Revision as of 21:52, 18 November 2006 view source"jamy" Jen & Amy (talk | contribs)15 edits →Influences of Temperament on Family Life← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 23:22, 8 February 2024 view source Utonsal (talk | contribs)97 editsm →Rusalov-Trofimova neurophysiological model of temperament: syntax correction | ||
(438 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Use American English|date=July 2023}} | |||
{{for|temperament in music|musical temperament}} | |||
{{Short description|Term used to indicate the mixture of the innate aspects of the personality}} | |||
{{for|temperament in dog fancy|conformation point}} | |||
{{About|psychological temperament|musical temperament|Musical temperament}} | |||
{{cleanup-date|November 2006}} | |||
{{pp|small=yes}} | |||
In psychology, '''temperament''' is the innate aspect of an individual's personality, such as ] or ]. | |||
In psychology, '''temperament''' broadly refers to consistent individual differences in behavior that are biologically based and are relatively independent of learning, system of values and attitudes. | |||
Temperament is defined as that part of the personality which is genetically based. Along with character, and those aspects acquired through learning, the two together are said to constitute personality. | |||
Some researchers point to association of temperament with formal dynamical features of behavior, such as energetic aspects, plasticity, sensitivity to specific reinforcers and emotionality.<ref name=Tro&Rusal>{{cite book | year=2007|last1=Rusalov|first1=VM |last2= Trofimova | first2=IN|title=Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.}}</ref> Temperament traits (such as neuroticism, sociability, impulsivity, etc.) are distinct patterns in behavior throughout a lifetime, but they are most noticeable and most studied in children. Babies are typically described by temperament, but longitudinal research in the 1920s began to establish temperament as something which is stable across the lifespan.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Friedman, Schustack|first1=Howard S., Miriam W.|title=Personality: Classic Theories and Modern Research|date=2016|publisher=Pearson Education|location=USA|isbn=9780133829808}}</ref> | |||
Historically the concept was part of the theory of the ], which had corresponding ]. It played an important part in premodern psychology, and was important to philosophers like ] and ]. | |||
==Definition== | |||
More recently, with the emphasis on the biological basis of personality, the relationship between temperament and character has been examined with renewed interest | |||
Temperament has been defined as "the constellation of inborn traits that determine a child's unique behavioral style and the way he or she experiences and reacts to the world."<ref>Kristal, J. (2005). The temperament perspective: Working with children's behavior styles. New York, NY: Brookes Publishing Co.</ref> | |||
It has also inspired artists like Carl Nielsen, and Hindemith, whose music is featured in George Balanchine's ballet "The Four Temperaments." See also ]. | |||
===Classification schemes=== | |||
==Temperament in infants, children and adults== | |||
Many classification schemes for temperament have been developed, and there is no consensus.<ref>{{cite web|last1=Kagan|first1=Jerome|title=Temperament|url=http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/temperament/according-experts/temperament|website=Encyclopedia of Early Childhood Development|access-date=17 March 2018}}</ref><ref name=FET/> The Latin word ''temperamentum'' means 'mixture'. | |||
===Temperament vs personality=== | |||
] and ] began a classic ] in the 1950s regarding infant temperament. The study focused on how temperamental qualities influence adjustment throughout life. Chess and Thomas rated young infants on nine temperament characteristics, which by itself, or with connection to another affects how well a child fits in at school, their friends, and at home. Behaviors for each one of these traits are on a continuum. If a child leans towards the high or low end of the scale, while this is typical, it could be a cause for concern. The specific behaviors are: activity level, initial ] vs. ], adaptability, ], regularity of ]ing and ] patterns, intensity of ], distractibility, sensory sensitivity and ]. | |||
Some commentators see temperament as one factor underlying personality.<ref>{{Cite journal|url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467|jstor=10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467|doi=10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467|title=An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality|year=2010|last1=Irina Trofimova|journal=The American Journal of Psychology|volume=123|issue=4|pages=467–480|pmid=21291163}}</ref> | |||
== Main models == | |||
Going to separate out the traits in their own paragraph and add more detail. | |||
===Four temperaments model=== | |||
Thomas and Chess found that many babies could be categorized into one of three groups: ''easy'', ''difficult'', and ''slow-to-warm-up''. (Thomas & Chess 1977). Not all children can be placed in one of these groups. Approximately 65% of children fit one of the patterns. Of the 65%, 40% fit the easy pattern, 10% fell into the difficult pattern, and 15% were slow to warm up. Each category has its own strength and weakness and one is not superior to another. | |||
Historically, in the second century AD, the physician ] described ] (melancholic, phlegmatic, sanguine and choleric), corresponding to the ] or bodily fluids.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.britannica.com/topic/temperament|title = Temperament | personality}}</ref> This historical concept was explored by ]s, ]s, ]s and psycho-physiologists from very early times of psychological science, with theories proposed by ], ], ], ], ] among others. In more recent history, ] had emphasized the importance of the ] in elementary education, the time when he believed the influence of temperament on the personality to be at its strongest.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://wn.rsarchive.org/Lectures/FourTemps/ForTem_index.html|title=The Four Temperaments: Lecture in Berlin|last=Steiner|first=Rudolf |year=1909|access-date=2009-04-21}}</ref><ref>{{cite book|last=Steiner|first=Rudolf|title=The Four Temperaments|isbn=978-0-910142-11-3|year=1985}}</ref> Neither Galen nor Steiner are generally applied to the contemporary study of temperament in the approaches of modern medicine or contemporary psychology. | |||
===Rusalov-Trofimova neurophysiological model of temperament=== | |||
Thomas and Chess showed that ''Easy'' babies readily adapt to new experiences, generally display positive moods and emotions and also have normal eating and sleeping patterns. ''Difficult'' babies tend to be very emotional, irritable and fussy, and cry a lot. They also tend to have irregular eating and sleeping patterns. ''Slow-to-warm-up'' babies have a low activity level, and tend to withdraw from new situations and people. They are slow to adapt to new experiences, but then slowly accepts them after repeated exposure. | |||
This model based on the longest tradition of neurophysiological experiments started within the investigations of types and properties of nervous systems by ]'s school. This experimental tradition started on studies with animals in 1910–20s but expanded its methodology to humans since 1930s and especially since 1960s, including EEG, caffeine tests, evoked potentials, behavioral tasks and other psychophysiological methods.<ref name=Rusal2018>{{cite journal |last1=Rusalov |first1=Vladimir |title=Functional systems theory and the activity-specific approach in psychological taxonomies |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |date=26 February 2018 |volume=373 |issue=1744 |pages=20170166 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2017.0166|pmid=29483350 |pmc=5832690 }}</ref> | |||
Thomas and Chess found that these broad patterns of temperamental qualities are remarkably stable through childhood. These traits are also found in children across all cultures. | |||
The latest version of this model is based on the "], on ]'s research in clinical neurophysiology and on the neurochemical model ].<ref name=FET/><ref name=TroRob/><ref name=Tro2018/> At the present time the model is associated with the ] and has 12 scales: | |||
Thomas and Chess also studied temperament and environment. One sample consisted of white middle class families with high educational status and the other was of Puerto Rican working class families. They found several differences. Among those were: | |||
====Endurance-related scales==== | |||
Parents of middle class children were more likely to report behavior problems before the age of nine and the children had sleep problems. This may be because children start preschool between the ages of three and four. Puerto Rican children under the age of five showed rare signs of sleep problems, however, sleep problems became more common at the age of six. | |||
* Motor-physical Endurance: the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged physical activity using well-defined behavioral elements | |||
* Social-verbal Endurance (sociability): the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged social-verbal activities using well-defined behavioral elements. | |||
* Mental Endurance, or Attention: the ability to stay focused on selected features of objects with suppression of behavioral reactivity to other features. | |||
====Scales related to speed of integration of behavior==== | |||
Middle class parents also placed great stress on the child’s early development, believing that problems in early ages were indicative of later problems in psychological development, whereas Puerto Rican parents felt their children would outgrow any problems. | |||
* Motor-physical Tempo: speed of integration of an action in physical manipulations with objects with well-defined scripts of actions | |||
* Plasticity: the ability to adapt quickly to changes in situations, to change the program of action, and to shift between different tasks | |||
* Social-verbal Tempo: the preferred speed of speech and ability to understand fast speech on well-known topics, reading and sorting of known verbal material | |||
====Scales related to type of orientation of behavior==== | |||
At the age of nine, the report of new problems dropped for middle class children but they rose in Puerto Rican children, possibly due to the demands of school. | |||
* Sensation Seeking (SS): behavioral orientation to well-defined and existing sensational objects and events, underestimation of outcomes of risky behavior. | |||
* Empathy: behavioral orientation to the emotional states/needs of others (ranging from empathic deafness in autism and schizophrenia disorders to social dependency). | |||
* Sensitivity to Probabilities: the drive to gather information about uniqueness, frequency and values of objects/events, to differentiate their specific features, to project these features in future actions. | |||
====Emotionality scales==== | |||
==Influences of Temperament on Family Life== | |||
* Satisfaction (Self-Confidence): A disposition to be satisfied with current state of events, a sense of security, confidence in the future. In spite of the optimism about outcomes of his or her activities, the respondent might be negligent in details. | |||
* Impulsivity: Initiation of actions based on immediate emotional reactivity rather than by planning or rational reasoning. | |||
* Neuroticism: A tendency to avoid novelty, unpredictable situations and uncertainty. Preference of well-known settings and people over unknown ones and a need for approval and feedback from people around. | |||
===Kagan's research=== | |||
Most experts agree that temperament does have a ] and ] basis; but researchers also agree that ] experiences can modify a child's temperament. Differences of temperament or behavior styles among each individual are important in family life. They affect the interactions among family members. While some children can adapt quickly and easily to family routines and get along with siblings, others who are more active or intense may have a difficult time adjusting. It is the interactions between these children and their parents and/or siblings that can lead to stress and friction within the family life. | |||
] and his colleagues have concentrated empirical research on a temperamental category termed "reactivity." Four-month-old infants who became "motorically aroused and distressed" to presentations of novel stimuli were termed ''highly reactive''. Those who remained "motorically relaxed and did not cry or fret to the same set of unfamiliar events" were termed ''low reactive''.<ref name=Kagan2>{{Cite journal | doi = 10.2307/1131931 | last1 = Kagan | first1 = J. | year = 1997 | title = Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity| journal = Child Development | volume = 68 | issue = 1| pages = 139–143 | pmid = 9084130 | jstor = 1131931 | url = http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:37980692 }}</ref> These high and low reactive infants were tested again at 14 and 21 months "in a variety of unfamiliar laboratory situations." Highly reactive infants were predominantly characterized by a profile of high fear to unfamiliar events, which Kagan termed inhibited. Contrastingly, low reactive children were minimally fearful to novel situations, and were characterized by an uninhibited profile (Kagan). However, when observed again at age 4.5, only a modest proportion of children maintained their expected profile due to mediating factors such as intervening family experiences. Those who remained highly inhibited or uninhibited after age 4.5 were at higher risk for developing anxiety and conduct disorders, respectively.<ref name=Kagan/> | |||
Parents can also differ in temperament. The mix between parents and children also has an affect on family life. These affects can be positive, frustrating or even conflicting. Parents can refer to the “Goodness of Fit.” “Goodness of fit” refers to the match or mismatch between children and other family members. For example, a slow paced parent may be irritated by a highly active child or if both parent and child are highly active and intense it could mean big conflict. This can be useful to parents for figuring out how temperaments affect family relationships. What may appear to be a behavioral problem may actually be a mismatch between the parent’s temperament and their child’s. By taking a closer look at the nine traits that Thomas and Chess revealed from their study, parents can gain a better understanding of their child’s temperament and their own. | |||
Kagan also used two additional classifications, one for infants who were inactive but cried frequently (distressed) and one for those who showed vigorous activity but little crying (aroused). Followed to age 14–17 years, these groups of children showed differing outcomes, including some differences in central nervous system activity. Teenagers who had been classed as high reactives when they were babies were more likely to be "subdued in unfamiliar situations, to report a dour mood and anxiety over the future, to be more religious."<ref>{{cite journal |jstor=30163598|title=The Preservation of Two Infant Temperaments into Adolescence|journal=Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development|volume=72|issue=2|pages=i–95|last1=Kagan|first1=Jerome|last2=Snidman|first2=Nancy|last3=Kahn|first3=Vali|last4=Towsley|first4=Sara|last5=Steinberg|first5=Laurence|last6=Fox|first6=Nathan A|year=2007}}</ref> | |||
The website Temperament by Dr. Arthur Becker-Weidman provides a checklist scale for parents to use helping them to discover areas of match and mismatch between theirselves and the child. The checklist is from The Center for Family Development, 2001. Here is an example of the checklist: | |||
_______________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ | |||
On each scale below place a markto indicate your estimate of where your child, you, and your partner are on that scale. Child =*, you=X, and your partner=/. | |||
===Thomas and Chess's nine temperament characteristics=== | |||
mid-point | |||
'''ACTIVITY LEVEL''' ↓ | |||
High/Active___________________________________________Low/Inactive | |||
Alexander Thomas, ], Herbert G. Birch, Margaret Hertzig and Sam Korn began the classic New York Longitudinal study in the early 1950s regarding infant temperament (Thomas, Chess & Birch, 1968). The study focused on how temperamental qualities influence adjustment throughout life. Chess, Thomas et al. rated young infants on nine temperament characteristics, each of which, by itself, or with connection to another, affects how well a child fits in at school, with their friends, and at home. Behaviors for each one of these traits are on a ]. If a child leans towards the high or low end of the scale, it could be a cause for concern. The specific behaviors are: activity level, regularity of ]ing and ] patterns, initial reaction, adaptability, intensity of ], ], distractibility, persistence and ], and sensory sensitivity. Redundancies between the categories have been found and a reduced list is normally used by psychologists today.<ref name=IReview>{{cite journal |last1=Zentner |first1=Marcel |last2=Bates |first2=John E. |title=Child Temperament: An Integrative Review of Concepts, Research Programs, and Measures |journal=International Journal of Developmental Science |date=1 January 2008 |volume=2 |issue=1–2 |pages=7–37 |citeseerx=10.1.1.598.1517 |doi=10.3233/DEV-2008-21203 }}</ref> | |||
'''REGULARITY''' | |||
Predictable___________________________________________Unpredictable | |||
Research by Thomas and Chess used the following nine temperament traits in children<ref name=psychpage>{{cite web|url=http://www.psychpage.com/family/library/temperm.htm|title=PsychPage.com Child Temperament|access-date=2009-04-21}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.schwablearning.org/articles.asp?r=495 |title=Your Child's Temperament: Some Basics |last=Firchow |first=Nancy M.L.S. |access-date=2009-04-21 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20060517192806/http://www.schwablearning.org/articles.asp?r=495 |archive-date=May 17, 2006 }}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://ohioline.osu.edu/flm02/FS05.html|title=Ohio State University Extension: Understanding Your Child's Temperament|access-date=2009-04-21|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20090421053122/http://ohioline.osu.edu/flm02/FS05.html|archive-date=2009-04-21|url-status=dead}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.umext.maine.edu/onlinepubs/htmpubs/4358.htm|title=University of Maine Cooperative Extension: Family Issues Facts|access-date=2009-04-21|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20090107202309/http://www.umext.maine.edu/onlinepubs/htmpubs/4358.htm|archive-date=2009-01-07|url-status=dead}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.keepkidshealthy.com/development/temperament_personality.html|title=KeepKidsHealthy.com Temperament and Personality|access-date=2009-04-21|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20090207202359/http://www.keepkidshealthy.com/development/temperament_personality.html|archive-date=2009-02-07|url-status=dead}}</ref> based on a classification scheme developed by Dr. Herbert Birch:<ref>Stella Chess and Alexander Thomas, ''Temperament'', Routledge: 1996. {{ISBN|978-0-87630-835-6}}. p. 31</ref> Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn found that many babies could be categorized into one of three groups: ''easy'', ''difficult'', and ''slow-to-warm-up'' (Thomas & Chess 1977).<ref name=psychpage/> Not all children can be placed in one of these groups. Approximately 65% of children fit one of the patterns. Of the 65%, 40% fit the easy pattern, 10% fell into the difficult pattern, and 15% were slow to warm up. Each category has its own strength and weakness and one is not superior to another.<ref>{{cite web | url=http://www.rookieparenting.com/raising-children-temperament-personality/ | title=Raising Children – Temperament and Personality | publisher=Rookie Parenting | date=29 September 2014 | access-date=12 October 2014}}</ref> | |||
'''INITIAL REACTION''' | |||
Bold/Approaches___________________________________________Cautious | |||
Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn showed that ''easy'' babies readily adapt to new experiences, generally display positive moods and emotions and also have normal eating and sleeping patterns. ''Difficult'' babies tend to be very emotional, irritable and fussy, and cry a lot. They also tend to have irregular eating and sleeping patterns. ''Slow-to-warm-up'' babies have a low activity level, and tend to withdraw from new situations and people. They are slow to adapt to new experiences, but accept them after repeated exposure. | |||
'''ADAPTABILITY''' | |||
Quick___________________________________________Gradual | |||
Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn found that these broad patterns of temperamental qualities are remarkably stable through childhood. These traits are also found in children across all cultures. | |||
'''INTENSITY''' | |||
Mild___________________________________________Intense | |||
Thomas and Chess also studied temperament and environment. One sample consisted of white middle-class families with high educational status and the other was of Puerto Rican working-class families. They found several differences. Among those were:<ref name=psychpage/> | |||
'''MOOD''' | |||
Sunny/Cheerful___________________________________________Stormy | |||
* Parents of middle class children were more likely to report behavior problems before the age of nine and the children had sleep problems. This may be because children start preschool between the ages of three and four. Puerto Rican children under the age of five showed rare signs of sleep problems, however, sleep problems became more common at the age of six. | |||
'''DISTRACTIBILITY''' | |||
* Middle-class parents also placed great stress on the child's early development, believing that problems in early ages were indicative of later problems in psychological development, whereas Puerto Rican parents felt their children would outgrow any problems. | |||
Rarely___________________________________________Easily | |||
* At the age of nine, the report of new problems dropped for middle class children but they rose in Puerto Rican children, possibly due to the demands of school. | |||
Observed traits: | |||
'''PERSISTENCE & ATTENTION SPAN''' | |||
Persistent/Long___________________________________________Intermitent/Short | |||
* Activity: refers to the child's physical energy. Is the child constantly moving, or does the child have a relaxing approach? A high-energy child may have difficulty sitting still in class, whereas a child with low energy can tolerate a very structured environment. The former may use gross motor skills like running and jumping more frequently. Conversely, a child with a lower activity level may rely more on fine motor skills, such as drawing and putting puzzles together. This trait can also refer to mental activity, such as deep thinking or reading—activities which become more significant as the person matures. | |||
'''SENSITIVITY''' | |||
* Regularity: also known as ''rhythmicity'', refers to the level of predictability in a child's biological functions, such as waking, becoming tired, hunger, and bowel movements. Does the child have a routine in eating and sleeping habits, or are these events more random? For example, a child with a high regularity rating may want to eat at 2 p.m. every day, whereas a child lower on the regularity scale may eat at sporadic times throughout the day. | |||
Nonreactive___________________________________________Sensitive | |||
* Initial reaction: also known as ''approach or withdrawal''. This refers to how the child responds (whether positively or negatively) to new people or environments. Does the child approach people or things in the environment without hesitation, or does the child shy away? A bold child tends to approach things quickly, as if without thinking, whereas a cautious child typically prefers to watch for a while before engaging in new experiences. | |||
* Adaptability: refers to how long it takes the child to adjust to change over time (as opposed to an initial reaction). Does the child adjust to the changes in their environment easily, or is the child resistant? A child who adjusts easily may be quick to settle into a new routine, whereas a resistant child may take a long time to adjust to the situation. | |||
*Intensity: refers to the energy level of a positive or negative response. Does the child react intensely to a situation, or does the child respond in a calm and quiet manner? A more intense child may jump up and down screaming with excitement, whereas a mild-mannered child may smile or show no emotion. | |||
* Mood: refers to the child's general tendency towards a happy or unhappy demeanor. All children have a variety of emotions and reactions, such as cheerful and stormy, happy and unhappy. Yet each child biologically tends to have a generally positive or negative outlook. A baby who frequently smiles and coos could be considered a cheerful baby, whereas a baby who frequently cries or fusses might be considered a stormy baby. | |||
*Distractibility: refers to the child's tendency to be sidetracked by other things going on around them. Does the child get easily distracted by what is happening in the environment, or can the child concentrate despite the interruptions? An easily distracted child is engaged by external events and has difficulty returning to the task at hand, whereas a rarely distracted child stays focused and completes the task at hand. | |||
*Persistence and attention span: refer to the child's length of time on a task and ability to stay with the task through frustrations—whether the child stays with an activity for a long period of time or loses interest quickly. | |||
*Sensitivity: refers to how easily a child is disturbed by changes in the environment. This is also called '''sensory threshold''' or '''threshold of responsiveness'''. Is the child bothered by external stimuli like noises, textures, or lights, or does the child seem to ignore them? A sensitive child may lose focus when a door slams, whereas a child less sensitive to external noises will be able to maintain focus. | |||
===Mary K. Rothbart's three dimensions of temperament=== | |||
If you find more than two mismatches there is a potential for conflict and parent-child difficulties. | |||
] views temperament as the individual personality differences in infants and young children that are present prior to the development of higher ] and social aspects of ].<ref name="Rothbart and Hwang">{{cite book|author1=Rothbart, M.K |author2=Hwang, J. |name-list-style=amp |title=Temperament and the development of competence and motivation. In A.J. Elliot & A.C. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation|publisher=Guilford Press|location=New York| year = 2005 |isbn=978-1-59385-606-9|pages=167–184|url=http://www.guilford.com/cgi-bin/cartscript.cgi?page=pr/elliot3.htm&dir=pp/sapp&cart_id=471466.23602}}</ref> Rothbart further defines temperament as individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation that manifest in the domains of emotion, activity and attention. Moving away from classifying infants into categories, Mary Rothbart identified three underlying dimensions of temperament.<ref name="Kail and Barnfield">{{cite book |vauthors=Kail RV, Barnfield A | title = Children and Their Development, Second Canadian Edition with MyDevelopmentLab | publisher = Pearson Education Canada | location = Toronto | year = 2011 | isbn = 978-0-13-255770-2 }}</ref> Using ] on data from 3- to 12-month-old children, three broad factors emerged and were labeled surgency/extraversion, negative affect, and effortful control.<ref name="Rothbart and Hwang" /> | |||
2001 Center for Family Development. (Temperament by Dr. Arthur Becker-Weidman 2006). | |||
_______________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ | |||
Parents can encourage new behaviors in their children, and with enough support a ''slow-to-warm-up'' child can become less shy, or a ''difficult'' baby can become more emotionally stable. More recently infants and children with temperament issues have been called "spirited" to avoid negative connotations of "''difficult''" and "''slow to warm up''." Numerous books have been written advising parents how to raise their spirited youngsters. | |||
====Surgency/extraversion==== | |||
==Understanding Temperament to Improve Family Life== | |||
]/extraversion includes positive anticipation, ], increased levels of activity and a desire for sensation seeking.<ref name="Rothbart in Merrill">{{cite journal|last=Rothbart|first=M K|title=Temperament and the pursuit of an integrated developmental psychology|journal=Merrill-Palmer Quarterly|date=Oct 2004|volume=50|issue=4|pages=492–505|url=http://digitalcommons.wayne.edu/mpq/vol50/iss4/7|doi=10.1353/mpq.2004.0035|s2cid=144970654}}</ref> This factor reflects the degree to which a child is generally happy, active, and enjoys vocalizing and seeking stimulation.<ref name="Kail and Barnfield" /> Increased levels of smiling and laughter are observed in babies high in surgency/extraversion.<ref name="Rothbart and Hwang" /> 10- to 11-year-olds with higher levels of surgency/extraversion are more likely to develop externalizing problems like acting out; however, they are less likely to develop internalizing problems such as ] and low ].<ref name=Ormel>{{cite journal|pmid=16300695|doi=10.1017/S0033291705005829|year=2005|last1=Ormel|first1=J|title=Internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence: General and dimension-specific effects of familial loadings and preadolescent temperament traits|journal=Psychological Medicine|volume=35|issue=12|pages=1825–35|last2=Oldehinkel|first2=A. J|last3=Ferdinand|first3=R. F|last4=Hartman|first4=C. A|last5=De Winter|first5=A. F|last6=Veenstra|first6=R|last7=Vollebergh|first7=W|last8=Minderaa|first8=R. B|last9=Buitelaar|first9=J. K|last10=Verhulst|first10=F. C|s2cid=10109488}}</ref> | |||
====Negative affect==== | |||
Understanding a child’s temperament can help reframe how parents interpret children’s behavior and the way parents think about the reasons for behaviors. By parents having access to this knowledge now helps them to guide their child in ways that respect the child’s individual differences. By understanding children’s temperaments and our own helps adults to work with them rather than try to change them. It is an opportunity to anticipate and understand a child’s reaction. It is also important to know that temperament does not excuse a child’s unacceptable behavior, but it does provide direction to how parents can respond to it. Making small and reasonable accommodations to routines can reduce tension. For example a child who is slow paced in the mornings may need an extra half hour to get ready. Knowing who or what may affect the child’s behavior can help to alleviate potential problems. | |||
Negative affect includes ], ], ], discomfort,<ref name="Rothbart in Merrill" /> and ].<ref name="Rothbart and Hwang" /> This factor reflects the degree to which a child is shy and not easily calmed.<ref name="Kail and Barnfield" /> Anger and frustration is seen as early as 2 to 3 months of age. Anger and frustration, together, predict externalizing and internalizing difficulties. Anger, alone, is later related to externalizing problems, while fear is associated with internalizing difficulties. Fear as evidenced by behavioral inhibition is seen as early as 7–10 months of age, and later predicts children's fearfulness and lower levels of ].<ref name="Rothbart, 2007">{{cite journal|last=Rothbart|first=M. K.|title=Temperament, development and personality|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|year=2007|volume=16|issue=4|pages=207–212|doi=10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00505.x|s2cid=15994633|url=http://brainstormquestions.wikispaces.com/file/view/Rothbart+-+Temperament,+Development+and+Personality.pdf}}</ref> | |||
====Effortful control==== | |||
Recognizing the child’s temperament and helping them to understand how it impacts his/her life as well as others is important. It is just as important for parents to recognize their own temperaments. Recognizing each individual’s temperament, will help to prevent and manage problems that may arise from the differences among family members. | |||
Effortful control includes the focusing and shifting of ], inhibitory control, perceptual sensitivity, and a low threshold for pleasure.<ref name="Rothbart in Merrill" /> This factor reflects the degree to which a child can focus attention, is not easily distracted,<ref name="Kail and Barnfield" /> can restrain a dominant response in order to execute a non-dominant response, and employ planning. When high in effortful control, six- to seven-year-olds tend to be more ] and lower in aggressiveness.<ref name="Rothbart in Merrill" /> Higher levels of effortful control at age seven also predict lower externalizing problems at age 11 years.<ref name=Valiente>{{cite journal|pmid=14633062|doi=10.1111/1467-6494.7106011|year=2003|last1=Valiente|first1=C|title=The relations of effortful control and reactive control to children's externalizing problems: A longitudinal assessment|journal=Journal of Personality|volume=71|issue=6|pages=1171–96|last2=Eisenberg|first2=N|last3=Smith|first3=C. L|last4=Reiser|first4=M|last5=Fabes|first5=R. A|last6=Losoya|first6=S|last7=Guthrie|first7=I. K|last8=Murphy|first8=B. C}}</ref> Children high on negative affect show decreased internalizing and externalizing problems when they are also high on effortful control.<ref name="Rothbart, 2007" /> Rothbart suggests that effortful control is dependent on the development of executive attention skills in the early years. In turn, executive attention skills allows greater self-control over reactive tendencies. Effortful control shows stability from infancy into the school years<ref name="Rothbart, 2007" /> and also predicts ].<ref name="Kochanska et al.">{{cite journal|pmid=10749079|doi=10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220|year=2000|last1=Kochanska|first1=G|title=Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development|journal=Developmental Psychology|volume=36|issue=2|pages=220–32|last2=Murray|first2=K. T|last3=Harlan|first3=E. T}}</ref> | |||
===Others=== | |||
Temperament continues into adulthood, and later studies by Chess and Thomas have shown that these characteristics continue to influence behavior and adjustment throughout the life-span. | |||
Solomon Diamond described temperaments based upon characteristics found in the animal world: fearfulness, aggressiveness, affiliativeness, and impulsiveness. His work has been carried forward by ] and ], who developed two measures of temperament: The Colorado Child Temperament Inventory, which includes aspects of Thomas and Chess's schema, and the ].<ref name=IReview/> | |||
We are still looking for information about the later study done and will add more here if we can find some. | |||
H. Hill Goldsmith and Joseph Campos used emotional characteristics to define temperament, originally analyzing five emotional qualities: motor activity, anger, fearfulness, pleasure/joy, and interest/persistence, but later expanding to include other emotions. They developed several measures of temperament: Lab-TAB and TBAQ.<ref name=IReview/> | |||
In addition to the initial clinical studies, academic psychologists have developed an interest in the field and researchers such as Bates, Buss & Plomin, ], and Rothbart have generated large bodies of research in the areas of ], ], and ]. | |||
Other temperament systems include those based upon theories of adult temperament (e.g. Gray and Martin's Temperament Assessment Battery for Children), or adult personality (e.g.the Big Five personality traits). | |||
==Artistic Temperament== | |||
The tendency for those who are highly artistic to show dramatic swings in emotion. While not solely possessed by artists, it is highly prevalent among artists of all mediums, including painters, sculptors, musicians, writers, etc. This behavior is often characterized by being highly passionate about subjects of importance to the possessor of this behavior, extremely dedicated to certain goals, often hyper-aware of the presence of others, and at other times seemingly oblivious to the presence of others (which explains the odd, extreme movements some artists may have while performing their art.) It is also accompanied by the full range of all the emotions, often elevated to extremes. There is a theory that this due to the high instance of bipolar disorder in the artistic community, however this is a subject of debate. Also, in some individuals its origin could be due to a self-fulfilling expectation that, "Since I am an artist, I should have an artistic temperament." | |||
== |
==Causal and correlating factors== | ||
===Biological basis for temperament=== | |||
{{see also|Four humours}} | |||
Scientists seeking evidence of a biological basis of personality have examined the relationship between temperament and ]s<ref>Depue, R. & Fu, Y. (2012) Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults. In: Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. (Eds.) Handbook of Temperament. NY: Guilford Publications, 368-399. (2012).</ref><ref name=FET>{{cite book|year=2016|last1=Trofimova|first1=IN|chapter=The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament |editor=Arnold, M.C. |title=Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life |location=New York |publisher=]|pages=77–147}}</ref><ref name=TroRob>{{cite journal|year=2016| last1=Trofimova|first1=IN|last2=Robbins|first2=TW|title=Temperament and arousal systems: a new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry|journal=Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews|volume=64|pages=382–402| doi=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.008| pmid=26969100| hdl=11375/26202| s2cid=13937324| url=https://www.repository.cam.ac.uk/handle/1810/274784|hdl-access=free}}</ref><ref name=Tro2018>{{cite journal|year=2018|last1=Trofimova|first1=IN|title=Functionality vs dimensionality in psychological taxonomies, and a puzzle of emotional valence |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences | volume=373|issue=1744|pages=20170167|doi=10.1098/rstb.2017.0167|pmid=29483351|pmc=5832691}}</ref> and character (defined in this context as developmental aspects of personality). Temperament is hypothesized to be associated with biological factors, but these have proven to be complex and diverse,<ref name=Kagan/><ref name=FET/> and biological correlations have proven hard to confirm.<ref name=Kagan/> | |||
In his lectures on education, ] brought a new approach to the ]: melancholic, phlegmatic, sanguine and choleric. He emphasized their importance in elementary education, as this is a time when the child is strongly affected by his or her nature in this respect. A person's temperament may change, especially in the pre-puberty years, and in any case diminishes in importance as the personality becomes more developed after puberty. | |||
===Temperament vs. psychiatric disorders === | |||
In any case, the temperament is not exclusive; most people combine aspects of all of them. One or two may dominate, however, or be prominent by their absence. In addition, for each temperament Steiner pointed out that there are less and more mature forms: the sullen, self-absorbed melancholic can mature to the sympathetic helper and/or the deep thinker. A person may transform his or her own temperament, as well, either by becoming more mature in what is naturally given or by metamorphosing into a different temperament. | |||
Several psychiatrists and differential psychologists have suggested that temperament and mental illness represent varying degrees along the same continuum of neurotransmitter imbalances in neurophysiological systems of behavioral regulation.<ref>{{cite journal|doi=10.1098/rstb.2017.0168|pmid=29483352|pmc=5832692|title=Assessing the continuum between temperament and affective illness: Psychiatric and mathematical perspectives|journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences|volume=373|issue=1744|pages=20170168|year=2018|last1=Sulis|first1=W.}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|pmc=4370336|year=2014|last1=Watson|first1=D|title=Personality, Emotions, and the Emotional Disorders|journal=Clinical Psychological Science|volume=2|issue=4|pages=422–442|last2=Naragon-Gainey|first2=K|doi=10.1177/2167702614536162|pmid=25815243}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|doi=10.1186/s12888-018-1695-x|pmid=29747614|pmc=5946468|title=There is more to mental illness than negative affect: Comprehensive temperament profiles in depression and generalized anxiety|journal=BMC Psychiatry|volume=18|issue=1|pages=125|year=2018|last1=Trofimova|first1=I.|last2=Sulis|first2=W |doi-access=free }}</ref> | |||
In fact, the original four types of temperament (choleric, melancholic, phlegmatic and sanguine) suggested by ] and ] resemble mild forms of types of psychiatric disorders described in modern classifications. Moreover, Hippocrates-Galen hypothesis of chemical imbalances as factors of consistent individual differences has also been validated by research in neurochemistry and psychopharmacology, though modern studies attribute this to different compounds. Many studies have examined the relationships between temperament traits (such as impulsivity, sensation seeking, neuroticism, endurance, plasticity, sociability or extraversion) and various neurotransmitter and hormonal systems (i.e., the very same systems implicated in mental disorders). | |||
Even though temperament and psychiatric disorders can be presented as, correspondingly, weak and strong imbalances within the same regulatory systems, it is incorrect to say that temperament is a weak degree of these disorders. Temperament might be a disposition to develop a mental disorder, but it should not be treated as a guaranteed marker for disorders. | |||
===Family life=== | |||
====Influences==== | |||
Most experts agree that temperament has a ] and ] basis, although environmental factors and maturation modify the ways a child's personality is expressed.<ref>. Philoctetes Center, New York; with panelists Sue Carter, Francis Champagne, ], Ed Nercessian, Donald Pfaff, ], Nadia Bruschweiler Stern</ref> The term "goodness of fit" refers to the match or mismatch between temperament and other personal characteristics and the specific features of the environment. Differences of temperament or behavior styles between individuals are important in family life. They affect the interactions among family members. While some children can adapt quickly and easily to family routines and get along with siblings, others who are more active or intense may have a difficult time adjusting. The interactions between these children and their parents or siblings are among a number of factors that can lead to stress and friction within the family. | |||
The temperament mix between parents and children also affects family life. For example, a slow-paced parent may be irritated by a highly active child; or if both parent and child are highly active and intense, conflict could result. This knowledge can help parents figure out how temperaments affect family relationships. What may appear to be a behavioral problem may actually be a mismatch between the parent's temperament and their child's. By taking a closer look at the nine traits that Thomas and Chess revealed from their study, parents can gain a better understanding of their child's temperament and their own. Parents may also notice that situational factors cause a child's temperament to seem problematic; for example, a child with low rhythmicity can cause difficulties for a family with a highly scheduled life, and a child with a high activity level may be difficult to cope with if the family lives in a crowded apartment upstairs from sensitive neighbors. | |||
Parents can encourage new behaviors in their children, and with enough support a ''slow-to-warm-up'' child can become less shy, or a ''difficult'' baby can become easier to handle. More recently infants and children with temperament issues have been called "spirited" to avoid negative connotations of "''difficult''" and "''slow to warm up''". Numerous books have been written advising parents how to raise their spirited youngsters. | |||
====Understanding for improvement==== | |||
Understanding a child's temperament can help reframe how parents interpret children's behavior and the way parents think about the reasons for behaviors. By parents having access to this knowledge now helps them to guide their child in ways that respect the child's individual differences. By understanding children's temperaments and our own helps adults to work with them rather than try to change them. It is an opportunity to anticipate and understand a child's reaction. It is also important to know that temperament does not excuse a child's unacceptable behavior, but it does provide direction to how parents can respond to it. Making small and reasonable accommodations to routines can reduce tension. For example, a child who is slow-paced in the mornings may need an extra half-hour to get ready. Knowing who or what may affect the child's behavior can help to alleviate potential problems. Although children obtain their temperament behaviors innately, a large part that helps determine a child's ability to develop and act in certain ways is determined by the parents. When a parent takes the time to identify and more importantly respond to the temperaments they are faced with in a positive way it will help them guide their child in trying to figure out the world. | |||
Recognizing the child's temperament and helping them to understand how it impacts his/her life as well as others is important. It is just as important for parents to recognize their own temperaments. Recognizing each individual's temperament, will help to prevent and manage problems that may arise from the differences among family members. | |||
Temperament continues into adulthood, and later studies by Chess and Thomas have shown that these characteristics continue to influence behavior and adjustment throughout the life-span. | |||
In addition to the initial clinical studies, academic psychologists have developed an interest in the field and researchers such as Bates, Buss & Plomin, ], ], ], Trofimova and Rothbart have generated large bodies of research in the areas of ], ], and ]. | |||
==Determination of temperament type== | |||
Temperament is determined through specific behavioral profiles, usually focusing on those that are both easily measurable and testable early in childhood. Commonly tested factors include traits related to energetic capacities (named as "Activity", "Endurance", "Extraversion"), traits related to emotionality (such as irritability, frequency of smiling), and approach or avoidance of unfamiliar events.<ref name=Kagan>{{citation|author=Jerome Kagan|title=Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development|year=2005}}</ref><ref name=FET/> There is generally a low correlation between descriptions by teachers and behavioral observations by scientists of features used in determining temperament.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bishop |first1=Gillian |last2=Spence |first2=Susan H. |last3=Mcdonald |first3=Casey |title=Can Parents and Teachers Provide a Reliable and Valid Report of Behavioral Inhibition? |journal=Child Development |date=November 2003 |volume=74 |issue=6 |pages=1899–1917 |doi=10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00645.x |pmid=14669903 |jstor=3696311 |s2cid=16653275 }}</ref> | |||
==See also== | ==See also== | ||
*] | *] | ||
*] | |||
*] | |||
*] | |||
*] | |||
*] | |||
*] | |||
==References== | ==References== | ||
{{Reflist|2}} | |||
==Additional References== | |||
*Anschütz, Marieke, ''Children and Their Temperaments''. ISBN 0-86315-175-2. | |||
* |
*Anschütz, Marieke, ''Children and Their Temperaments''. {{ISBN|0-86315-175-2}}. | ||
*Carey, William B., ''Understanding Your Child's Temperament''. {{ISBN|1-4134-7028-9}}. | |||
*Steiner, Rudolf, ''The Four Temperaments''. ISBN 910142114 {{Please check ISBN|910142114}} | |||
*Diamond, S. (1957). ''Personality and temperament'' New York: Harper | |||
*Kagan J. ''Galen's prophecy: temperament in human nature''. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1994. | |||
*Kagan J, Snidman NC. ''The long shadow of temperament''. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press; 2004. | |||
*Kohnstamm GA, Bates JE, Rothbart MK, eds. ''Temperament in childhood'' Oxford, United Kingdom: John Wiley and Sons; 1989:59-73. | |||
*Neville, Helen F., and Diane Clark Johnson, "Temperament Tools: Working with Your Child's Inborn Traits". {{ISBN|1-884734-34-0}}. | |||
*Shick, Lyndall,"Understanding Temperament: Strategies for Creating Family Harmony". {{ISBN|1-884734-32-4}}. | |||
*{{Cite journal | doi = 10.2307/1131512 | last1 = Seifer | first1 = RA | last2 = Sameroff | first2 = AJ | last3 = Barrette | first3 = LC | last4 = Krafchuk | first4 = E. | year = 1994 | title = Infant temperament measured by multiple observations and mother report| journal = Child Development | volume = 65 | issue = 5| pages = 1478–1490 | pmid = 7982363 | jstor = 1131512 }} | |||
*Thomas, Chess & Birch (1968). Temperament and Behavior Disorders in Children. New York, New York University Press | |||
==External links== | |||
] | |||
{{Commons category|Temperaments}} | |||
* {{cite journal |last1=Cloninger |first1=C. Robert |last2=Cloninger |first2=Kevin M. |last3=Zwir |first3=Igor |last4=Keltikangas-Järvinen |first4=Liisa |title=The complex genetics and biology of human temperament: a review of traditional concepts in relation to new molecular findings |journal=Translational Psychiatry |date=11 November 2019 |volume=9 |issue=1 |page=290 |doi=10.1038/s41398-019-0621-4 |pmid=31712636 |pmc=6848211 }} | |||
*Henig, Robin Marantz. . ''New York Times Magazine'', September 29, 2009. Retrieved October 3, 2009. | |||
{{Authority control}} | |||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] |
Latest revision as of 23:22, 8 February 2024
Term used to indicate the mixture of the innate aspects of the personality This article is about psychological temperament. For musical temperament, see Musical temperament.
In psychology, temperament broadly refers to consistent individual differences in behavior that are biologically based and are relatively independent of learning, system of values and attitudes.
Some researchers point to association of temperament with formal dynamical features of behavior, such as energetic aspects, plasticity, sensitivity to specific reinforcers and emotionality. Temperament traits (such as neuroticism, sociability, impulsivity, etc.) are distinct patterns in behavior throughout a lifetime, but they are most noticeable and most studied in children. Babies are typically described by temperament, but longitudinal research in the 1920s began to establish temperament as something which is stable across the lifespan.
Definition
Temperament has been defined as "the constellation of inborn traits that determine a child's unique behavioral style and the way he or she experiences and reacts to the world."
Classification schemes
Many classification schemes for temperament have been developed, and there is no consensus. The Latin word temperamentum means 'mixture'.
Temperament vs personality
Some commentators see temperament as one factor underlying personality.
Main models
Four temperaments model
Historically, in the second century AD, the physician Galen described four classical temperaments (melancholic, phlegmatic, sanguine and choleric), corresponding to the four humors or bodily fluids. This historical concept was explored by philosophers, psychologists, psychiatrists and psycho-physiologists from very early times of psychological science, with theories proposed by Immanuel Kant, Hermann Lotze, Ivan Pavlov, Carl Jung, Gerardus Heymans among others. In more recent history, Rudolf Steiner had emphasized the importance of the four classical temperaments in elementary education, the time when he believed the influence of temperament on the personality to be at its strongest. Neither Galen nor Steiner are generally applied to the contemporary study of temperament in the approaches of modern medicine or contemporary psychology.
Rusalov-Trofimova neurophysiological model of temperament
This model based on the longest tradition of neurophysiological experiments started within the investigations of types and properties of nervous systems by Ivan Pavlov's school. This experimental tradition started on studies with animals in 1910–20s but expanded its methodology to humans since 1930s and especially since 1960s, including EEG, caffeine tests, evoked potentials, behavioral tasks and other psychophysiological methods.
The latest version of this model is based on the "Activity-specific approach in temperament research, on Alexander Luria's research in clinical neurophysiology and on the neurochemical model Functional Ensemble of Temperament. At the present time the model is associated with the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire and has 12 scales:
Endurance-related scales
- Motor-physical Endurance: the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged physical activity using well-defined behavioral elements
- Social-verbal Endurance (sociability): the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged social-verbal activities using well-defined behavioral elements.
- Mental Endurance, or Attention: the ability to stay focused on selected features of objects with suppression of behavioral reactivity to other features.
Scales related to speed of integration of behavior
- Motor-physical Tempo: speed of integration of an action in physical manipulations with objects with well-defined scripts of actions
- Plasticity: the ability to adapt quickly to changes in situations, to change the program of action, and to shift between different tasks
- Social-verbal Tempo: the preferred speed of speech and ability to understand fast speech on well-known topics, reading and sorting of known verbal material
Scales related to type of orientation of behavior
- Sensation Seeking (SS): behavioral orientation to well-defined and existing sensational objects and events, underestimation of outcomes of risky behavior.
- Empathy: behavioral orientation to the emotional states/needs of others (ranging from empathic deafness in autism and schizophrenia disorders to social dependency).
- Sensitivity to Probabilities: the drive to gather information about uniqueness, frequency and values of objects/events, to differentiate their specific features, to project these features in future actions.
Emotionality scales
- Satisfaction (Self-Confidence): A disposition to be satisfied with current state of events, a sense of security, confidence in the future. In spite of the optimism about outcomes of his or her activities, the respondent might be negligent in details.
- Impulsivity: Initiation of actions based on immediate emotional reactivity rather than by planning or rational reasoning.
- Neuroticism: A tendency to avoid novelty, unpredictable situations and uncertainty. Preference of well-known settings and people over unknown ones and a need for approval and feedback from people around.
Kagan's research
Jerome Kagan and his colleagues have concentrated empirical research on a temperamental category termed "reactivity." Four-month-old infants who became "motorically aroused and distressed" to presentations of novel stimuli were termed highly reactive. Those who remained "motorically relaxed and did not cry or fret to the same set of unfamiliar events" were termed low reactive. These high and low reactive infants were tested again at 14 and 21 months "in a variety of unfamiliar laboratory situations." Highly reactive infants were predominantly characterized by a profile of high fear to unfamiliar events, which Kagan termed inhibited. Contrastingly, low reactive children were minimally fearful to novel situations, and were characterized by an uninhibited profile (Kagan). However, when observed again at age 4.5, only a modest proportion of children maintained their expected profile due to mediating factors such as intervening family experiences. Those who remained highly inhibited or uninhibited after age 4.5 were at higher risk for developing anxiety and conduct disorders, respectively.
Kagan also used two additional classifications, one for infants who were inactive but cried frequently (distressed) and one for those who showed vigorous activity but little crying (aroused). Followed to age 14–17 years, these groups of children showed differing outcomes, including some differences in central nervous system activity. Teenagers who had been classed as high reactives when they were babies were more likely to be "subdued in unfamiliar situations, to report a dour mood and anxiety over the future, to be more religious."
Thomas and Chess's nine temperament characteristics
Alexander Thomas, Stella Chess, Herbert G. Birch, Margaret Hertzig and Sam Korn began the classic New York Longitudinal study in the early 1950s regarding infant temperament (Thomas, Chess & Birch, 1968). The study focused on how temperamental qualities influence adjustment throughout life. Chess, Thomas et al. rated young infants on nine temperament characteristics, each of which, by itself, or with connection to another, affects how well a child fits in at school, with their friends, and at home. Behaviors for each one of these traits are on a continuum. If a child leans towards the high or low end of the scale, it could be a cause for concern. The specific behaviors are: activity level, regularity of sleeping and eating patterns, initial reaction, adaptability, intensity of emotion, mood, distractibility, persistence and attention span, and sensory sensitivity. Redundancies between the categories have been found and a reduced list is normally used by psychologists today.
Research by Thomas and Chess used the following nine temperament traits in children based on a classification scheme developed by Dr. Herbert Birch: Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn found that many babies could be categorized into one of three groups: easy, difficult, and slow-to-warm-up (Thomas & Chess 1977). Not all children can be placed in one of these groups. Approximately 65% of children fit one of the patterns. Of the 65%, 40% fit the easy pattern, 10% fell into the difficult pattern, and 15% were slow to warm up. Each category has its own strength and weakness and one is not superior to another.
Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn showed that easy babies readily adapt to new experiences, generally display positive moods and emotions and also have normal eating and sleeping patterns. Difficult babies tend to be very emotional, irritable and fussy, and cry a lot. They also tend to have irregular eating and sleeping patterns. Slow-to-warm-up babies have a low activity level, and tend to withdraw from new situations and people. They are slow to adapt to new experiences, but accept them after repeated exposure.
Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig and Korn found that these broad patterns of temperamental qualities are remarkably stable through childhood. These traits are also found in children across all cultures.
Thomas and Chess also studied temperament and environment. One sample consisted of white middle-class families with high educational status and the other was of Puerto Rican working-class families. They found several differences. Among those were:
- Parents of middle class children were more likely to report behavior problems before the age of nine and the children had sleep problems. This may be because children start preschool between the ages of three and four. Puerto Rican children under the age of five showed rare signs of sleep problems, however, sleep problems became more common at the age of six.
- Middle-class parents also placed great stress on the child's early development, believing that problems in early ages were indicative of later problems in psychological development, whereas Puerto Rican parents felt their children would outgrow any problems.
- At the age of nine, the report of new problems dropped for middle class children but they rose in Puerto Rican children, possibly due to the demands of school.
Observed traits:
- Activity: refers to the child's physical energy. Is the child constantly moving, or does the child have a relaxing approach? A high-energy child may have difficulty sitting still in class, whereas a child with low energy can tolerate a very structured environment. The former may use gross motor skills like running and jumping more frequently. Conversely, a child with a lower activity level may rely more on fine motor skills, such as drawing and putting puzzles together. This trait can also refer to mental activity, such as deep thinking or reading—activities which become more significant as the person matures.
- Regularity: also known as rhythmicity, refers to the level of predictability in a child's biological functions, such as waking, becoming tired, hunger, and bowel movements. Does the child have a routine in eating and sleeping habits, or are these events more random? For example, a child with a high regularity rating may want to eat at 2 p.m. every day, whereas a child lower on the regularity scale may eat at sporadic times throughout the day.
- Initial reaction: also known as approach or withdrawal. This refers to how the child responds (whether positively or negatively) to new people or environments. Does the child approach people or things in the environment without hesitation, or does the child shy away? A bold child tends to approach things quickly, as if without thinking, whereas a cautious child typically prefers to watch for a while before engaging in new experiences.
- Adaptability: refers to how long it takes the child to adjust to change over time (as opposed to an initial reaction). Does the child adjust to the changes in their environment easily, or is the child resistant? A child who adjusts easily may be quick to settle into a new routine, whereas a resistant child may take a long time to adjust to the situation.
- Intensity: refers to the energy level of a positive or negative response. Does the child react intensely to a situation, or does the child respond in a calm and quiet manner? A more intense child may jump up and down screaming with excitement, whereas a mild-mannered child may smile or show no emotion.
- Mood: refers to the child's general tendency towards a happy or unhappy demeanor. All children have a variety of emotions and reactions, such as cheerful and stormy, happy and unhappy. Yet each child biologically tends to have a generally positive or negative outlook. A baby who frequently smiles and coos could be considered a cheerful baby, whereas a baby who frequently cries or fusses might be considered a stormy baby.
- Distractibility: refers to the child's tendency to be sidetracked by other things going on around them. Does the child get easily distracted by what is happening in the environment, or can the child concentrate despite the interruptions? An easily distracted child is engaged by external events and has difficulty returning to the task at hand, whereas a rarely distracted child stays focused and completes the task at hand.
- Persistence and attention span: refer to the child's length of time on a task and ability to stay with the task through frustrations—whether the child stays with an activity for a long period of time or loses interest quickly.
- Sensitivity: refers to how easily a child is disturbed by changes in the environment. This is also called sensory threshold or threshold of responsiveness. Is the child bothered by external stimuli like noises, textures, or lights, or does the child seem to ignore them? A sensitive child may lose focus when a door slams, whereas a child less sensitive to external noises will be able to maintain focus.
Mary K. Rothbart's three dimensions of temperament
Mary K. Rothbart views temperament as the individual personality differences in infants and young children that are present prior to the development of higher cognitive and social aspects of personality. Rothbart further defines temperament as individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation that manifest in the domains of emotion, activity and attention. Moving away from classifying infants into categories, Mary Rothbart identified three underlying dimensions of temperament. Using factor analysis on data from 3- to 12-month-old children, three broad factors emerged and were labeled surgency/extraversion, negative affect, and effortful control.
Surgency/extraversion
Surgency/extraversion includes positive anticipation, impulsivity, increased levels of activity and a desire for sensation seeking. This factor reflects the degree to which a child is generally happy, active, and enjoys vocalizing and seeking stimulation. Increased levels of smiling and laughter are observed in babies high in surgency/extraversion. 10- to 11-year-olds with higher levels of surgency/extraversion are more likely to develop externalizing problems like acting out; however, they are less likely to develop internalizing problems such as shyness and low self-esteem.
Negative affect
Negative affect includes fear, frustration, sadness, discomfort, and anger. This factor reflects the degree to which a child is shy and not easily calmed. Anger and frustration is seen as early as 2 to 3 months of age. Anger and frustration, together, predict externalizing and internalizing difficulties. Anger, alone, is later related to externalizing problems, while fear is associated with internalizing difficulties. Fear as evidenced by behavioral inhibition is seen as early as 7–10 months of age, and later predicts children's fearfulness and lower levels of aggression.
Effortful control
Effortful control includes the focusing and shifting of attention, inhibitory control, perceptual sensitivity, and a low threshold for pleasure. This factor reflects the degree to which a child can focus attention, is not easily distracted, can restrain a dominant response in order to execute a non-dominant response, and employ planning. When high in effortful control, six- to seven-year-olds tend to be more empathetic and lower in aggressiveness. Higher levels of effortful control at age seven also predict lower externalizing problems at age 11 years. Children high on negative affect show decreased internalizing and externalizing problems when they are also high on effortful control. Rothbart suggests that effortful control is dependent on the development of executive attention skills in the early years. In turn, executive attention skills allows greater self-control over reactive tendencies. Effortful control shows stability from infancy into the school years and also predicts conscience.
Others
Solomon Diamond described temperaments based upon characteristics found in the animal world: fearfulness, aggressiveness, affiliativeness, and impulsiveness. His work has been carried forward by Arnold Buss and Robert Plomin, who developed two measures of temperament: The Colorado Child Temperament Inventory, which includes aspects of Thomas and Chess's schema, and the EAS Survey for Children.
H. Hill Goldsmith and Joseph Campos used emotional characteristics to define temperament, originally analyzing five emotional qualities: motor activity, anger, fearfulness, pleasure/joy, and interest/persistence, but later expanding to include other emotions. They developed several measures of temperament: Lab-TAB and TBAQ.
Other temperament systems include those based upon theories of adult temperament (e.g. Gray and Martin's Temperament Assessment Battery for Children), or adult personality (e.g.the Big Five personality traits).
Causal and correlating factors
Biological basis for temperament
Scientists seeking evidence of a biological basis of personality have examined the relationship between temperament and neurotransmitter systems and character (defined in this context as developmental aspects of personality). Temperament is hypothesized to be associated with biological factors, but these have proven to be complex and diverse, and biological correlations have proven hard to confirm.
Temperament vs. psychiatric disorders
Several psychiatrists and differential psychologists have suggested that temperament and mental illness represent varying degrees along the same continuum of neurotransmitter imbalances in neurophysiological systems of behavioral regulation.
In fact, the original four types of temperament (choleric, melancholic, phlegmatic and sanguine) suggested by Hippocrates and Galen resemble mild forms of types of psychiatric disorders described in modern classifications. Moreover, Hippocrates-Galen hypothesis of chemical imbalances as factors of consistent individual differences has also been validated by research in neurochemistry and psychopharmacology, though modern studies attribute this to different compounds. Many studies have examined the relationships between temperament traits (such as impulsivity, sensation seeking, neuroticism, endurance, plasticity, sociability or extraversion) and various neurotransmitter and hormonal systems (i.e., the very same systems implicated in mental disorders).
Even though temperament and psychiatric disorders can be presented as, correspondingly, weak and strong imbalances within the same regulatory systems, it is incorrect to say that temperament is a weak degree of these disorders. Temperament might be a disposition to develop a mental disorder, but it should not be treated as a guaranteed marker for disorders.
Family life
Influences
Most experts agree that temperament has a genetic and biological basis, although environmental factors and maturation modify the ways a child's personality is expressed. The term "goodness of fit" refers to the match or mismatch between temperament and other personal characteristics and the specific features of the environment. Differences of temperament or behavior styles between individuals are important in family life. They affect the interactions among family members. While some children can adapt quickly and easily to family routines and get along with siblings, others who are more active or intense may have a difficult time adjusting. The interactions between these children and their parents or siblings are among a number of factors that can lead to stress and friction within the family.
The temperament mix between parents and children also affects family life. For example, a slow-paced parent may be irritated by a highly active child; or if both parent and child are highly active and intense, conflict could result. This knowledge can help parents figure out how temperaments affect family relationships. What may appear to be a behavioral problem may actually be a mismatch between the parent's temperament and their child's. By taking a closer look at the nine traits that Thomas and Chess revealed from their study, parents can gain a better understanding of their child's temperament and their own. Parents may also notice that situational factors cause a child's temperament to seem problematic; for example, a child with low rhythmicity can cause difficulties for a family with a highly scheduled life, and a child with a high activity level may be difficult to cope with if the family lives in a crowded apartment upstairs from sensitive neighbors.
Parents can encourage new behaviors in their children, and with enough support a slow-to-warm-up child can become less shy, or a difficult baby can become easier to handle. More recently infants and children with temperament issues have been called "spirited" to avoid negative connotations of "difficult" and "slow to warm up". Numerous books have been written advising parents how to raise their spirited youngsters.
Understanding for improvement
Understanding a child's temperament can help reframe how parents interpret children's behavior and the way parents think about the reasons for behaviors. By parents having access to this knowledge now helps them to guide their child in ways that respect the child's individual differences. By understanding children's temperaments and our own helps adults to work with them rather than try to change them. It is an opportunity to anticipate and understand a child's reaction. It is also important to know that temperament does not excuse a child's unacceptable behavior, but it does provide direction to how parents can respond to it. Making small and reasonable accommodations to routines can reduce tension. For example, a child who is slow-paced in the mornings may need an extra half-hour to get ready. Knowing who or what may affect the child's behavior can help to alleviate potential problems. Although children obtain their temperament behaviors innately, a large part that helps determine a child's ability to develop and act in certain ways is determined by the parents. When a parent takes the time to identify and more importantly respond to the temperaments they are faced with in a positive way it will help them guide their child in trying to figure out the world.
Recognizing the child's temperament and helping them to understand how it impacts his/her life as well as others is important. It is just as important for parents to recognize their own temperaments. Recognizing each individual's temperament, will help to prevent and manage problems that may arise from the differences among family members.
Temperament continues into adulthood, and later studies by Chess and Thomas have shown that these characteristics continue to influence behavior and adjustment throughout the life-span.
In addition to the initial clinical studies, academic psychologists have developed an interest in the field and researchers such as Bates, Buss & Plomin, Kagan, Rusalov, Cloninger, Trofimova and Rothbart have generated large bodies of research in the areas of personality, neuroscience, and behavioral genetics.
Determination of temperament type
Temperament is determined through specific behavioral profiles, usually focusing on those that are both easily measurable and testable early in childhood. Commonly tested factors include traits related to energetic capacities (named as "Activity", "Endurance", "Extraversion"), traits related to emotionality (such as irritability, frequency of smiling), and approach or avoidance of unfamiliar events. There is generally a low correlation between descriptions by teachers and behavioral observations by scientists of features used in determining temperament.
See also
- Four temperaments
- Functional Ensemble of Temperament
- Keirsey Temperament Sorter
- Socionics temperaments
- Structure of Temperament Questionnaire
- Big Five Personality Traits
- Blood type personality theory
References
- Rusalov, VM; Trofimova, IN (2007). Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.
- Friedman, Schustack, Howard S., Miriam W. (2016). Personality: Classic Theories and Modern Research. USA: Pearson Education. ISBN 9780133829808.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Kristal, J. (2005). The temperament perspective: Working with children's behavior styles. New York, NY: Brookes Publishing Co.
- Kagan, Jerome. "Temperament". Encyclopedia of Early Childhood Development. Retrieved 17 March 2018.
- ^ Trofimova, IN (2016). "The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament". In Arnold, M.C. (ed.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp. 77–147.
- Irina Trofimova (2010). "An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality". The American Journal of Psychology. 123 (4): 467–480. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467. JSTOR 10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467. PMID 21291163.
- "Temperament | personality".
- Steiner, Rudolf (1909). "The Four Temperaments: Lecture in Berlin". Retrieved 2009-04-21.
- Steiner, Rudolf (1985). The Four Temperaments. ISBN 978-0-910142-11-3.
- Rusalov, Vladimir (26 February 2018). "Functional systems theory and the activity-specific approach in psychological taxonomies". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170166. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0166. PMC 5832690. PMID 29483350.
- ^ Trofimova, IN; Robbins, TW (2016). "Temperament and arousal systems: a new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 64: 382–402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.008. hdl:11375/26202. PMID 26969100. S2CID 13937324.
- ^ Trofimova, IN (2018). "Functionality vs dimensionality in psychological taxonomies, and a puzzle of emotional valence". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170167. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0167. PMC 5832691. PMID 29483351.
- Kagan, J. (1997). "Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity". Child Development. 68 (1): 139–143. doi:10.2307/1131931. JSTOR 1131931. PMID 9084130.
- ^ Jerome Kagan (2005), Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development
- Kagan, Jerome; Snidman, Nancy; Kahn, Vali; Towsley, Sara; Steinberg, Laurence; Fox, Nathan A (2007). "The Preservation of Two Infant Temperaments into Adolescence". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 72 (2): i–95. JSTOR 30163598.
- ^ Zentner, Marcel; Bates, John E. (1 January 2008). "Child Temperament: An Integrative Review of Concepts, Research Programs, and Measures". International Journal of Developmental Science. 2 (1–2): 7–37. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.598.1517. doi:10.3233/DEV-2008-21203.
- ^ "PsychPage.com Child Temperament". Retrieved 2009-04-21.
- Firchow, Nancy M.L.S. "Your Child's Temperament: Some Basics". Archived from the original on May 17, 2006. Retrieved 2009-04-21.
- "Ohio State University Extension: Understanding Your Child's Temperament". Archived from the original on 2009-04-21. Retrieved 2009-04-21.
- "University of Maine Cooperative Extension: Family Issues Facts". Archived from the original on 2009-01-07. Retrieved 2009-04-21.
- "KeepKidsHealthy.com Temperament and Personality". Archived from the original on 2009-02-07. Retrieved 2009-04-21.
- Stella Chess and Alexander Thomas, Temperament, Routledge: 1996. ISBN 978-0-87630-835-6. p. 31
- "Raising Children – Temperament and Personality". Rookie Parenting. 29 September 2014. Retrieved 12 October 2014.
- ^ Rothbart, M.K & Hwang, J. (2005). Temperament and the development of competence and motivation. In A.J. Elliot & A.C. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 167–184. ISBN 978-1-59385-606-9.
- ^ Kail RV, Barnfield A (2011). Children and Their Development, Second Canadian Edition with MyDevelopmentLab. Toronto: Pearson Education Canada. ISBN 978-0-13-255770-2.
- ^ Rothbart, M K (Oct 2004). "Temperament and the pursuit of an integrated developmental psychology". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 50 (4): 492–505. doi:10.1353/mpq.2004.0035. S2CID 144970654.
- Ormel, J; Oldehinkel, A. J; Ferdinand, R. F; Hartman, C. A; De Winter, A. F; Veenstra, R; Vollebergh, W; Minderaa, R. B; Buitelaar, J. K; Verhulst, F. C (2005). "Internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence: General and dimension-specific effects of familial loadings and preadolescent temperament traits". Psychological Medicine. 35 (12): 1825–35. doi:10.1017/S0033291705005829. PMID 16300695. S2CID 10109488.
- ^ Rothbart, M. K. (2007). "Temperament, development and personality" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (4): 207–212. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00505.x. S2CID 15994633.
- Valiente, C; Eisenberg, N; Smith, C. L; Reiser, M; Fabes, R. A; Losoya, S; Guthrie, I. K; Murphy, B. C (2003). "The relations of effortful control and reactive control to children's externalizing problems: A longitudinal assessment". Journal of Personality. 71 (6): 1171–96. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.7106011. PMID 14633062.
- Kochanska, G; Murray, K. T; Harlan, E. T (2000). "Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development". Developmental Psychology. 36 (2): 220–32. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220. PMID 10749079.
- Depue, R. & Fu, Y. (2012) Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults. In: Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. (Eds.) Handbook of Temperament. NY: Guilford Publications, 368-399. (2012).
- Sulis, W. (2018). "Assessing the continuum between temperament and affective illness: Psychiatric and mathematical perspectives". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170168. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0168. PMC 5832692. PMID 29483352.
- Watson, D; Naragon-Gainey, K (2014). "Personality, Emotions, and the Emotional Disorders". Clinical Psychological Science. 2 (4): 422–442. doi:10.1177/2167702614536162. PMC 4370336. PMID 25815243.
- Trofimova, I.; Sulis, W (2018). "There is more to mental illness than negative affect: Comprehensive temperament profiles in depression and generalized anxiety". BMC Psychiatry. 18 (1): 125. doi:10.1186/s12888-018-1695-x. PMC 5946468. PMID 29747614.
- Development of Temperament Symposium (2008). Philoctetes Center, New York; with panelists Sue Carter, Francis Champagne, Susan Coates, Ed Nercessian, Donald Pfaff, Daniel Schechter, Nadia Bruschweiler Stern
- Bishop, Gillian; Spence, Susan H.; Mcdonald, Casey (November 2003). "Can Parents and Teachers Provide a Reliable and Valid Report of Behavioral Inhibition?". Child Development. 74 (6): 1899–1917. doi:10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00645.x. JSTOR 3696311. PMID 14669903. S2CID 16653275.
Additional References
- Anschütz, Marieke, Children and Their Temperaments. ISBN 0-86315-175-2.
- Carey, William B., Understanding Your Child's Temperament. ISBN 1-4134-7028-9.
- Diamond, S. (1957). Personality and temperament New York: Harper
- Kagan J. Galen's prophecy: temperament in human nature. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1994.
- Kagan J, Snidman NC. The long shadow of temperament. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press; 2004.
- Kohnstamm GA, Bates JE, Rothbart MK, eds. Temperament in childhood Oxford, United Kingdom: John Wiley and Sons; 1989:59-73.
- Neville, Helen F., and Diane Clark Johnson, "Temperament Tools: Working with Your Child's Inborn Traits". ISBN 1-884734-34-0.
- Shick, Lyndall,"Understanding Temperament: Strategies for Creating Family Harmony". ISBN 1-884734-32-4.
- Seifer, RA; Sameroff, AJ; Barrette, LC; Krafchuk, E. (1994). "Infant temperament measured by multiple observations and mother report". Child Development. 65 (5): 1478–1490. doi:10.2307/1131512. JSTOR 1131512. PMID 7982363.
- Thomas, Chess & Birch (1968). Temperament and Behavior Disorders in Children. New York, New York University Press
External links
- Cloninger, C. Robert; Cloninger, Kevin M.; Zwir, Igor; Keltikangas-Järvinen, Liisa (11 November 2019). "The complex genetics and biology of human temperament: a review of traditional concepts in relation to new molecular findings". Translational Psychiatry. 9 (1): 290. doi:10.1038/s41398-019-0621-4. PMC 6848211. PMID 31712636.
- Henig, Robin Marantz. "Understanding the Anxious Mind". New York Times Magazine, September 29, 2009. Retrieved October 3, 2009.