to calculate the ] coefficient, which describes the proportion of the ] that is due to actualized genetic effects for some trait in a given population in a specific environment at a specific time. According to ] and ], the heritability coefficient for antisocial behavior is estimated to be between 0.40 and 0.58.<ref name=Asghate2011/>

The methodology often used in biosocial criminology (that of ]) has been criticized for producing inflated heritability estimates,<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Burt |first1=Callie H. |last2=Simons |first2=Ronald L.|author-link2=Ronald L. Simons |date=2014-05-01 |title=Pulling Back the Curtain on Heritability Studies: Biosocial Criminology in the Postgenomic Era |journal=Criminology |language=en |volume=52 |issue=2 |pages=223–262 |doi=10.1111/1745-9125.12036 |issn=1745-9125}}</ref> though biosocial criminologists maintain that these criticisms are baseless.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Barnes |first1=J. C. |last2=Wright |first2=John Paul|author-link2=John Paul Wright |last3=Boutwell |first3=Brian B. |author-link3=Brian Boutwell|last4=Schwartz |first4=Joseph A. |last5=Connolly |first5=Eric J. |last6=Nedelec |first6=Joseph L. |last7=Beaver |first7=Kevin M. |author-link7=Kevin Beaver|date=2014-11-01 |title=Demonstrating the Validity of Twin Research in Criminology |journal=Criminology |language=en |volume=52 |issue=4 |pages=588–626 |doi=10.1111/1745-9125.12049 |issn=1745-9125}}</ref> Criminal justice researchers Brian Boutwell and J.C. Barnes argue that many sociological studies that do not control for genetic inheritance of risk factors have misleading or unreliable results.<ref></ref>


=== Neurophysiology === === Neurophysiology ===
Another approach is to examine the relationship between neurophysiology and criminality. One example is that measured levels of neurotransmitters such as ] and ] have been associated with criminal behavior. Another is that ] studies give strong evidence for that both brain structure and function are involved in criminal behaviors. The ] creates emotions such as anger and jealousy that ultimately may cause criminal behavior. The ] is involved in delaying gratification and impulse control and moderates the impulses from the limbic system. If this balance is shifted in favor of the limbic system this may contribute to criminal behavior. ] argues that "life-course-persistent offenders" make up only 6% of the population but commits more than 50% of all crimes and that this is due to a combination neurophysiological deficits and an adverse environment that creates a criminal path that is very difficult to break once started.<ref name=Asghate2011/> Another approach is to examine the relationship between neurophysiology and criminality. One example is that measured levels of neurotransmitters such as ] and ] have been associated with criminal behavior. Another is that ] studies give strong evidence that both brain structure and function are involved in criminal behaviors. The ] creates emotions such as anger and jealousy that ultimately may cause criminal behavior. The ] is involved in delaying gratification and impulse control and moderates the impulses from the limbic system. If this balance is shifted in favor of the limbic system this may contribute to criminal behavior. ] argues that "life-course-persistent offenders" make up only 6% of the population but commits more than 50% of all crimes and that this is due to a combination neurophysiological deficits and an adverse environment that creates a criminal path that is very difficult to break once started.<ref name=Asghate2011/>


=== Evolutionary psychology === === Evolutionary psychology ===
Men can potentially have many children with little effort; women only a few with great effort. This is argued{{who|date=June 2012}} to contribute to males having more variable ] than females. One argued consequence of this is that males are more ], and more violently aggressive, than females since they face higher reproductive competition from their own sex than females. In particular, low status males may easily become completely childless. Under such circumstances it may been have evolutionary useful to take very high risks and use violent aggression in order to try increase status and reproductive success rather than become genetically extinct. This may explain why males have higher crime rates than females and why low status and being unmarried is associated with criminality. It may also explain why the degree of income inequality of a society is a better predictor than the absolute income level of the society for male-male homocides. This because increased income inequality increases status differences in society while changing the average income level may not do so. Furthermore, competition over females are argued to evolutionary have been particularly intensive in late adolescence and young adulthood which may explain why crime rates are particularly high during this period.<ref name=AEP>Aurelio José Figueredo, Paul Robert Gladden, Zachary Hohman. The evolutionary psychology of criminal behaviour. In {{cite doi|10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001}}</ref> Men can potentially have many children with little effort; women only a few with great effort. One argued consequence of this is that males are more aggressive, and more violently aggressive, than females, since they face higher reproductive competition from their own sex than females. In particular, low-status males may be more likely to remain completely childless. Under such circumstances, it may have been evolutionarily useful to take very high risks and use violent aggression in order to try to increase status and reproductive success rather than become genetically extinct. This may explain why males have higher crime rates than females and why low status and being unmarried is associated with criminality. It may also explain why the degree of income inequality of a society is a better predictor than the absolute income level of the society for male-male homicides; income inequality creates social disparity, while differing average income levels may not do so. Furthermore, competition over females is argued to have been particularly intensive in late adolescence and young adulthood, which is theorized to explain why crime rates are particularly high during this period.<ref name=AEP>Aurelio José Figueredo, Paul Robert Gladden, Zachary Hohman. The evolutionary psychology of criminal behaviour. In {{Cite book | last1 = Roberts | first1 = S. C. | editor1-last = Roberts | doi = 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001 | editor1-first = S. Craig | title = Applied Evolutionary Psychology | year = 2011 | publisher = Oxford University Press| isbn = 9780199586073 }}</ref>


The "evolutionary neuroandrogenic theory" focuses on the hormone ] as a factor influencing aggression and criminality and being evolutionary beneficial during certain forms of competition. In most species, males are more aggressive than females. Castration of males usually has a pacifying effect on aggressive behavior in males. In humans, males engage in crime and especially violent crime more than females. The involvement in crime usually rises in the early teens to mid teens which happen at the same time as testosterone levels rise. Research on the relationship between testosterone and aggression is difficult since the only reliable measurement of brain testosterone is by a ] which is not done for research purposes. Studies therefore have often instead used more unreliable measurements from blood or saliva. Most studies support a link between adult criminality and testosterone although the relationship is modest if examined separately for each sex. Nearly all studies of juvenile delinquency and testosterone are not significant. Most studies have also found testosterone to be associated with behaviors or personality traits linked with criminality such as ] and ]. Many studies have also been done on the relationship between more general aggressive behavior/feelings and testosterone. About half the studies have found a relationship and about half no relationship.<ref name="EllisBeaver2009">{{cite book|author1=Lee Ellis| author2=Kevin M. Beaver|author3=John Wright| title=Handbook of Crime Correlates| url=http://books.google.com/books?id=eD0ttBXoMvQC| date=1 April 2009| publisher=Academic Press| isbn=978-0-12-373612-3}}</ref> The "]" focuses on the hormone ] as a factor influencing aggression and criminality and being beneficial during certain forms of competition.<ref name="psychology-criminalbehavior-law.com">{{cite web|title=Should We Be More Concerned With Prenatal Exposure in the Prevention Of Crime?|url=http://www.psychology-criminalbehavior-law.com/2015/04/should-we-be-more-concerned-with-prenatal-exposure-in-the-prevention-of-crime//|access-date=23 April 2015}}</ref> In most species, males are more aggressive than females. Castration of males usually has a pacifying effect on aggressive behavior in males.<ref name="crimetimes.org">{{cite web|title=MILD TESTOSTERONE REDUCTION EFFECTIVE AGAINST AGGRESSION?|url=http://www.crimetimes.org/95d/w95dp6.htm|access-date=23 April 2015|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150504184242/http://www.crimetimes.org/95d/w95dp6.htm|archive-date=4 May 2015|url-status=dead}}</ref> In humans, males engage in crime and especially violent crime more than females. The involvement in crime usually rises in the early teens to mid teens in correlation with the rise of testosterone levels. Research on the relationship between testosterone and aggression is difficult since the only reliable measurement of brain testosterone is by ], which is not done for research purposes. Studies therefore have often instead used less reliable measurements from blood or saliva. Some studies support a link between adult criminality and testosterone, although the relationship is modest if examined separately for each sex. A significant link between juvenile delinquency and testosterone levels has not been established. Some studies have also found testosterone to be associated with behaviors or personality traits linked with criminality such as ] and alcoholism. Many studies have also been done on the relationship between more general aggressive behavior/feelings and testosterone. About half the studies have found a relationship and about half no relationship.<ref name="EllisBeaver2009">{{cite book |author1=Lee Ellis| author2=Kevin M. Beaver|author3=John Wright| title=Handbook of Crime Correlates| url=https://books.google.com/books?id=eD0ttBXoMvQC| date=1 April 2009| publisher=Academic Press| isbn=978-0-12-373612-3}}</ref>


Many conflicts causing homocides involve status conflicts, protecting reputation, and seemingly trivial insults.<ref name=AEP/> ] in his book '']'' argues that in non-state societies without a police it was very important to have a credible ] against aggression. Therefore it was important to have a reputation for retaliation, causing humans to develop instincts for ] as well as for protecting reputation ("]"). Pinker argues that the development of the state and the police have dramatically reduced the level of violence compared to the ancestral environment. Whenever the state breaks down, which can be very locally such as in poor areas of a city, humans again organize in groups for protection and aggression and concepts such as violent revenge and protecting honor again become extremely important. Many conflicts causing homicides involve status conflicts, protecting reputation, and seemingly trivial insults.<ref name=AEP/> ] in his book '']'' argues that in non-state societies without a police it was very important to have a credible ] against aggression. Therefore, it was important to have a reputation for retaliation, causing humans to develop instincts for ] as well as for protecting reputation ("honor"). Pinker argues that the development of the state and the police have dramatically reduced the level of violence compared to the ancestral environment. Whenever the state breaks down, which can be very locally such as in poor areas of a city, humans again organize in groups for protection and aggression and concepts such as violent revenge and protecting honor again become extremely important.


Some cultures place greater emphasis on protecting honor than other cultures. One explanation is that protecting honor was in the ancestral past relatively more important for herders than for farmers. The livestock of herders were easily and quickly stolen. As a result it was important to constantly show toughness as a deterrence which may cause a higher level of violence. The predictions of the theory was confirmed in a cross-cultural examination of traditional farming and herding Spanish-American societies. However, the prediction that sedentary fishing societies would place a low emphasis on honor was not confirmed.<ref name=AEP/> Some cultures place greater emphasis on protecting honor than other cultures. One explanation is that protecting honor was in the ancestral past relatively more important for herders than for farmers. The livestock of herders were easily and quickly stolen. As a result, it was important to constantly show toughness as a deterrence, which may cause a higher level of violence. The predictions of the theory was confirmed in a cross-cultural examination of traditional farming and herding Spanish-American societies. However, the prediction that sedentary fishing societies would place a low emphasis on honor was not confirmed.<ref name=AEP/>


The degree of cultural ] is strongly associated with the burden of ]. It has been argued that this is due to collectivism and associated characteristics such as ] avoidance limiting the spread of infectious diseases. Other characteristics such as strong ] and willingness to defend the ]'s honor may promote violence. A study found strong associations between several forms of violent criminal behavior and both infectious disease rates across U.S states and degree of cultural collectivism across U.S. states. The associations remained strong after controlling for income inequality.<ref name=AEP/> The degree of cultural ] is strongly associated with the burden of ]. It has been argued that this is due to collectivism and associated characteristics such as ] avoidance limiting the spread of infectious diseases. Other characteristics such as strong ] and willingness to defend the ]'s honor may promote violence. A study found strong associations between several forms of violent criminal behavior and both infectious disease rates across U.S states and degree of cultural collectivism across U.S. states. The associations remained strong after controlling for income inequality.<ref name=AEP/>


==== Specific forms ==== ==== Specific forms ====
Evolutionary psychology researchers have proposed several evolutionary explanations for ]. One is that psychopathy represents a frequency-dependent, socially ] strategy. This may work as long as there are few other psychopaths in the community since more psychopaths means increasing the risk of encountering another psychopath as well as non-psychopaths likely adapting more countermeasures against cheaters.<ref>{{cite doi|10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009}}</ref><ref>{{cite doi|10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x}}</ref> Evolutionary psychology researchers have proposed several evolutionary explanations for ]. One is that psychopathy represents a frequency-dependent, socially ] strategy. This may benefit the psychopath as long as there are few other psychopaths in the community since more psychopaths means increasing the risk of encountering another psychopath as well as non-psychopaths likely adapting more countermeasures against cheaters.<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Glenn | first1 = A. L. | last2 = Kurzban | first2 = R. | last3 = Raine | first3 = A. | doi = 10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009 | title = Evolutionary theory and psychopathy | journal = Aggression and Violent Behavior | volume = 16 | issue = 5 | pages = 371 | year = 2011 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Buss | first1 = D. M. | title = How Can Evolutionary Psychology Successfully Explain Personality and Individual Differences? | doi = 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x | journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science | volume = 4 | issue = 4 | pages = 359–366 | year = 2009 | pmid = 26158983| s2cid = 2565416 }}</ref><ref>Harpending, H. C., & Sobus, J. (1987). Sociopathy as an adaptation. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, 63-72.</ref>


] are theories that explore to what degree, if any, evolutionary adaptations influence the psychology of rapists. Such theories are highly controversial, as traditional theories typically do not consider rape to be a behavioral adaptation. Some object to such theories on ethical, religious, political, as well as scientific grounds. Others argue that a correct knowledge of the causes of rape is necessary in order to develop effective preventive measures. ] are theories that explore to what degree, if any, evolutionary adaptations influence the psychology of rapists. Such theories are highly controversial, as traditional theories typically do not consider rape to be a behavioral adaptation. Some object to such theories on ethical, religious, political, as well as scientific grounds. Others argue that a correct knowledge of the causes of rape is necessary in order to develop effective preventive measures.


The ] is a term used to describe the observed, in many but not all studies, significantly higher rate of ] being abused by ] as compared to genetic parents. The effect has been explained by application of ] theories. There have also been various criticisms of these theories{{who|date=June 2012}}.<ref name=DalyWilson2007>Daly & Wilson (2007) In Crawford & Krebs (Eds) Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology, pp. 383-400. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.</ref> The ] is the alleged higher rate of ] being abused by ] as compared to genetic parents, observed in some, but not all, studies. An explanation of this affect has been attempted by application of ] theories. There have also been various criticisms of these theories.<ref name=DalyWilson2007>Daly & Wilson (2007) {{webarchive |url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110516102016/http://psych.mcmaster.ca/dalywilson/Cinderella_Effect.pdf |date=16 May 2011 }} In Crawford & Krebs (Eds) Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology, pp. 383-400. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.</ref>


] is one of the few forms of violence more often done by women than men. Cross-cultural research have found that this is more likely to occur when the child has deformities or illnesses as well as when there are lacking resources due to factors such as poverty, other children requiring resources, and no male support. Such a child may have a low chance of reproductive success in which case it would decrease the mother's ] to spend resources on the child, in particular since women generally have a greater ] than men.<ref>{{cite doi|10.1037/a0026610}}</ref> ] is one of the few forms of violence more often done by women than men. Cross-cultural research has found that this is more likely to occur when the child has deformities or illnesses as well as when there are lacking resources due to factors such as poverty, other children requiring resources, and no male support. Such a child may have a low chance of reproductive success, in which case it would decrease the mother's ] to spend resources on the child, in particular since women generally have a greater ] than men.<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Liddle | first1 = J. R. | last2 = Shackelford | first2 = T. K. | last3 = Weekes–Shackelford | first3 = V. A. | title = Why can't we all just get along? Evolutionary perspectives on violence, homicide, and war | doi = 10.1037/a0026610 | journal = Review of General Psychology | volume = 16 | pages = 24–36 | year = 2012 | s2cid = 142984456 }}</ref>


==== Criminal justice ==== ==== Criminal justice ====
Punishment of exploitative behaviors harmful to the group was likely a recurring problem in the ancestral environment. As such humans are argued to have developed a range of psychological mechanisms for handling this. Punishment can be a deterrent to undesired behaviors but excessive punishment can also be harmful to the group. Thus, human are argued to favor a proportional response based on how severe the offence is. Cross-cultural research have a found a high agreement regarding how relatively harmful different crimes are perceived to be. On the other hand, evolutionary novel factors that may be rational to consider from a deterrent perspective, such as how difficult it is for the modern police to detect the crime, do not seem to affect people's perceptions of appropriate punishments.<ref name=AEP2>Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In {{cite doi|10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001}}</ref> Punishment of exploitative behaviors harmful to the group was likely a recurring problem in the ancestral environment. As such humans are argued to have developed a range of psychological mechanisms for handling this. Punishment can be a deterrent to undesired behaviors but excessive punishment can also be harmful to the group. Thus, humans are argued to favor a proportional response based on how severe the offence is. Cross-cultural research have a found a high agreement regarding how relatively harmful different crimes are perceived to be. On the other hand, evolutionary novel factors that may be rational to consider from a deterrent perspective, such as how difficult it is for the modern police to detect the crime, do not seem to affect people's perceptions of appropriate punishments.<ref name=AEP2>Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In {{Cite book | last1 = Roberts | first1 = S. C. | editor1-last = Roberts | doi = 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001 | editor1-first = S. Craig | title = Applied Evolutionary Psychology | year = 2011 | publisher = Oxford University Press| isbn = 9780199586073 }}</ref>


Once a crime's severity has been judged there is a choice regarding how to respond. In some cases in the ancestral environment there may have been benefits from future interactions with the offender which some forms of punishment may have prevented as compared to responses such as reparations or rehabilitation. Research suggests that individuals may modify what they think are appropriate forms of response to offenders based on factors that once in the past small-group environment may have indicated that they could personally benefit from continued interactions with the offender such as kinship, in-group or out-group membership, possession of resources, sexual attractiveness, expressed remorse, intentionality, and prior history of cooperation and exploitation.<ref name=AEP2/> Once a crime's severity has been judged, there is a choice regarding how to respond. In some cases in the ancestral environment there may have been benefits from future interactions with the offender which some forms of punishment may have prevented as compared to responses such as reparations or rehabilitation. Research suggests that individuals may modify what they think are appropriate forms of response to offenders based on factors that once in the past small-group environment may have indicated that they could personally benefit from continued interactions with the offender such as kinship, in-group or out-group membership, possession of resources, sexual attractiveness, expressed remorse, intentionality, and prior history of cooperation and exploitation.<ref name=AEP2/>


==See also== ==See also==
{{Portal|Biology|Society}}
* ] * ]
* ] * ]
* ] * ]
* ] * ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ]


==References== ==References==
Line 52: Line 60:


==Further reading== ==Further reading==
* Anthony Walsh, Kevin M. Beaver, ''Biosocial criminology: new directions in theory and research'', Taylor & Francis, 2008, ISBN 0-415-98944-2 *Walsh, A., & Beaver, K.M. (2008). ''Biosocial criminology: new directions in theory and research.'' New York City: Taylor & Francis, {{ISBN|0-415-98944-2}}.
* Anthony Walsh, Lee Ellis, ''Biosocial criminology: challenging environmentalism's supremacy'', Nova Science Publishers, 2003, ISBN 1-59033-774-3 *Anthony Walsh, Lee Ellis, ''Biosocial criminology: challenging environmentalism's supremacy'', Nova Science Publishers, 2003, {{ISBN|1-59033-774-3}}
*Matt DeLisi, Michael G Vaughn, ''The Routledge International Handbook of Biosocial Criminology'', Routledge, 2015, {{ISBN|9781317936749}}
* Kevin Beaver. ''Biosocial Criminology: A Primer'' Ken Hunt Publishing Company. 2009. *Kevin Beaver. ''Biosocial Criminology: A Primer'' Ken Hunt Publishing Company. 2009.
*The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture
*Jerome H. Barkow (Editor), Leda Cosmides (Editor), John Tooby (Editor) *The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture . Jerome H. Barkow (Editor), Leda Cosmides (Editor), John Tooby (Editor)
*Homicide (Foundations of Human Behavior) *Homicide (Foundations of Human Behavior) , Margo Wilson (Author), Martin Daly (Author)
*Margo Wilson (Author), Martin Daly (Author)
*How the Mind Works , Steven Pinker (Author) *How the Mind Works , Steven Pinker (Author)
*Demonic Males by Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson *Demonic Males by Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson
*Before the Dawn: Recovering the Lost History of Our Ancestors
*Human Morality and Sociality: Evolutionary and Comparative Perspectives by Henrik Hogh-Olesen, Christophe Boesch, Leda Cosmides and Azar Gat (Jan 19, 2010) *Human Morality and Sociality: Evolutionary and Comparative Perspectives by Henrik Hogh-Olesen, Christophe Boesch, Leda Cosmides and Azar Gat (Jan 19, 2010)
*Sex, Evolution and Behavior by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson *Sex, Evolution and Behavior by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson
*Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (4th Edition) by David M. Buss (Feb 28, 2011) *Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (4th Edition) by David M. Buss (Feb 28, 2011)

{{Evolutionary psychology}}
{{Authority control}}


] ]
Line 70: Line 79:
] ]
] ]
]

Latest revision as of 06:47, 5 November 2024

Psychosocial examination of crime

This article contains weasel words: vague phrasing that often accompanies biased or unverifiable information. Such statements should be clarified or removed. (June 2012)
Criminology and penology
Theory
Major theorists
Types of crime
Methods
Penology
Justice in penology
Schools
Subfields
Browse
Part of a series on
Evolutionary biology
Darwin's finches by John Gould
Processes and outcomes
Natural history
History of evolutionary theory
Fields and applications
Social implications

Biosocial criminology is an interdisciplinary field that aims to explain crime and antisocial behavior by exploring biocultural factors. While contemporary criminology has been dominated by sociological theories, biosocial criminology also recognizes the potential contributions of fields such as behavioral genetics, neuropsychology, and evolutionary psychology.

Approaches

Environment

Environment has a significant effect on genetic expression. Disadvantaged environments enhance antisocial gene expression, suppress prosocial gene action and prevent the realization of genetic potential.

Genes and environments operating in tandem (interacting) were required to produce significant antisocial behavior, while neither was powerful enough to produce it independent of the other. That is, children genetically at risk for antisocial behavior reared in positive family environments did not display antisocial behavior, and children not at genetic risk did not become antisocial in adverse family environments.

Behavioral genetics

One approach to studying the role of genetics for crime is to calculate the heritability coefficient, which describes the proportion of the variance that is due to actualized genetic effects for some trait in a given population in a specific environment at a specific time. According to Kevin Beaver and Anthony Walsh, the heritability coefficient for antisocial behavior is estimated to be between 0.40 and 0.58.

The methodology often used in biosocial criminology (that of twin studies) has been criticized for producing inflated heritability estimates, though biosocial criminologists maintain that these criticisms are baseless. Criminal justice researchers Brian Boutwell and J.C. Barnes argue that many sociological studies that do not control for genetic inheritance of risk factors have misleading or unreliable results.

Neurophysiology

Another approach is to examine the relationship between neurophysiology and criminality. One example is that measured levels of neurotransmitters such as serotonin and dopamine have been associated with criminal behavior. Another is that neuroimaging studies give strong evidence that both brain structure and function are involved in criminal behaviors. The limbic system creates emotions such as anger and jealousy that ultimately may cause criminal behavior. The prefrontal cortex is involved in delaying gratification and impulse control and moderates the impulses from the limbic system. If this balance is shifted in favor of the limbic system this may contribute to criminal behavior. Terrie Moffitt's developmental theory of crime argues that "life-course-persistent offenders" make up only 6% of the population but commits more than 50% of all crimes and that this is due to a combination neurophysiological deficits and an adverse environment that creates a criminal path that is very difficult to break once started.

Evolutionary psychology

Men can potentially have many children with little effort; women only a few with great effort. One argued consequence of this is that males are more aggressive, and more violently aggressive, than females, since they face higher reproductive competition from their own sex than females. In particular, low-status males may be more likely to remain completely childless. Under such circumstances, it may have been evolutionarily useful to take very high risks and use violent aggression in order to try to increase status and reproductive success rather than become genetically extinct. This may explain why males have higher crime rates than females and why low status and being unmarried is associated with criminality. It may also explain why the degree of income inequality of a society is a better predictor than the absolute income level of the society for male-male homicides; income inequality creates social disparity, while differing average income levels may not do so. Furthermore, competition over females is argued to have been particularly intensive in late adolescence and young adulthood, which is theorized to explain why crime rates are particularly high during this period.

The "evolutionary neuroandrogenic theory" focuses on the hormone testosterone as a factor influencing aggression and criminality and being beneficial during certain forms of competition. In most species, males are more aggressive than females. Castration of males usually has a pacifying effect on aggressive behavior in males. In humans, males engage in crime and especially violent crime more than females. The involvement in crime usually rises in the early teens to mid teens in correlation with the rise of testosterone levels. Research on the relationship between testosterone and aggression is difficult since the only reliable measurement of brain testosterone is by lumbar puncture, which is not done for research purposes. Studies therefore have often instead used less reliable measurements from blood or saliva. Some studies support a link between adult criminality and testosterone, although the relationship is modest if examined separately for each sex. A significant link between juvenile delinquency and testosterone levels has not been established. Some studies have also found testosterone to be associated with behaviors or personality traits linked with criminality such as antisocial behavior and alcoholism. Many studies have also been done on the relationship between more general aggressive behavior/feelings and testosterone. About half the studies have found a relationship and about half no relationship.

Many conflicts causing homicides involve status conflicts, protecting reputation, and seemingly trivial insults. Steven Pinker in his book The Blank Slate argues that in non-state societies without a police it was very important to have a credible deterrence against aggression. Therefore, it was important to have a reputation for retaliation, causing humans to develop instincts for revenge as well as for protecting reputation ("honor"). Pinker argues that the development of the state and the police have dramatically reduced the level of violence compared to the ancestral environment. Whenever the state breaks down, which can be very locally such as in poor areas of a city, humans again organize in groups for protection and aggression and concepts such as violent revenge and protecting honor again become extremely important.

Some cultures place greater emphasis on protecting honor than other cultures. One explanation is that protecting honor was in the ancestral past relatively more important for herders than for farmers. The livestock of herders were easily and quickly stolen. As a result, it was important to constantly show toughness as a deterrence, which may cause a higher level of violence. The predictions of the theory was confirmed in a cross-cultural examination of traditional farming and herding Spanish-American societies. However, the prediction that sedentary fishing societies would place a low emphasis on honor was not confirmed.

The degree of cultural collectivism is strongly associated with the burden of infectious disease. It has been argued that this is due to collectivism and associated characteristics such as out-group avoidance limiting the spread of infectious diseases. Other characteristics such as strong in-group–out-group bias and willingness to defend the ingroup's honor may promote violence. A study found strong associations between several forms of violent criminal behavior and both infectious disease rates across U.S states and degree of cultural collectivism across U.S. states. The associations remained strong after controlling for income inequality.

Specific forms

Evolutionary psychology researchers have proposed several evolutionary explanations for psychopathy. One is that psychopathy represents a frequency-dependent, socially parasitic strategy. This may benefit the psychopath as long as there are few other psychopaths in the community since more psychopaths means increasing the risk of encountering another psychopath as well as non-psychopaths likely adapting more countermeasures against cheaters.

Sociobiological theories of rape are theories that explore to what degree, if any, evolutionary adaptations influence the psychology of rapists. Such theories are highly controversial, as traditional theories typically do not consider rape to be a behavioral adaptation. Some object to such theories on ethical, religious, political, as well as scientific grounds. Others argue that a correct knowledge of the causes of rape is necessary in order to develop effective preventive measures.

The Cinderella effect is the alleged higher rate of stepchildren being abused by stepparents as compared to genetic parents, observed in some, but not all, studies. An explanation of this affect has been attempted by application of evolutionary psychology theories. There have also been various criticisms of these theories.

Infanticide is one of the few forms of violence more often done by women than men. Cross-cultural research has found that this is more likely to occur when the child has deformities or illnesses as well as when there are lacking resources due to factors such as poverty, other children requiring resources, and no male support. Such a child may have a low chance of reproductive success, in which case it would decrease the mother's inclusive fitness to spend resources on the child, in particular since women generally have a greater parental investment than men.

Criminal justice

Punishment of exploitative behaviors harmful to the group was likely a recurring problem in the ancestral environment. As such humans are argued to have developed a range of psychological mechanisms for handling this. Punishment can be a deterrent to undesired behaviors but excessive punishment can also be harmful to the group. Thus, humans are argued to favor a proportional response based on how severe the offence is. Cross-cultural research have a found a high agreement regarding how relatively harmful different crimes are perceived to be. On the other hand, evolutionary novel factors that may be rational to consider from a deterrent perspective, such as how difficult it is for the modern police to detect the crime, do not seem to affect people's perceptions of appropriate punishments.

Once a crime's severity has been judged, there is a choice regarding how to respond. In some cases in the ancestral environment there may have been benefits from future interactions with the offender which some forms of punishment may have prevented as compared to responses such as reparations or rehabilitation. Research suggests that individuals may modify what they think are appropriate forms of response to offenders based on factors that once in the past small-group environment may have indicated that they could personally benefit from continued interactions with the offender such as kinship, in-group or out-group membership, possession of resources, sexual attractiveness, expressed remorse, intentionality, and prior history of cooperation and exploitation.

See also

References

  1. ^ Kevin M. Beaver and Anthony Walsh. 2011. Biosocial Criminology. Chapter 1 in The Ashgate Research Companion to Biosocial Theories of Crime. 2011. Ashgate.
  2. Burt, Callie H.; Simons, Ronald L. (1 May 2014). "Pulling Back the Curtain on Heritability Studies: Biosocial Criminology in the Postgenomic Era". Criminology. 52 (2): 223–262. doi:10.1111/1745-9125.12036. ISSN 1745-9125.
  3. Barnes, J. C.; Wright, John Paul; Boutwell, Brian B.; Schwartz, Joseph A.; Connolly, Eric J.; Nedelec, Joseph L.; Beaver, Kevin M. (1 November 2014). "Demonstrating the Validity of Twin Research in Criminology". Criminology. 52 (4): 588–626. doi:10.1111/1745-9125.12049. ISSN 1745-9125.
  4. Is crime genetic? Scientists don't know because they're afraid to ask
  5. ^ Aurelio José Figueredo, Paul Robert Gladden, Zachary Hohman. The evolutionary psychology of criminal behaviour. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
  6. "Should We Be More Concerned With Prenatal Exposure in the Prevention Of Crime?". Retrieved 23 April 2015.
  7. "MILD TESTOSTERONE REDUCTION EFFECTIVE AGAINST AGGRESSION?". Archived from the original on 4 May 2015. Retrieved 23 April 2015.
  8. Lee Ellis; Kevin M. Beaver; John Wright (1 April 2009). Handbook of Crime Correlates. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-373612-3.
  9. Glenn, A. L.; Kurzban, R.; Raine, A. (2011). "Evolutionary theory and psychopathy". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 16 (5): 371. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009.
  10. Buss, D. M. (2009). "How Can Evolutionary Psychology Successfully Explain Personality and Individual Differences?". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 4 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x. PMID 26158983. S2CID 2565416.
  11. Harpending, H. C., & Sobus, J. (1987). Sociopathy as an adaptation. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, 63-72.
  12. Daly & Wilson (2007) Is the "Cinderella Effect" controversial? Archived 16 May 2011 at the Wayback Machine In Crawford & Krebs (Eds) Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology, pp. 383-400. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  13. Liddle, J. R.; Shackelford, T. K.; Weekes–Shackelford, V. A. (2012). "Why can't we all just get along? Evolutionary perspectives on violence, homicide, and war". Review of General Psychology. 16: 24–36. doi:10.1037/a0026610. S2CID 142984456.
  14. ^ Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.

Further reading

  • Walsh, A., & Beaver, K.M. (2008). Biosocial criminology: new directions in theory and research. New York City: Taylor & Francis, ISBN 0-415-98944-2.
  • Anthony Walsh, Lee Ellis, Biosocial criminology: challenging environmentalism's supremacy, Nova Science Publishers, 2003, ISBN 1-59033-774-3
  • Matt DeLisi, Michael G Vaughn, The Routledge International Handbook of Biosocial Criminology, Routledge, 2015, ISBN 9781317936749
  • Kevin Beaver. Biosocial Criminology: A Primer Ken Hunt Publishing Company. 2009.
  • The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture . Jerome H. Barkow (Editor), Leda Cosmides (Editor), John Tooby (Editor)
  • Homicide (Foundations of Human Behavior) , Margo Wilson (Author), Martin Daly (Author)
  • How the Mind Works , Steven Pinker (Author)
  • Demonic Males by Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson
  • Human Morality and Sociality: Evolutionary and Comparative Perspectives by Henrik Hogh-Olesen, Christophe Boesch, Leda Cosmides and Azar Gat (Jan 19, 2010)
  • Sex, Evolution and Behavior by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson
  • Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (4th Edition) by David M. Buss (Feb 28, 2011)
Evolutionary psychology
Evolutionary
processes
Areas
Cognition /
Emotion
Culture
Development
Human factors /
Mental health
Sex
Sex differences
Related subjects
Academic disciplines
Research topics
Theoretical positions
Categories: