Revision as of 12:55, 9 February 2020 editMarcocapelle (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users, Page movers555,717 edits removed Category:Biology of gender; added Category:Sex differences in humans using HotCat← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 05:33, 11 November 2024 edit undoOAbot (talk | contribs)Bots439,234 editsm Open access bot: hdl, arxiv, doi updated in citation with #oabot. | ||
(570 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Short description|Area of scientific research}} | |||
{{Sex differences}} | {{Sex differences}} | ||
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | <!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
Sex differences in ] have long been a topic of debate among researchers and scholars. It is now recognized that there are no significant sex differences in average IQ,<ref name="Hunt">{{cite book|last1=Hunt|first1= Earl B.|author-link1=Earl B. Hunt|title =Human Intelligence|isbn =978-1139495110|publisher=]|year=2010|page=389|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=DwO4TtKAiCoC&pg=PA389}}</ref><ref name=":7">{{Cite book |last=Halpern |first=Diane F. |chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=guzbKF8vTVcC&pg=PA964 |title=Encyclopedia of Women and Gender |publisher=Elsevier Science |year=2001 |isbn=0080548490 |editor-last=Worell |editor-first=Judith |pages=964 |chapter=Sex Difference Research – Cognitive Abilities |author-link=Diane F. Halpern}}</ref> though particular subtypes of intelligence vary somewhat between sexes.<ref name="Hunt 2">{{cite book |last1=Hunt |first1=Earl B. |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=DwO4TtKAiCoC&pg=PA378 |title=Human Intelligence |publisher=] |year=2010 |isbn=978-1139495110 |pages=378–379 |author-link1=Earl B. Hunt}}</ref><ref name="Mackintosh">{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=BcKcAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA362|title=IQ and Human Intelligence|vauthors=Mackintosh N|publisher=]|year=2011|isbn=978-0199585595|pages=362–363}}</ref> | |||
While some test batteries show slightly greater intelligence in males, others show slightly greater intelligence in females.<ref name="Hunt 2" /><ref name="Mackintosh"/> In particular, studies have shown female subjects performing better on tasks related to ],<ref name="Hunt"/> and males performing better on tasks related to rotation of objects in space, often categorized as ].<ref name="Terry">{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=4s5WCgAAQBAJ&pg=PA356|title=Learning and Memory: Basic Principles, Processes, and Procedures, Fourth Edition|vauthors=Terry WS|publisher=]|year=2015|isbn=978-1317350873|page=356}}</ref> | |||
Some research indicates that male advantages on some ] are minimized when controlling for socioeconomic factors.<ref name="Mackintosh"/> It has also been hypothesized that there is slightly ] in male scores in certain areas compared to female scores, leading to males' being over-represented at the top and bottom extremes of the distribution, though the evidence for this hypothesis is inconclusive.<ref name=":9" /> | |||
⚫ | == Historical perspectives == | ||
⚫ | <!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> |
||
⚫ | Prior to the 20th century, it was a commonly held view that men were intellectually superior to women.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Lips|first1=Hilary M.|title=Sex & Gender: An Introduction|date=1997|publisher=Mayfield|location=Mountain View, Calif. |
||
In 1875, ] said that women were incapable of abstract thought and could not understand issues of justice and had only the ability to understand issues of care.<ref name="Worell">{{cite book|vauthors=Worell J|title=Encyclopedia of Women and Gender, Two-Volume Set: Sex Similarities and Differences and the Impact of Society on Gender|isbn =0122272455|publisher=]|year=2001|page=441|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=7SXhBdqejgYC&pg=PA441}}</ref> In 1925, ] also stated that women were less morally developed in the concept of justice and, unlike men, were more influenced by feeling than rational thought.<ref name="Worell"/> Early brain studies comparing mass and volumes between the sexes concluded that women were intellectually inferior because ].<ref name="Fine">{{cite book|vauthors=Fine C|title=Delusions of Gender: The Real Science Behind Sex Differences|isbn =1848313969|publisher=]|year=2005|page=96|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=JbdkAgAAQBAJ&pg=PT96}}</ref> Many believed that the size difference caused women to be excitable, emotional, sensitive, and therefore not suited for political participation.<ref name="Grandner">Margarete Grandner, Austrian women in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries: cross-disciplinary perspectives, Berghahn Books, 1996, {{ISBN|1-57181-045-5}}, {{ISBN|978-1-57181-045-8}}</ref> | |||
⚫ | In the |
||
⚫ | During the early |
||
⚫ | In his 1916 study of children's IQs, psychologist ] concluded that "the intelligence of girls, at least up to 14 years, does not differ materially from that of boys". He did, however, find "rather marked" differences on a minority of tests. For example, he found boys were "decidedly better" in arithmetical reasoning, while girls were "superior" at answering comprehension questions. He also proposed that discrimination, lack of opportunity, women's responsibilities in motherhood, or emotional factors may have accounted for the fact that few women had careers in intellectual fields.<ref name="Terman">{{cite book |
||
==IQ research == | |||
==Research on general intelligence == | |||
{{see also|Intelligence quotient}} | {{see also|Intelligence quotient}} | ||
===Background=== | ===Background=== | ||
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> |
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
There is no statistically significant difference between the average ] of men and women.<ref name="Hunt" /><ref name="Sternberg" /><ref name="Mackintosh"/><ref name="Child">{{cite book|vauthors=Child D|title=Psychology and the teacher|isbn =978-0826487155|publisher=]|year=2007|page=305|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Jj8mAQAAIAAJ}}</ref> Average differences have been reported, however, on some tests of mathematics and verbal ability in certain contexts.<ref name="Mackintosh" /><ref name="Chrisler JC">{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Xtq0M1f_aIMC&pg=PA302|title=Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology: Volume 1: Gender Research in General and Experimental Psychology|vauthors=Chrisler JC, McCreary DR|publisher=]|year=2010|isbn=978-1441914651|page=302}}</ref><ref name="Terry" /> Some studies have suggested that there may be more variability in cognitive ability among males than among females,<ref name="Chrisler JC" /> but others have contradicted this,<ref name=":4" /> or presented evidence that differential variability is culturally rather than biologically determined.<ref name=":8" /> According to psychologist ], "there are both differences and similarities in the cognitive abilities of women and men, but there is no data-based rationale to support the idea that either is the smarter or superior sex."<ref name=":7" /> | |||
Chamorro-Premuzic et al. stated, "The g factor, which is often used synonymously with general intelligence, is a latent variable which emerges in a factor analysis of various cognitive ('IQ') tests. They are not exactly the same thing. ''g'' is an ''indicator'' or ''measure'' of general intelligence; it's not general intelligence itself."<ref name="Chamorro-Premuzic">{{cite book|vauthors=Chamorro-Premuzic T, Stumm SV, Furnham A|title =The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences|isbn=978-1444343113|publisher=]|year=2011|page=416|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=fVUVgc9q_34C&pg=RA1-PT416}}</ref> | |||
=== Findings === | |||
All or most of the major tests commonly used to measure intelligence have been constructed so that there are no overall score differences between males and females. Thus, there is little difference between the average IQ scores of men and women.<ref name="Worell 2">{{cite book|vauthors=Worell J|title=Encyclopedia of Women and Gender, Two-Volume Set: Sex Similarities and Differences and the Impact of Society on Gender|isbn =0122272455|publisher=]|year=2001|page=964|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=guzbKF8vTVcC&pg=PA964}}</ref><ref name="Child">{{cite book|vauthors=Child D|title=Psychology and the teacher|isbn =978-0826487155|publisher=]|year=2007|page=305|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Jj8mAQAAIAAJ}}</ref> Differences have been reported, however, in specific areas such as mathematics and verbal measures.<ref name="Mackintosh"/><ref name="Chrisler JC"/><ref name="Terry"/> Also, studies have found the variability of male scores is greater than that of female scores, resulting in more males than females in the top and bottom of the IQ distribution.<ref name="Chrisler JC"/> | |||
⚫ | <!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
Although most tests show no sex difference, there are some that do. For example, it has been found that female subjects tend to perform better on tests of verbal abilities and processing speed while males tend to perform better on tests of visual-spatial ability and ].<ref name=":10" /><ref name=":2" /> For verbal fluency, females have been specifically found to perform slightly better, on average, in vocabulary and reading comprehension and significantly higher in speech production and essay writing.<ref name=":0">{{cite book |last1=Hyde |first1=Janet Shibley |title=Biological, Social, and Organizational Components of Success for Women in Academic Science and Engineering: Report of a Workshop |date=2006 |publisher=National Academies Press |isbn=978-0-309-10041-0 |pages=127–137 |chapter=Women in Science and Mathematics: Gender Similarities in Abilities and Sociocultural Forces |chapter-url=https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK23762/}}</ref> Males have been specifically found to perform better, on average, in spatial visualization, spatial perception, and mental rotation.<ref name=":0" /> None of these findings, however, suggest an advantage for either sex in ],<ref name=":2" /> nor in ].<ref name=":10" /> | |||
=== In favor of males or females in ''g'' factor === | |||
{{further|Richard Lynn|Helmuth Nyborg|J. Philippe Rushton}} | |||
⚫ | <!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> |
||
⚫ | Most studies find either a very small difference or no sex difference with regard to general intelligence.<ref name="Hunt 2" /><ref name="Nyborg">{{cite book |doi=10.1016/B978-0-08-043793-4.X5033-8 |title=The Scientific Study of General Intelligence |year=2003 |isbn=9780080437934 }}</ref> In 2000, researchers ] and Francisco J. Abad conducted a large study of 10,475 adults on five IQ tests taken from the Primary Mental Abilities and found negligible or no significant sex differences. The tests conducted were on vocabulary, spatial rotation, verbal fluency and inductive reasoning.<ref name="Nyborg" /> | ||
Research, using the ] (WAIS III and WAIS-R), that finds general intelligence in favor of males indicates a very small difference.<ref name="Hunt 2"/> This is consistent across countries.<ref name="Hunt 2"/> In the United States and Canada, the IQ points range from two to three points in favor of males, while the points rise to four points in favor of males in China and Japan.<ref name="Hunt 2"/> By contrast, some research finds greater advantage for adult females.<ref name="Mackintosh"/> For children in the United States and the Netherlands, there are one to two IQ point differences in favor of boys.<ref name="Hunt 2"/> Other research has found a slight advantage for girls on the residual verbal factor.<ref name="Hunt 2"/> | |||
⚫ | The literature on sex differences in intelligence has produced inconsistent results due to the type of testing used, and this has resulted in debate among researchers.<ref name="Chamorro-Premuzic">{{cite book |last1=Chamorro-Premuzic |first1=Tomas |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=F_SyBwAAQBAJ&pg=PA346 |title=The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences |last2=Stumm |first2=Sophie von |last3=Furnham |first3=Adrian |publisher=] |year=2015 |isbn=978-1119050308 |pages=346, 363 |author2-link=Sophie von Stumm}}</ref> Garcia (2002) argues that there might be a small insignificant sex difference in ''intelligence in general'' (IQ) but this may not necessarily reflect a sex difference in ''general intelligence'' or ''g'' factor.<ref name="Chamorro-Premuzic" /> Although most researchers distinguish between ''g'' and IQ, those that argued for greater male intelligence asserted that IQ and ''g'' are synonymous (Lynn & Irwing 2004) and so the real division comes from defining IQ in relation to ''g'' factor. In 2008, Lynn and Irwing proposed that since ] ability correlates highest with ''g'' factor, researchers would have no choice but to accept greater male intelligence if differences on working memory tasks are found. As a result, a ] study published by Schmidt (2009) conducted an investigation into this proposal by measuring sex differences on an ] working memory task. The results found no sex difference in working memory capacity, thus contradicting the position put forward by Lynn and Irwing (2008) and more in line with those arguing for no sex differences in intelligence.<ref name="Chamorro-Premuzic" /> | ||
A 2004 ] by ] and ] published in 2005 found that the mean IQ of men exceeded that of women by up to 5 points on the ] test.<ref name="Lynn2005">{{cite journal |doi=10.1348/000712605X53542 |title=Sex differences in means and variability on the progressive matrices in university students: A meta-analysis |year=2005 |last1=Irwing |first1=Paul |last2=Lynn |first2=Richard |journal=British Journal of Psychology |volume=96 |issue=4 |pages=505–24 |pmid=16248939|url=https://semanticscholar.org/paper/fac3107206dd96ca62068de890170198140b9d28 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.008 |title=Sex differences on the progressive matrices: A meta-analysis |year=2004 |last1=Lynn |first1=Richard |last2=Irwing |first2=Paul |journal=Intelligence |volume=32 |issue=5 |pages=481–498}}</ref> Lynn's findings were debated in a series of articles for '']''.<ref name="Lynn debate">{{cite book|vauthors=Child D|title=The Psychologist|publisher=]|year=2006|page=10|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ar4kAQAAIAAJ}}</ref> He argued that there is a greater male advantage than most tests indicate, stating that because girls mature faster than boys, and that cognitive competence increases with physiological age, rather than with calender age, the male-female difference is small or negative prior to puberty, but males have an advantage after adolescence and this advantage continues into adulthood.<ref name="Hunt 2"/> | |||
⚫ | A 2012 review by researchers ], ], ], ], ], ] and ] discussed ]'s 1998 studies on sex differences in intelligence. Jensen's tests were significantly g-loaded but were not set up to get rid of any sex differences (''read ]''). They summarized his conclusions finding "No evidence was found for sex differences in the mean level of g or in the variability of g. Males, on average, excel on some factors; females on others." Jensen's conclusion that no overall sex differences existed for ''g'' has been reinforced by researchers who analyzed this issue with a battery of 42 mental ability tests among adults and found no sex difference.<ref name=":2">{{cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |title=Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments |journal=American Psychologist |date=February 2012 |volume=67 |issue=2 |pages=130–159 |doi=10.1037/a0026699 |pmid=22233090 }}</ref> | ||
=== In favor of no sex differences or inconclusive consensus === | |||
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | |||
A large analysis by five researchers with a representative sample size of over 15,000 participants found no support for sex differences in IQ, neither among children nor among adults.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Iliescu |first1=Dragos |last2=Ilie |first2=Alexandra |last3=Ispas |first3=Dan |last4=Dobrean |first4=Anca |last5=Clinciu |first5=Aurel Ion |title=Sex differences in intelligence: A multi-measure approach using nationally representative samples from Romania |journal=Intelligence |date=September 2016 |volume=58 |pages=54–61 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2016.06.007 }}</ref> | |||
⚫ | Most studies find either a very small difference |
||
A 2022 meta-analysis found that even small sex-based differences in general intelligence were an artifact of older tests, with current test batteries showing no statistically significant difference between the sexes, but that differences in intelligence sub-types such as processing speed (favoring females) and visual-spatial reasoning (favoring males) remained even when controlling for test age.<ref name=":10">{{Cite journal |last1=Giofrè |first1=D. |last2=Allen |first2=K. |last3=Toffalini |first3=E. |last4=Caviola |first4=S. |date=22 September 2022 |title=The Impasse on Gender Differences in Intelligence: a Meta-Analysis on WISC Batteries |journal=Educational Psychology Review |volume=34 |issue=4 |pages=2543–2568|doi=10.1007/s10648-022-09705-1 |hdl=11567/1107935 |hdl-access=free }}</ref> | |||
⚫ | The literature on sex differences in intelligence has produced inconsistent results due to the type of testing used, and this has resulted in debate among researchers.<ref name=" |
||
⚫ | A 2012 review by researchers ], ], ], ], ], ] and ] discussed ]'s 1998 studies on sex differences in intelligence. Jensen's tests were significantly g-loaded but were not set up to get rid of any sex differences (''read ]''). They summarized his conclusions |
||
Although most of the tests showed no difference, there were some that did. For example, they found female subjects performed better on verbal abilities while males performed better on visuospatial abilities.<ref name=":2" /> For verbal fluency, females have been specifically found to perform slightly better in vocabulary and reading comprehension but significantly higher in speech production and essay writing.<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal|title = Women in Science and Mathematics|url = https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK23762/|date = 2006-01-01|first = National Academy of Sciences|last = (us)|first2 = National Academy of Engineering|last2 = (us)|first3 = and Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Maximizing the Potential of Women in Academic Science and|last3 = Engineering|publisher = National Academies Press (US)}}</ref> Males have been specifically found to perform better on spatial visualization, spatial perception, and mental rotation.<ref name=":0" /> Researchers had then recommended that general models such as ] and ] be divided into ], perceptual and ] domains of ''g''; this is because, as this model is applied, females excel at verbal and perceptual tasks while males on visuospatial tasks, thus evening out the sex differences on IQ tests.<ref name=":2" /> | |||
=== Variability === | === Variability === | ||
{{Main|Variability hypothesis}} | {{Main|Variability hypothesis}} | ||
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> |
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
Some studies have identified the degree of IQ ] as a difference between males and females. |
Some studies have identified the degree of IQ ] as a difference between males and females. Some researchers have argued that males tend to show greater variability on many traits, a view which is termed the ]; for example, having both highest and lowest scores on tests of cognitive abilities.<ref name="Chrisler JC" /> Other research has been published which contradicts this hypothesis, however, showing either equal variability between the sexes in some cultural contexts or else greater representation of females at the upper extreme of some measures of cognitive ability.<ref name=":9">{{Cite book|last1=Bosson|first1=Jennifer K.|title=The Psychology of Sex and Gender|last2=Buckner|first2=Camille E.|last3=Vandello|first3=Joseph A.|publisher=SAGE Publications|year=2020|isbn=978-1544394015|location=New York|pages=250–251}}</ref> | ||
Feingold ( |
Feingold (1992) and Hedges and Nowell (1995) reported that, despite average sex differences being small and relatively stable over time, test score variances of males were generally larger than those of females.<ref name=":6">{{Cite book|title = Sex and Gender|url = https://archive.org/details/sexgender0000arch_l8q2|url-access = registration|pages=–188|publisher = Cambridge University Press|date = 2002-07-11|isbn = 9780521635332|first1 = John|last1 = Archer|first2 = Barbara|last2 = Lloyd}}</ref> Feingold "found that males were more variable than females on tests of quantitative reasoning, spatial visualisation, spelling, and general knowledge. ... Hedges and Nowell go one step further and demonstrate that, with the exception of performance on tests of reading comprehension, perceptual speed, and associative memory, more males than females were observed among high-scoring individuals."<ref name=":6" /> | ||
In regards to variability in mathematics performance, a meta-analysis by Lindberg et al. (2010) found male-to-female variance ratios ranged from 0.88 to 1.34 across studies with an average of 1.07, indicating nearly equivalent male and female variances.<ref name=":4" /> The authors note that greater male variability is not ubiquitous, and ratios less than 1.0 have been reported in some national and international data sets.<ref name=":4" /> A review by Hyde et al. (2009) also evaluated the topic of greater male variability in mathematics performance.<ref name=":8">{{Cite journal|last1=Hyde|first1=Janet S.|last2=Mertz|first2=Janet E.|date=2009-06-02|title=Gender, culture, and mathematics performance|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America|volume=106|issue=22|pages=8801–8807|doi=10.1073/pnas.0901265106|issn=0027-8424|pmc=2689999|pmid=19487665|bibcode=2009PNAS..106.8801H|doi-access=free}}</ref> The review found that the gender gap among the highest performers has narrowed over time in the U.S., is not found among some ethnic groups and in some nations, and correlates with several measures of gender inequality.<ref name=":8" /> The authors conclude that greater male variability in math performance is largely an artifact of cultural factors as opposed to innate biological sex differences.<ref name=":8" /> | |||
== Brain and intelligence == | == Brain and intelligence == | ||
{{see also|Neuroscience of sex differences}} | {{see also|Neuroscience of sex differences}} | ||
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> |
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
Differences in brain physiology between sexes do not necessarily relate to differences in intellect. Although men have larger brains, men and women |
Differences in brain physiology between sexes do not necessarily relate to differences in intellect. Although men have larger brains, men and women have equal IQs.<ref name="Kalat">{{cite book|vauthors=Kalat JW|title=Biological Psychology|isbn =978-1111831004|publisher=]|year=2012|pages=118, 120|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=evbdjAQYA1cC&pg=PA118}}</ref> For men, the gray matter volume in the ] and ]s correlates with IQ; for women, the gray matter volume in the frontal lobe and ] (which is used in language processing) correlates with IQ.<ref name="Garrett">{{cite book|vauthors=Garrett B, Hough G|title=Brain & Behavior: An Introduction to Behavioral Neuroscience|isbn =978-1506349190|publisher=]|year=2017|page=118|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=4Xg4DwAAQBAJ&pg=PA377}}</ref> Women have greater cortical thickness, ] and cortical surface area (controlling for body size) which compensates for smaller ].<ref>{{Cite book|title = Sex difference in the human brain, their underpinnings and implications|url = https://books.google.com/books?id=JFpq6hYQRhQC&pg=PA6|publisher = ]|date =2010 |isbn = 9780444536310|pages=6–7|language = en}}</ref> Meta-analysis and studies have found that brain size explains 6–12% of variance among individual intelligence and cortical thickness explains 5%.<ref name="Pietschnig">{{Cite journal|title = Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean?|journal = Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews|date = 2015-10-01|pages = 411–432|volume = 57|doi = 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.017|first1 = Jakob|last1 = Pietschnig|first2 = Lars|last2 = Penke|first3 = Jelte M.|last3 = Wicherts|first4 = Michael|last4 = Zeiler|first5 = Martin|last5 = Voracek|pmid=26449760|s2cid = 23180321}}</ref> | ||
Although a meta-analysis of 148 samples from over 8000 participants reported a weak correlation between brain size and IQ,<ref name="Pietschnig" /> men and women did not differ in IQ, and the researchers concluded that "it is not warranted to interpret brain size as an isomorphic proxy of human intelligence differences."<ref name="Pietschnig" /> Brain volume contributes little to IQ test performance. Outside of comparing intelligence levels of the sexes, brain size is only one of numerous factors that influence intelligence, alongside white matter integrity, overall developmental stability, parieto-frontal neuronal networks, neuronal efficiency, and cortical ]. Brain structural integrity seems to be more important as a biological basis.<ref name="Pietschnig" /> | |||
Women have greater cortical thickness, ] and cortical surface area (controlling for body size) which compensates for smaller brain size.<ref>{{Cite book|title = Sex difference in the human brain, their underpinnings and implications|url = https://books.google.com/books?id=JFpq6hYQRhQC&pg=PA6|publisher = ]|date =2010 |isbn = 9780444536310|pages=6–7|language = en}}</ref> Meta-analysis and studies have found that brain size explains 6–12% of variance among individual intelligence and cortical thickness explains 5%.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean?|journal = Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews|date = 2015-10-01|pages = 411–432|volume = 57|doi = 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.017|first = Jakob|last = Pietschnig|first2 = Lars|last2 = Penke|first3 = Jelte M.|last3 = Wicherts|first4 = Michael|last4 = Zeiler|first5 = Martin|last5 = Voracek|pmid=26449760}}</ref> | |||
In 2021, Lise Eliot et al found no difference in overall male/female abilities in verbal, spatial or emotion processing.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Eliot |first1=Lise |last2=Ahmed |first2=Adnan |last3=Khan |first3=Hiba |last4=Patel |first4=Julie |title=Dump the 'dimorphism': Comprehensive synthesis of human brain studies reveals few male-female differences beyond size |journal=Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews |date=June 2021 |volume=125 |pages=667–697 |doi=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.026 |doi-access=free }}</ref> | |||
==Mathematics performance== | ==Mathematics performance== | ||
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> |
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
⚫ | ] compete in the USS California Science Experience at Naval Surface Warfare Center Corona Division.]] | ||
⚫ | Across countries, males have performed better on ] tests than females, but there is the possibility male-female difference in math scores is related to ] in social roles.<ref name="Sternberg">{{cite book|vauthors=Sternberg RJ, Kaufman SB|title=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence|isbn =978-1111831004|publisher=]|year=2011|page=877|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=FtYeTcNwzQ4C&pg=PA877}}</ref> Some psychologists believe that many historical and current sex differences in mathematics performance may be related to boys' higher likelihood of receiving math encouragement than girls. Parents were, and sometimes still are, more likely to consider a son's mathematical achievement as being a natural skill while a daughter's mathematical achievement is more likely to be seen as something she studied hard for.<ref name="Wood, Samuel 2004">Wood, Samuel; Wood, Ellen; Boyd Denise (2004). "World of Psychology, The (Fifth Edition)", Allyn & Bacon {{ISBN|0-205-36137-4}}.{{page needed|date=September 2021}}</ref> This difference in attitude may discourage girls and women from further involvement in mathematics-related subjects and careers.<ref name="Wood, Samuel 2004" /> | ||
⚫ | ] compete in the USS California Science Experience at Naval Surface Warfare |
||
In a 2008 study paid for by the ] in the United States, researchers found that girls perform as well as boys on standardized math tests. They attributed this to girls now taking as many advanced math courses as boys, unlike in the past.<ref name="Davis">{{Cite book|last=Davis|first=Andrew S.|title=Handbook of Pediatric Neuropsychology|date=2010|publisher=Springer Publishing Company|isbn=978-0-82-615737-9|page=1089|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=SU9-LSh4HgcC&pg=PA1089}}</ref><ref name="Crooks">{{cite book|vauthors=Crooks RL, Baur K|title=Our Sexuality|isbn =978-1305887428|publisher=]|year=2016|page=125|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=isIaCgAAQBAJ&pg=PT157}}</ref> | |||
A performance difference in ] on the ] and international ]<ref>, p.196. ] 2015.</ref> exists in favor of males, though differences in mathematics course performance measures favor females.<ref name="Gallagher">Ann M. Gallagher, ], ''Gender differences in mathematics: an integrative psychological approach'', Cambridge University Press, 2005, {{ISBN|0-521-82605-5}}, {{ISBN|978-0-521-82605-1}}{{page needed|date=June 2013}}</ref> In 1983, Benbow concluded that the study showed a large sex difference by age 13 and that it was especially pronounced at the high end of the distribution.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1126/science.6648516 |title=Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability: More facts |year=1983 |last1=Benbow |first1=C. |last2=Stanley |first2=J. |journal=Science |volume=222 |issue=4627 |pages=1029–31 |pmid=6648516|bibcode=1983Sci...222.1029P }}</ref> However, Gallagher and Kaufman criticized Benbow's and others' reports, which found that males were over-represented in the highest percentages, on the grounds that they had not ensured representative sampling.<ref name="Gallagher"/> | |||
There is also evidence that boys are over-represented among the very best and very worst performers on measures of mathematical ability and standardized measures of IQ.<ref name="Davis"/> Some research suggests that differences in mathematics course performance measures favor females.<ref name="Gallagher">Ann M. Gallagher, ], ''Gender differences in mathematics: an integrative psychological approach'', Cambridge University Press, 2005, {{ISBN|0-521-82605-5}}, {{ISBN|978-0-521-82605-1}}{{page needed|date=June 2013}}</ref> A small performance difference in mathematics on the ]<ref>, p.196. ] 2015.</ref> persists in favor of males, though the gap has shrunk from 40 points (5.0%) in 1975<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Burton |first1=Nancy W. |last2=Lewis |first2=Charles |last3=Robertson |first3=Nancy |title=SEX DIFFERENCES IN SAT® SCORES |journal=ETS Research Report Series |date=December 1988 |volume=1988 |issue=2 |doi=10.1002/j.2330-8516.1988.tb00314.x }}</ref> to 18 points (2.3%) in 2020.<ref>{{Cite web|date=2021-09-08|title=2021 SAT Suite of Assessments Program Results – The College Board|url=https://reports.collegeboard.org/sat-suite-program-results|access-date=2021-09-16|website=College Board Program Results|language=en}}</ref> However, the SAT is not a representative sample, given that it tests only college-bound students, and more women than men have attended college since the 1990s.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Stoet|first1=Gijsbert|last2=Geary|first2=David C.|date=2020-06-23|title=Gender differences in the pathways to higher education|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|volume=117|issue=25|pages=14073–14076|doi=10.1073/pnas.2002861117|pmid=32513710|pmc=7322061|doi-access=free|bibcode=2020PNAS..11714073S }}</ref> Conversely, the international PISA exam provides representative samples. On the 2018 math PISA, there was no statistically significant difference between the performances of girls and boys in 39.5% of the 76 countries that participated. Meanwhile, boys outperformed girls in 32 countries (42.1%), while girls outperformed boys in 14 (18.4%).<ref name=":3">{{Cite web|title=Home|url=https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/sites/f56f8c26-en/index.html?itemId=/content/component/f56f8c26-en|access-date=2021-09-16|website=www.oecd-ilibrary.org|language=en}}</ref> On average, boys performed 5 points (1%) higher than girls. However, overall, the gender gap in math and science for boys and girls from similar socio-economic backgrounds was not significant.<ref name=":3" /> | |||
In nearly every study on the subject, males have out-performed females on mathematics in high school, but the size of the male-female difference, across countries, is related to ] in social roles.<ref name="Sternberg">{{cite book|vauthors=Sternberg RJ, Kaufman SB|title=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence|isbn =978-1111831004|publisher=]|year=2011|page=877|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=FtYeTcNwzQ4C&pg=PA877}}</ref> In a 2008 study paid for by the ] in the United States, however, researchers stated that "girls perform as well as boys on standardized math tests. Although 20 years ago, high school boys performed better than girls in math, the researchers found that is no longer the case. The reason, they said, is simple: Girls used to take fewer advanced math courses than boys, but now they are taking just as many."<ref>Lewin, Tamar (July 25, 2008)., '']''.</ref><ref name="Crooks">{{cite book|vauthors=Crooks RL, Baur K|title=Our Sexuality|isbn =978-1305887428|publisher=]|year=2016|page=125|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=isIaCgAAQBAJ&pg=PT157}}</ref> However, the study indicated that, while boys and girls performed similarly on average, boys were over-represented among the very best performers as well as among the very worst.<ref>Winstein, Keith J. (July 25, 2008). , '']'' (New York).</ref> | |||
On the math portion of the 2019 TIMMS, taken at a similar age as the PISA, girls outperformed boys by 3 points on average, although the difference was not statistically significant.<ref>{{cite web | url=https://timss2019.org/reports/average-achievement-by-gender-m8/index.html#:~:text=In%20TIMSS%202019%2C%20there%20was,than%20girls%20in%206%20countries | title=TIMSS 2019 International Reports – TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center at Boston College }}</ref> A meta-analysis of nearly half a million participants using data from both the TIMMS and the PISA found that differences were negligible, although girls outperfomed boys in some countries and the opposite occurred in others.<ref>{{cite web | url=https://www.scientificamerican.com/podcast/episode/no-gender-gap-in-math-10-01-06/ | title=No Gender Gap in Math | website=] }}</ref> | |||
A 2011 meta-analysis with 242 studies from 1990 to 2007 involving 1,286,350 people found no overall sex difference of performance in mathematics. The meta-analysis also found that although there were no overall differences, a small sex difference that favored males in complex problem solving is still present in high school.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = New Trends in Gender and Mathematics Performance: A Meta-Analysis|journal = Psychological Bulletin|date = 2010-11-01|issn = 0033-2909|pmc = 3057475|pmid = 21038941|pages = 1123–1135|volume = 136|issue = 6|doi = 10.1037/a0021276|first = Sara M.|last = Lindberg|first2 = Janet Shibley|last2 = Hyde|first3 = Jennifer L.|last3 = Petersen|first4 = Marcia C.|last4 = Linn}}</ref> | |||
A 2008 meta-analysis published in '']'' using data from over 7 million students found no statistically significant differences between the mathematical capabilities of males and females.<ref>{{Cite web|title=Study: No gender differences in math performance|url=https://news.wisc.edu/study-no-gender-differences-in-math-performance/|access-date=2021-09-16|website=news.wisc.edu|language=en-US}}</ref> A 2011 meta-analysis with 242 studies from 1990 to 2007 involving 1,286,350 people found no overall sex difference of performance in mathematics. The meta-analysis also found that although there were no overall differences, a small sex difference that favored males in complex problem solving was still present in high school. However, the authors note that boys continue to take more physics courses than girls, which train complex solving abilities and may provide stronger training than pure mathematics.<ref name=":4">{{cite journal|last1=Lindberg|first1=Sara M.|last2=Hyde|first2=Janet Shibley|last3=Petersen|first3=Jennifer L.|last4=Linn|first4=Marcia C.|date=2010|title=New trends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|volume=136|issue=6|pages=1123–1135|doi=10.1037/a0021276|pmc=3057475|pmid=21038941}}</ref> | |||
⚫ | |||
The mathematics ] examination results in ] have often been inconsistent about which sex performed better. In 2008, 14.7% of females scored an A or above, while only 13.9% of males did the same. But in 2024, only 15.5% of females scored an A or above, while 18% of males did.<ref name="t540">{{cite web | title=GCSE outcomes in England | website=Ofqual | url=https://analytics.ofqual.gov.uk/apps/GCSE/Outcomes/ | access-date=2024-08-26}}</ref> A 2020 analysis of gender differences in the mathematical abilities of 13 million students in Italy found that males performed better at mathematics and that this difference appeared to increase the richer the Italian regions were, which is also characterized by greater gender equality.<ref name="y086">{{cite journal | last1=Giofrè | first1=D. | last2=Cornoldi | first2=C. | last3=Martini | first3=A. | last4=Toffalini | first4=E. | title=A population level analysis of the gender gap in mathematics: Results on over 13 million children using the INVALSI dataset | journal=Intelligence | publisher=Elsevier BV | volume=81 | year=2020 | issn=0160-2896 | doi=10.1016/j.intell.2020.101467 | doi-access=free | page=101467| arxiv=2112.11940 }}</ref> | |||
] has been shown to affect performance and confidence in mathematics of both males and females.<ref name="Fine" /><ref name="Gallagher" /> | |||
One line of inquiry has focused on the role that ] might play in mathematics performance differences between male and female test-takers.<ref name="Gallagher" /> Systematic reviews and meta-analyses suggest that stereotype threat is implicated in performance differences on some mathematics tests, though the effect appears to vary considerably in different social contexts and for different test conditions.<ref name=":42">{{cite journal |vauthors=Nguyen HH, Ryan AM |date=November 2008 |title=Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=93 |issue=6 |pages=1314–34 |doi=10.1037/a0012702 |pmid=19025250}}</ref><ref name=":52">{{Cite journal |last1=Walton |first1=Gregory M. |last2=Spencer |first2=Steven J. |date=2009-09-01 |title=Latent Ability: Grades and Test Scores Systematically Underestimate the Intellectual Ability of Negatively Stereotyped Students |journal=Psychological Science |volume=20 |issue=9 |pages=1132–1139 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x |issn=0956-7976 |pmid=19656335 |s2cid=25810191|doi-access=free }}</ref><ref name=":22">{{Cite journal |last1=Picho |first1=Katherine |last2=Rodriguez |first2=Ariel |last3=Finnie |first3=Lauren |date=May 2013 |title=Exploring the Moderating Role of Context on the Mathematics Performance of Females Under Stereotype Threat: A Meta-Analysis |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237000996 |journal=The Journal of Social Psychology |volume=153 |issue=3 |pages=299–333 |doi=10.1080/00224545.2012.737380 |pmid=23724702 |s2cid=45950675}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Pennington |first=Charlotte R. |date=2016-01-11 |title=Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators |journal=]|volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=e0146487 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0146487 |pmid=26752551 |pmc=4713435 |bibcode=2016PLoSO..1146487P |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref name=":14">{{Cite journal |last1=Liu |first1=Songqi |last2=Liu |first2=Pei |last3=Wang |first3=Mo |last4=Zhang |first4=Baoshan |date=July 2020 |title=Effectiveness of Stereotype Threat Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/343149798 |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=106 |issue=6 |pages=921–949 |doi=10.1037/apl0000770 |pmid=32772526 |s2cid=221098319}}</ref> | |||
== Reading and verbal skills == | |||
Studies have shown a female advantage in reading and verbal skills.<ref name="Hunt3">{{cite book|last1=Hunt|first1= Earl B.|author-link1=Earl B. Hunt|title =Human Intelligence|isbn =978-1139495110|publisher=]|year=2010|page=387|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=DwO4TtKAiCoC&pg=PA387}}</ref> On the international PISA reading exam, girls consistently outperform boys across all countries, and all differences are statistically significant. In the most recent PISA exam (2018), girls outperformed boys by almost 30 points.<ref>{{cite book |doi=10.1787/fb6da32e-en |chapter=PISA 2018 Data |title=PISA 2018 Results (Volume II) |date=2019 |isbn=978-92-64-89352-8 }}</ref> On average in OECD countries, 28% of boys did not obtain a reading proficiency level of 2. | |||
Studies have shown that girls spend more time reading than boys and read more for fun, likely contributing to the gap.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Hughes-Hassell|first1=Sandra|last2=Rodge|first2=Pradnya|date=2007|title=The Leisure Reading Habits of Urban Adolescents|url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/40015543|journal=Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy|volume=51|issue=1|pages=22–33|doi=10.1598/JAAL.51.1.3|jstor=40015543|issn=1081-3004}}</ref> Some psychologists believe that many historical and current sex differences in mathematics performance may be related to girls' higher likelihood of receiving reading encouragement than boys. Parents were, and sometimes still are, more likely to consider a daughter's reading achievement as being a natural skill while a son's reading achievement is more likely to be seen as something he studied hard for.<ref name="Wood, Samuel 2004"/> | |||
== Spatial ability == | == Spatial ability == | ||
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> |
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP and/or WP:MEDRS, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
] tests |
] tests]] | ||
] | ] | ||
Meta-studies show a male advantage in ], assessing horizontality and verticality, and a male advantage for most aspects of ].<ref name=":1">{{cite journal |last1=Fernandez-Baizan |first1=C. |last2=Arias |first2=J.L. |last3=Mendez |first3=M. |title=Spatial memory in young adults: Gender differences in egocentric and allocentric performance |journal=Behavioural Brain Research |date=February 2019 |volume=359 |pages=694–700 |doi=10.1016/j.bbr.2018.09.017 |pmid=30273614 |hdl=10651/49938 |s2cid=52879258 |hdl-access=free }}</ref><ref name="Voyer">{{cite journal |last1=Voyer |first1=Daniel |last2=Voyer |first2=Susan D. |last3=Saint-Aubin |first3=Jean |title=Sex differences in visual-spatial working memory: A meta-analysis |journal=Psychonomic Bulletin & Review |date=April 2017 |volume=24 |issue=2 |pages=307–334 |doi=10.3758/s13423-016-1085-7 |pmid=27357955 |doi-access=free |quote=All the tasks produced a male advantage, except for memory for location, where a female advantage emerged.}}</ref><ref name="Chrisler">{{Cite book |last = Chrisler |first = Joan C|title = Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology|publisher = Springer|isbn = 9781441914644|author2=Donald R. McCreary|date=2010|page=265|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Xtq0M1f_aIMC&pg=PA265}}</ref> Women have an advantage for certain components of spatial memory. Whereas men show a selective advantage for fine-grained metric positional reconstruction, where absolute spatial coordinates are emphasized, women show an advantage in spatial location memory, which is the ability to accurately remember relative object positions (where objects are);<ref name="Voyer" /><ref name="Becker">{{Cite book|last1= Becker |first1 = Jill B. |last2=Berkley|first2=Karen J.|last3= Geary|first3=Nori|last4=Hampson|first4=Elizabeth|last5=Herman|first5=James P. |last6= Young|first6=Elizabeth |title=Sex Differences in the Brain: From Genes to Behavior|date=2007|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn =978-0198042556|page=316|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=IeaLXPWsbuAC&pg=PA316}}</ref><ref name="Bosson">{{Cite book|last1 = Bosson |first1 =Jennifer K.|last2=Buckner|first2=Camille E.|last3=Vandello|first3=Joseph A.|title=The Psychology of Sex and Gender|date=2021|publisher=SAGE Publications|isbn=978-1544394039|page=271|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=B74IEAAAQBAJ&pg=PT271}}</ref> however, the advantage in spatial location memory is small and inconsistent across studies.<ref name="Bosson" /> | |||
A proposed evolutionary hypothesis is that men and women evolved different mental abilities to adapt to their different roles, including labor-based roles, in society.<ref name="Bosson" /> For example, "ancestral women more often foraged for fruits, vegetables, and roots over large geographic regions."<ref name="Bosson" /> The labor-based role explanation suggests that men may have evolved greater spatial abilities as a result of behaviors such as navigating during a ].<ref name="Weiten, labor-based role">{{Cite book|last=Weiten|first=Wayne|title=Psychology: Themes and Variations|date=2016|publisher=Cengage Learning|isbn=978-1-30-585612-7|page=27|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ALkaCgAAQBAJ&pg=PT77}}</ref> | |||
Sex differences in mental rotation and ] in favor of males have been observed across multiple nations, lending credence to biological origins, though countries with greater gender equality also had greater gaps in these areas, lending credence to possible social influence as well.<ref name="pmidhttps://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19130205/">{{cite journal|vauthors=Lippa RA, Collaer ML, Peters M| title=Sex differences in mental rotation and line angle judgments are positively associated with gender equality and economic development across 53 nations. | journal=Arch Sex Behav | year= 2010 | volume= 39 | issue= 4 | pages= 990–7 | pmid=19130205 | doi=10.1007/s10508-008-9460-8}} </ref> | |||
A number of studies have shown that women tend to rely more on visual information than men in a number of spatial tasks related to perceived orientation.<ref>Witkin, H. A., Lewis, H. B., Hertzman, M., Machover, K., Meissner, P. B. & Wapner, S. (1954) Personality Through Perception. An Experimental and Clinical Study. Harper, New York.</ref> | |||
Results from studies conducted in the physical environment are not conclusive about sex differences |
Results from studies conducted in the physical environment are not conclusive about sex differences. Various studies on the same task show no differences. There are studies that show no difference in finding one's way between two places.<ref name="Devlin">Devlin, Ann Sloan, Mind and maze: spatial cognition and environmental behavior, Praeger, 2001, {{ISBN|0-275-96784-0}}, {{ISBN|978-0-275-96784-0}}{{page needed|date=June 2013}}</ref> | ||
Performance in mental rotation and similar spatial tasks is affected by gender expectations.<ref name="Fine" |
Performance in mental rotation and similar spatial tasks is affected by gender expectations.<ref name="Fine">{{cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=JbdkAgAAQBAJ&pg=PT96 |title=Delusions of Gender: The Real Science Behind Sex Differences |vauthors=Fine C |publisher=] |year=2005 |isbn=1848313969 |page=96}}</ref> For example, studies show that being told before the test that men typically perform better, or that the task is linked with jobs like aviation engineering typically associated with men versus jobs like fashion design typically associated with women, will negatively affect female performance on spatial rotation and positively influence it when subjects are told the opposite.<ref>{{cite book |doi=10.1037/11546-006 |chapter=Taking Science Seriously: Straight Thinking About Spatial Sex Differences |title=Why aren't more women in science?: Top researchers debate the evidence |year=2007 |last1=Newcombe |first1=Nora S. |pages=69–77 |isbn=978-1-59147-485-2 }}</ref> | ||
Playing computer or ] games increases mental rotation ability, especially for females.<ref name="Freiman">{{Cite book|last1=Freiman|first1=Viktor|last2=Tassell|first2=Janet Lynne|title=Creativity and Technology in Mathematics Education|date=2018|publisher=Springer|isbn=978-3-31-972381-5|page=310|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=__1sDwAAQBAJ&pg=PA310}}</ref> Playing action video games in particular benefits spatial abilities in females more than in males, up to a point where sex differences in spatial attention are eliminated.<ref name="Freiman"/> Gender generally has an influence on ]. Action video games such as ]s, adventure games, and sports games are generally preferred by male players, while female players tend to prefer games such as puzzle, card, and ] games.<ref name="Quandt">{{Cite book|last1=Quandt|first1=Thorsten|last2=Kröger|first2=Sonja|title=Multiplayer: The Social Aspects of Digital Gaming|date=2013|publisher=Routledge|isbn=978-1-13-409219-2|page=36|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=zd_eAQAAQBAJ&pg=PA36}}</ref><ref name="Donsbach">{{Cite book|last=Donsbach|first=Wolfgang|title=The Concise Encyclopedia of Communication|date=2015|publisher=John Wiley & Sons|isbn=978-1-11-878923-0|pages=104–105|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=K2ORBgAAQBAJ&pg=PA104}}</ref> | |||
The possibility of ] and other ]s as a cause of sex differences in psychology has been a subject of study. Adult women who were exposed to unusually high levels of androgens in the ] due to ] score significantly higher on tests of spatial ability.<ref name="Davis AS">{{cite book|vauthors=Davis AS, D'Amato RC|title=Handbook of Pediatric Neuropsychology|isbn =978-0826157379|publisher=]|year=2010|page=1089|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=SU9-LSh4HgcC&pg=PA1089}}</ref> Some research has found positive correlations between testosterone levels in healthy males and measures of spatial ability.<ref name="Lunenfeld">{{cite book|vauthors=Lunenfeld B, Gooren LJ, Morales A, Morley J|title=Textbook of Men's Health and Aging|isbn =978-0203089798|publisher=]|year=2007|page=249|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=9vEeMOwKY4AC&pg=PA249}}</ref> However, the relationship is complex.<ref>{{cite book |doi=10.1016/S0079-6123(08)64456-8 |chapter=Performance and Intelligence in Hormonally Different Groups |title=Sex Differences in the Brain - the Relation Between Structure and Function |series=Progress in Brain Research |year=1984 |last1=Nyborg |first1=H. |isbn=978-0-444-80532-4 |volume=61 |pages=491–508 |pmid=6396713 }}</ref> | |||
The possibility of ] and other ]s as a cause of sex differences in psychology has been a subject of study, but results have been mixed. A meta-analysis of women who were exposed to unusually high levels of androgens in the ] due to ] concluded that there is no evidence of enhanced spatial ability among these individuals.<ref name="Collaer">{{cite journal |last1=Collaer |first1=Marcia L. |last2=Hines |first2=Melissa |title=No Evidence for Enhancement of Spatial Ability with Elevated Prenatal Androgen Exposure in Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia: A Meta-Analysis |journal=Archives of Sexual Behavior |date=February 2020 |volume=49 |issue=2 |pages=395–411 |doi=10.1007/s10508-020-01645-7 |pmid=32052211 |pmc=8784244 |s2cid=211101661 }}</ref> The meta-analysis speculates that average sex differences in some spatial tasks could be partially explained by androgen exposure at a different time of the life span, such as during ], or by the different socialization males and females experience.<ref name="Collaer" /> In addition, a meta-analysis showed that, although female-to-male transgender individuals who received testosterone therapy did improve their spatial abilities, male-to-female transgender individuals who took androgen-suppressants also showed an improvement or no deterioration of spatial skills.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Karalexi |first1=Maria A. |last2=Georgakis |first2=Marios K. |last3=Dimitriou |first3=Nikolaos G. |last4=Vichos |first4=Theodoros |last5=Katsimpris |first5=Andreas |last6=Petridou |first6=Eleni Th. |last7=Papadopoulos |first7=Fotios C. |title=Gender-affirming hormone treatment and cognitive function in transgender young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis |journal=Psychoneuroendocrinology |date=September 2020 |volume=119 |pages=104721 |doi=10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104721 |pmid=32512250 |s2cid=219123865 |doi-access=free }}</ref> | |||
== Sex differences in academics == | == Sex differences in academics == | ||
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> |
<!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
⚫ | A 2014 meta-analysis of sex differences in scholastic achievement published in the journal of '']'' found females outperformed males in teacher-assigned school marks throughout elementary, junior/middle, high school and at both undergraduate and graduate ] level.<ref name=":5">{{Cite journal|url = https://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/releases/bul-a0036620.pdf|title = Gender Differences in Scholastic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis|last = Voyeur|first = Daniel|date = 2014|journal = Psychological Bulletin|doi = 10.1037/a0036620|pmid = 24773502|volume = 140|issue = 4|pages = 1174–1204}}</ref> The meta-analysis done by researchers Daniel Voyer and Susan D. Voyer was from the ] drew from 97 years of 502 effect sizes and 369 samples stemming from the year 1914 to 2011, and found that the magnitude of higher female performance was not affected by year of publication, thereby contradicting recent claims of "]" in school achievement.<ref name=":5" /> | ||
A 2015 study by researchers Gijsbert Stoet and ] from the journal of ] reported that girl's overall education achievement is better in 70 percent of all the 47–75 countries that participated in ].<ref name="Stoet 137–151">{{Cite journal|title = Sex differences in academic achievement are not related to political, economic, or social equality|journal = Intelligence|date = 1 January 2015|pages = 137–151|volume = 48|doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.006|first1 = Gijsbert|last1 = Stoet|first2 = David C.|last2 = Geary| s2cid=143234406 }}</ref> The study consisting of 1.5 million 15-year-olds found higher overall female achievement across reading, mathematics, and science literacy and better performance across 70% of participating countries, including many with considerable gaps in economic and political equality, and they fell behind in only 4% of countries.<ref name="Stoet 137–151" /> Stoet et al. said sex differences in educational achievement are not reliably linked to gender equality.<ref name="Stoet 137–151" /> | |||
However, there is some evidence that there is bias in teacher grading against males, although evidence for this is not fully conclusive.<ref name="y770">{{cite web | last1=Liberto | first1=Adriana Di | last2=Casula | first2=Laura | title=Teacher Assessments versus Standardized Tests: Is Acting "Girly" an Advantage? | website=IZA | url=https://www.iza.org/publications/dp/10458/teacher-assessments-versus-standardizedtests-%20is-acting-girly-an-advantage | access-date=2024-09-22}}</ref><ref name="l080">{{cite journal | last=Terrier | first=Camille | title=Boys lag behind: How teachers' gender biases affect student achievement | journal=Economics of Education Review | publisher=Elsevier BV | volume=77 | year=2020 | issn=0272-7757 | doi=10.1016/j.econedurev.2020.101981 | page=101981| url=https://econpapers.repec.org/RePEc:iza:izadps:dp10343 | hdl=10419/149202 | hdl-access=free }}</ref><ref name="r454">{{cite journal | last1=Lievore | first1=Ilaria | last2=Triventi | first2=Moris | title=Do teacher and classroom characteristics affect the way in which girls and boys are graded? | journal=British Journal of Sociology of Education | publisher=Informa UK Limited | volume=44 | issue=1 | date=2022-10-17 | issn=0142-5692 | doi=10.1080/01425692.2022.2122942 | pages=97–122}}</ref><ref name="z851">{{cite journal | last1=Berg | first1=Petter | last2=Palmgren | first2=Ola | last3=Tyrefors | first3=Björn | title=Gender grading bias in junior high school mathematics | journal=Applied Economics Letters | volume=27 | issue=11 | date=2020-06-24 | issn=1350-4851 | doi=10.1080/13504851.2019.1646862 | pages=915–919| url=https://www.ifn.se/wfiles/wp/wp1263.pdf }}</ref> According to a global report performed by the ] of over 60 countries, girls were given higher marks in comparison to boys with the same ability.<ref name="u634">{{cite web | last=Coughlan | first=Sean | title=Teachers 'give higher marks to girls' | website=BBC News | date=2015-03-05 | url=https://www.bbc.com/news/education-31751672 | access-date=2024-10-09}}</ref> | |||
⚫ | == Historical perspectives == | ||
⚫ | <!-- NOTE: Per WP:SCHOLARSHIP, please to stick to secondary or tertiary sourcing for material you add to this article. Avoid primary sources. Avoid single study material unless covered in a secondary or tertiary source.--> | ||
⚫ | Prior to the 20th century, it was a commonly held view that men were intellectually superior to women.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Lips|first1=Hilary M.|title=Sex & Gender: An Introduction|date=1997|publisher=Mayfield|isbn=978-1559346306|edition=3rd|location=Mountain View, Calif.|page=40}}</ref><ref>{{cite book|last1=Denmark|first1=Florence L.|url=https://archive.org/details/psychologywomenh00denm|title=Psychology of Women: A Handbook of Issues and Theories|last2=Paludi|first2=Michele A.|date=2008|publisher=Praeger|isbn=978-0275991623|edition=2nd|location=Westport, Conn.|pages=–11|url-access=limited}}</ref> Early brain studies comparing mass and volumes between the sexes suggested that women were intellectually inferior because ].<ref name="Fine" /> Writer ] publicly disputed this idea with ], former ]. | ||
⚫ | In the 19th century, whether men and women had equal intelligence was seen by many as a prerequisite for the granting of ].<ref name="Grandner">Margarete Grandner, Austrian women in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries: cross-disciplinary perspectives, Berghahn Books, 1996, {{ISBN|1-57181-045-5}}, {{ISBN|978-1-57181-045-8}}</ref> ] argued that women were not permitted to realize their full potential, as they were confined to the roles of child-rearing and ]. | ||
⚫ | During the early 20th century, the scientific consensus shifted to the view that sex plays no role in intelligence.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Burt|first1=C. L.|last2=Moore|first2=R. C.|year=1912|title=The mental differences between the sexes|journal=Journal of Experimental Pedagogy|volume=1|issue=273–284|pages=355–388}}</ref> | ||
⚫ | A 2014 meta-analysis of sex differences in scholastic achievement published in the journal of '']'' found females outperformed males in teacher-assigned school marks throughout elementary, junior/middle, high school and at both undergraduate and graduate ] level.<ref name=":5">{{Cite journal|url = https://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/releases/bul-a0036620.pdf|title = Gender Differences in Scholastic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis|last = Voyeur|first = Daniel|date = 2014|journal = Psychological Bulletin|doi = 10.1037/a0036620|pmid = 24773502| |
||
⚫ | In his 1916 study of children's IQs, psychologist ] concluded that "the intelligence of girls, at least up to 14 years, does not differ materially from that of boys". He did, however, find "rather marked" differences on a minority of tests. For example, he found boys were "decidedly better" in arithmetical reasoning, while girls were "superior" at answering comprehension questions. He also proposed that discrimination, lack of opportunity, women's responsibilities in motherhood, or emotional factors may have accounted for the fact that few women had careers in intellectual fields.<ref name="Terman">{{cite book|last=Terman|first=Lewis M.|url=https://archive.org/details/measurementofint008006mbp|title=The measurement of intelligence: an explanation of and a complete guide for the use of the Stanford revision and extension of the Binet-Simon intelligence scale|publisher=Houghton Mifflin|year=1916|location=Boston|pages=–72|oclc=186102}}</ref><ref name="Dennis">{{cite book|url=https://archive.org/details/historicalreadin0000denn|title=Historical readings in developmental psychology|vauthors=Dennis W|publisher=]|year=1972|isbn=9780390262998|page=|url-access=registration}}</ref> | ||
Beyond sex differences in academic ability, recent research has also been focusing on women's underrepresentation in higher education, especially in the fields of natural science, technology, engineering and mathematics (]).<ref>S. J. Ceci, W. M. Williams, Why Aren’t More Women in Science? (APA Books, Washington, DC, 2007)</ref> | |||
== See also == | == See also == | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
Line 109: | Line 121: | ||
==References== | ==References== | ||
{{ |
{{Reflist|2}} | ||
{{Sex differences in humans}} | {{Sex differences in humans}} | ||
{{Evolutionary psychology}} | |||
] | ] | ||
] | ] |
Latest revision as of 05:33, 11 November 2024
Area of scientific researchPart of a series on |
Sex differences in humans |
---|
Biology |
Medicine and health |
Neuroscience and psychology |
Sociology and society |
Sex differences in human intelligence have long been a topic of debate among researchers and scholars. It is now recognized that there are no significant sex differences in average IQ, though particular subtypes of intelligence vary somewhat between sexes.
While some test batteries show slightly greater intelligence in males, others show slightly greater intelligence in females. In particular, studies have shown female subjects performing better on tasks related to verbal ability, and males performing better on tasks related to rotation of objects in space, often categorized as spatial ability.
Some research indicates that male advantages on some cognitive tests are minimized when controlling for socioeconomic factors. It has also been hypothesized that there is slightly higher variability in male scores in certain areas compared to female scores, leading to males' being over-represented at the top and bottom extremes of the distribution, though the evidence for this hypothesis is inconclusive.
IQ research
See also: Intelligence quotientBackground
There is no statistically significant difference between the average IQ scores of men and women. Average differences have been reported, however, on some tests of mathematics and verbal ability in certain contexts. Some studies have suggested that there may be more variability in cognitive ability among males than among females, but others have contradicted this, or presented evidence that differential variability is culturally rather than biologically determined. According to psychologist Diane Halpern, "there are both differences and similarities in the cognitive abilities of women and men, but there is no data-based rationale to support the idea that either is the smarter or superior sex."
Findings
Although most tests show no sex difference, there are some that do. For example, it has been found that female subjects tend to perform better on tests of verbal abilities and processing speed while males tend to perform better on tests of visual-spatial ability and crystallized intelligence. For verbal fluency, females have been specifically found to perform slightly better, on average, in vocabulary and reading comprehension and significantly higher in speech production and essay writing. Males have been specifically found to perform better, on average, in spatial visualization, spatial perception, and mental rotation. None of these findings, however, suggest an advantage for either sex in general intelligence, nor in fluid intelligence.
Most studies find either a very small difference or no sex difference with regard to general intelligence. In 2000, researchers Roberto Colom and Francisco J. Abad conducted a large study of 10,475 adults on five IQ tests taken from the Primary Mental Abilities and found negligible or no significant sex differences. The tests conducted were on vocabulary, spatial rotation, verbal fluency and inductive reasoning.
The literature on sex differences in intelligence has produced inconsistent results due to the type of testing used, and this has resulted in debate among researchers. Garcia (2002) argues that there might be a small insignificant sex difference in intelligence in general (IQ) but this may not necessarily reflect a sex difference in general intelligence or g factor. Although most researchers distinguish between g and IQ, those that argued for greater male intelligence asserted that IQ and g are synonymous (Lynn & Irwing 2004) and so the real division comes from defining IQ in relation to g factor. In 2008, Lynn and Irwing proposed that since working memory ability correlates highest with g factor, researchers would have no choice but to accept greater male intelligence if differences on working memory tasks are found. As a result, a neuroimaging study published by Schmidt (2009) conducted an investigation into this proposal by measuring sex differences on an n-back working memory task. The results found no sex difference in working memory capacity, thus contradicting the position put forward by Lynn and Irwing (2008) and more in line with those arguing for no sex differences in intelligence.
A 2012 review by researchers Richard E. Nisbett, Joshua Aronson, Clancy Blair, William Dickens, James Flynn, Diane F. Halpern and Eric Turkheimer discussed Arthur Jensen's 1998 studies on sex differences in intelligence. Jensen's tests were significantly g-loaded but were not set up to get rid of any sex differences (read differential item functioning). They summarized his conclusions finding "No evidence was found for sex differences in the mean level of g or in the variability of g. Males, on average, excel on some factors; females on others." Jensen's conclusion that no overall sex differences existed for g has been reinforced by researchers who analyzed this issue with a battery of 42 mental ability tests among adults and found no sex difference.
A large analysis by five researchers with a representative sample size of over 15,000 participants found no support for sex differences in IQ, neither among children nor among adults.
A 2022 meta-analysis found that even small sex-based differences in general intelligence were an artifact of older tests, with current test batteries showing no statistically significant difference between the sexes, but that differences in intelligence sub-types such as processing speed (favoring females) and visual-spatial reasoning (favoring males) remained even when controlling for test age.
Variability
Main article: Variability hypothesisSome studies have identified the degree of IQ variance as a difference between males and females. Some researchers have argued that males tend to show greater variability on many traits, a view which is termed the variability hypothesis; for example, having both highest and lowest scores on tests of cognitive abilities. Other research has been published which contradicts this hypothesis, however, showing either equal variability between the sexes in some cultural contexts or else greater representation of females at the upper extreme of some measures of cognitive ability.
Feingold (1992) and Hedges and Nowell (1995) reported that, despite average sex differences being small and relatively stable over time, test score variances of males were generally larger than those of females. Feingold "found that males were more variable than females on tests of quantitative reasoning, spatial visualisation, spelling, and general knowledge. ... Hedges and Nowell go one step further and demonstrate that, with the exception of performance on tests of reading comprehension, perceptual speed, and associative memory, more males than females were observed among high-scoring individuals."
In regards to variability in mathematics performance, a meta-analysis by Lindberg et al. (2010) found male-to-female variance ratios ranged from 0.88 to 1.34 across studies with an average of 1.07, indicating nearly equivalent male and female variances. The authors note that greater male variability is not ubiquitous, and ratios less than 1.0 have been reported in some national and international data sets. A review by Hyde et al. (2009) also evaluated the topic of greater male variability in mathematics performance. The review found that the gender gap among the highest performers has narrowed over time in the U.S., is not found among some ethnic groups and in some nations, and correlates with several measures of gender inequality. The authors conclude that greater male variability in math performance is largely an artifact of cultural factors as opposed to innate biological sex differences.
Brain and intelligence
See also: Neuroscience of sex differencesDifferences in brain physiology between sexes do not necessarily relate to differences in intellect. Although men have larger brains, men and women have equal IQs. For men, the gray matter volume in the frontal and parietal lobes correlates with IQ; for women, the gray matter volume in the frontal lobe and Broca's area (which is used in language processing) correlates with IQ. Women have greater cortical thickness, cortical complexity and cortical surface area (controlling for body size) which compensates for smaller brain size. Meta-analysis and studies have found that brain size explains 6–12% of variance among individual intelligence and cortical thickness explains 5%.
Although a meta-analysis of 148 samples from over 8000 participants reported a weak correlation between brain size and IQ, men and women did not differ in IQ, and the researchers concluded that "it is not warranted to interpret brain size as an isomorphic proxy of human intelligence differences." Brain volume contributes little to IQ test performance. Outside of comparing intelligence levels of the sexes, brain size is only one of numerous factors that influence intelligence, alongside white matter integrity, overall developmental stability, parieto-frontal neuronal networks, neuronal efficiency, and cortical gyrification. Brain structural integrity seems to be more important as a biological basis.
In 2021, Lise Eliot et al found no difference in overall male/female abilities in verbal, spatial or emotion processing.
Mathematics performance
Across countries, males have performed better on mathematics tests than females, but there is the possibility male-female difference in math scores is related to gender inequality in social roles. Some psychologists believe that many historical and current sex differences in mathematics performance may be related to boys' higher likelihood of receiving math encouragement than girls. Parents were, and sometimes still are, more likely to consider a son's mathematical achievement as being a natural skill while a daughter's mathematical achievement is more likely to be seen as something she studied hard for. This difference in attitude may discourage girls and women from further involvement in mathematics-related subjects and careers.
In a 2008 study paid for by the National Science Foundation in the United States, researchers found that girls perform as well as boys on standardized math tests. They attributed this to girls now taking as many advanced math courses as boys, unlike in the past.
There is also evidence that boys are over-represented among the very best and very worst performers on measures of mathematical ability and standardized measures of IQ. Some research suggests that differences in mathematics course performance measures favor females. A small performance difference in mathematics on the SAT persists in favor of males, though the gap has shrunk from 40 points (5.0%) in 1975 to 18 points (2.3%) in 2020. However, the SAT is not a representative sample, given that it tests only college-bound students, and more women than men have attended college since the 1990s. Conversely, the international PISA exam provides representative samples. On the 2018 math PISA, there was no statistically significant difference between the performances of girls and boys in 39.5% of the 76 countries that participated. Meanwhile, boys outperformed girls in 32 countries (42.1%), while girls outperformed boys in 14 (18.4%). On average, boys performed 5 points (1%) higher than girls. However, overall, the gender gap in math and science for boys and girls from similar socio-economic backgrounds was not significant.
On the math portion of the 2019 TIMMS, taken at a similar age as the PISA, girls outperformed boys by 3 points on average, although the difference was not statistically significant. A meta-analysis of nearly half a million participants using data from both the TIMMS and the PISA found that differences were negligible, although girls outperfomed boys in some countries and the opposite occurred in others.
A 2008 meta-analysis published in Science using data from over 7 million students found no statistically significant differences between the mathematical capabilities of males and females. A 2011 meta-analysis with 242 studies from 1990 to 2007 involving 1,286,350 people found no overall sex difference of performance in mathematics. The meta-analysis also found that although there were no overall differences, a small sex difference that favored males in complex problem solving was still present in high school. However, the authors note that boys continue to take more physics courses than girls, which train complex solving abilities and may provide stronger training than pure mathematics.
The mathematics GCSE examination results in England have often been inconsistent about which sex performed better. In 2008, 14.7% of females scored an A or above, while only 13.9% of males did the same. But in 2024, only 15.5% of females scored an A or above, while 18% of males did. A 2020 analysis of gender differences in the mathematical abilities of 13 million students in Italy found that males performed better at mathematics and that this difference appeared to increase the richer the Italian regions were, which is also characterized by greater gender equality.
One line of inquiry has focused on the role that stereotype threat might play in mathematics performance differences between male and female test-takers. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses suggest that stereotype threat is implicated in performance differences on some mathematics tests, though the effect appears to vary considerably in different social contexts and for different test conditions.
Reading and verbal skills
Studies have shown a female advantage in reading and verbal skills. On the international PISA reading exam, girls consistently outperform boys across all countries, and all differences are statistically significant. In the most recent PISA exam (2018), girls outperformed boys by almost 30 points. On average in OECD countries, 28% of boys did not obtain a reading proficiency level of 2.
Studies have shown that girls spend more time reading than boys and read more for fun, likely contributing to the gap. Some psychologists believe that many historical and current sex differences in mathematics performance may be related to girls' higher likelihood of receiving reading encouragement than boys. Parents were, and sometimes still are, more likely to consider a daughter's reading achievement as being a natural skill while a son's reading achievement is more likely to be seen as something he studied hard for.
Spatial ability
Meta-studies show a male advantage in mental rotation, assessing horizontality and verticality, and a male advantage for most aspects of spatial memory. Women have an advantage for certain components of spatial memory. Whereas men show a selective advantage for fine-grained metric positional reconstruction, where absolute spatial coordinates are emphasized, women show an advantage in spatial location memory, which is the ability to accurately remember relative object positions (where objects are); however, the advantage in spatial location memory is small and inconsistent across studies.
A proposed evolutionary hypothesis is that men and women evolved different mental abilities to adapt to their different roles, including labor-based roles, in society. For example, "ancestral women more often foraged for fruits, vegetables, and roots over large geographic regions." The labor-based role explanation suggests that men may have evolved greater spatial abilities as a result of behaviors such as navigating during a hunt.
Sex differences in mental rotation and judgement of line angles in favor of males have been observed across multiple nations, lending credence to biological origins, though countries with greater gender equality also had greater gaps in these areas, lending credence to possible social influence as well.
Results from studies conducted in the physical environment are not conclusive about sex differences. Various studies on the same task show no differences. There are studies that show no difference in finding one's way between two places.
Performance in mental rotation and similar spatial tasks is affected by gender expectations. For example, studies show that being told before the test that men typically perform better, or that the task is linked with jobs like aviation engineering typically associated with men versus jobs like fashion design typically associated with women, will negatively affect female performance on spatial rotation and positively influence it when subjects are told the opposite.
Playing computer or video games increases mental rotation ability, especially for females. Playing action video games in particular benefits spatial abilities in females more than in males, up to a point where sex differences in spatial attention are eliminated. Gender generally has an influence on preference of game genre. Action video games such as first-person shooters, adventure games, and sports games are generally preferred by male players, while female players tend to prefer games such as puzzle, card, and platform games.
The possibility of testosterone and other androgens as a cause of sex differences in psychology has been a subject of study, but results have been mixed. A meta-analysis of women who were exposed to unusually high levels of androgens in the womb due to congenital adrenal hyperplasia concluded that there is no evidence of enhanced spatial ability among these individuals. The meta-analysis speculates that average sex differences in some spatial tasks could be partially explained by androgen exposure at a different time of the life span, such as during mini-puberty, or by the different socialization males and females experience. In addition, a meta-analysis showed that, although female-to-male transgender individuals who received testosterone therapy did improve their spatial abilities, male-to-female transgender individuals who took androgen-suppressants also showed an improvement or no deterioration of spatial skills.
Sex differences in academics
A 2014 meta-analysis of sex differences in scholastic achievement published in the journal of Psychological Bulletin found females outperformed males in teacher-assigned school marks throughout elementary, junior/middle, high school and at both undergraduate and graduate university level. The meta-analysis done by researchers Daniel Voyer and Susan D. Voyer was from the University of New Brunswick drew from 97 years of 502 effect sizes and 369 samples stemming from the year 1914 to 2011, and found that the magnitude of higher female performance was not affected by year of publication, thereby contradicting recent claims of "boy crisis" in school achievement.
A 2015 study by researchers Gijsbert Stoet and David C. Geary from the journal of Intelligence reported that girl's overall education achievement is better in 70 percent of all the 47–75 countries that participated in PISA. The study consisting of 1.5 million 15-year-olds found higher overall female achievement across reading, mathematics, and science literacy and better performance across 70% of participating countries, including many with considerable gaps in economic and political equality, and they fell behind in only 4% of countries. Stoet et al. said sex differences in educational achievement are not reliably linked to gender equality.
However, there is some evidence that there is bias in teacher grading against males, although evidence for this is not fully conclusive. According to a global report performed by the OECD of over 60 countries, girls were given higher marks in comparison to boys with the same ability.
Historical perspectives
Prior to the 20th century, it was a commonly held view that men were intellectually superior to women. Early brain studies comparing mass and volumes between the sexes suggested that women were intellectually inferior because they have smaller and lighter brains. Writer Helen H. Gardener publicly disputed this idea with William A. Hammond, former Surgeon General of the United States Army.
In the 19th century, whether men and women had equal intelligence was seen by many as a prerequisite for the granting of suffrage. Leta Hollingworth argued that women were not permitted to realize their full potential, as they were confined to the roles of child-rearing and housekeeping.
During the early 20th century, the scientific consensus shifted to the view that sex plays no role in intelligence.
In his 1916 study of children's IQs, psychologist Lewis Terman concluded that "the intelligence of girls, at least up to 14 years, does not differ materially from that of boys". He did, however, find "rather marked" differences on a minority of tests. For example, he found boys were "decidedly better" in arithmetical reasoning, while girls were "superior" at answering comprehension questions. He also proposed that discrimination, lack of opportunity, women's responsibilities in motherhood, or emotional factors may have accounted for the fact that few women had careers in intellectual fields.
See also
- Sex differences in emotional intelligence
- Sex differences in humans
- Emotional intelligence
- Sex differences in psychology
- Heritability of IQ
References
- ^ Hunt, Earl B. (2010). Human Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. p. 389. ISBN 978-1139495110.
- ^ Halpern, Diane F. (2001). "Sex Difference Research – Cognitive Abilities". In Worell, Judith (ed.). Encyclopedia of Women and Gender. Elsevier Science. p. 964. ISBN 0080548490.
- ^ Hunt, Earl B. (2010). Human Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. pp. 378–379. ISBN 978-1139495110.
- ^ Mackintosh N (2011). IQ and Human Intelligence. OUP Oxford. pp. 362–363. ISBN 978-0199585595.
- ^ Terry WS (2015). Learning and Memory: Basic Principles, Processes, and Procedures, Fourth Edition. Psychology Press. p. 356. ISBN 978-1317350873.
- ^ Bosson, Jennifer K.; Buckner, Camille E.; Vandello, Joseph A. (2020). The Psychology of Sex and Gender. New York: SAGE Publications. pp. 250–251. ISBN 978-1544394015.
- ^ Sternberg RJ, Kaufman SB (2011). The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. p. 877. ISBN 978-1111831004.
- Child D (2007). Psychology and the teacher. Continuum. p. 305. ISBN 978-0826487155.
- ^ Chrisler JC, McCreary DR (2010). Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology: Volume 1: Gender Research in General and Experimental Psychology. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 302. ISBN 978-1441914651.
- ^ Lindberg, Sara M.; Hyde, Janet Shibley; Petersen, Jennifer L.; Linn, Marcia C. (2010). "New trends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 136 (6): 1123–1135. doi:10.1037/a0021276. PMC 3057475. PMID 21038941.
- ^ Hyde, Janet S.; Mertz, Janet E. (2009-06-02). "Gender, culture, and mathematics performance". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (22): 8801–8807. Bibcode:2009PNAS..106.8801H. doi:10.1073/pnas.0901265106. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 2689999. PMID 19487665.
- ^ Giofrè, D.; Allen, K.; Toffalini, E.; Caviola, S. (22 September 2022). "The Impasse on Gender Differences in Intelligence: a Meta-Analysis on WISC Batteries". Educational Psychology Review. 34 (4): 2543–2568. doi:10.1007/s10648-022-09705-1. hdl:11567/1107935.
- ^ Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (February 2012). "Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments". American Psychologist. 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. PMID 22233090.
- ^ Hyde, Janet Shibley (2006). "Women in Science and Mathematics: Gender Similarities in Abilities and Sociocultural Forces". Biological, Social, and Organizational Components of Success for Women in Academic Science and Engineering: Report of a Workshop. National Academies Press. pp. 127–137. ISBN 978-0-309-10041-0.
- ^ The Scientific Study of General Intelligence. 2003. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-043793-4.X5033-8. ISBN 9780080437934.
- ^ Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas; Stumm, Sophie von; Furnham, Adrian (2015). The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 346, 363. ISBN 978-1119050308.
- Iliescu, Dragos; Ilie, Alexandra; Ispas, Dan; Dobrean, Anca; Clinciu, Aurel Ion (September 2016). "Sex differences in intelligence: A multi-measure approach using nationally representative samples from Romania". Intelligence. 58: 54–61. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2016.06.007.
- ^ Archer, John; Lloyd, Barbara (2002-07-11). Sex and Gender. Cambridge University Press. pp. 187–188. ISBN 9780521635332.
- Kalat JW (2012). Biological Psychology. Cengage Learning. pp. 118, 120. ISBN 978-1111831004.
- Garrett B, Hough G (2017). Brain & Behavior: An Introduction to Behavioral Neuroscience. SAGE Publications. p. 118. ISBN 978-1506349190.
- Sex difference in the human brain, their underpinnings and implications. Elsevier. 2010. pp. 6–7. ISBN 9780444536310.
- ^ Pietschnig, Jakob; Penke, Lars; Wicherts, Jelte M.; Zeiler, Michael; Voracek, Martin (2015-10-01). "Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean?". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 57: 411–432. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.017. PMID 26449760. S2CID 23180321.
- Eliot, Lise; Ahmed, Adnan; Khan, Hiba; Patel, Julie (June 2021). "Dump the 'dimorphism': Comprehensive synthesis of human brain studies reveals few male-female differences beyond size". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 125: 667–697. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.026.
- ^ Wood, Samuel; Wood, Ellen; Boyd Denise (2004). "World of Psychology, The (Fifth Edition)", Allyn & Bacon ISBN 0-205-36137-4.
- ^ Davis, Andrew S. (2010). Handbook of Pediatric Neuropsychology. Springer Publishing Company. p. 1089. ISBN 978-0-82-615737-9.
- Crooks RL, Baur K (2016). Our Sexuality. Cengage Learning. p. 125. ISBN 978-1305887428.
- ^ Ann M. Gallagher, James C. Kaufman, Gender differences in mathematics: an integrative psychological approach, Cambridge University Press, 2005, ISBN 0-521-82605-5, ISBN 978-0-521-82605-1
- "Gender differences in mathematics performance", p.196. OECD 2015.
- Burton, Nancy W.; Lewis, Charles; Robertson, Nancy (December 1988). "SEX DIFFERENCES IN SAT® SCORES". ETS Research Report Series. 1988 (2). doi:10.1002/j.2330-8516.1988.tb00314.x.
- "2021 SAT Suite of Assessments Program Results – The College Board". College Board Program Results. 2021-09-08. Retrieved 2021-09-16.
- Stoet, Gijsbert; Geary, David C. (2020-06-23). "Gender differences in the pathways to higher education". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 117 (25): 14073–14076. Bibcode:2020PNAS..11714073S. doi:10.1073/pnas.2002861117. PMC 7322061. PMID 32513710.
- ^ "Home". www.oecd-ilibrary.org. Retrieved 2021-09-16.
- "TIMSS 2019 International Reports – TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center at Boston College".
- "No Gender Gap in Math". Scientific American.
- "Study: No gender differences in math performance". news.wisc.edu. Retrieved 2021-09-16.
- "GCSE outcomes in England". Ofqual. Retrieved 2024-08-26.
- Giofrè, D.; Cornoldi, C.; Martini, A.; Toffalini, E. (2020). "A population level analysis of the gender gap in mathematics: Results on over 13 million children using the INVALSI dataset". Intelligence. 81. Elsevier BV: 101467. arXiv:2112.11940. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2020.101467. ISSN 0160-2896.
- Nguyen HH, Ryan AM (November 2008). "Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 93 (6): 1314–34. doi:10.1037/a0012702. PMID 19025250.
- Walton, Gregory M.; Spencer, Steven J. (2009-09-01). "Latent Ability: Grades and Test Scores Systematically Underestimate the Intellectual Ability of Negatively Stereotyped Students". Psychological Science. 20 (9): 1132–1139. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 19656335. S2CID 25810191.
- Picho, Katherine; Rodriguez, Ariel; Finnie, Lauren (May 2013). "Exploring the Moderating Role of Context on the Mathematics Performance of Females Under Stereotype Threat: A Meta-Analysis". The Journal of Social Psychology. 153 (3): 299–333. doi:10.1080/00224545.2012.737380. PMID 23724702. S2CID 45950675.
- Pennington, Charlotte R. (2016-01-11). "Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators". PLOS One. 11 (1): e0146487. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1146487P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146487. PMC 4713435. PMID 26752551.
- Liu, Songqi; Liu, Pei; Wang, Mo; Zhang, Baoshan (July 2020). "Effectiveness of Stereotype Threat Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review". Journal of Applied Psychology. 106 (6): 921–949. doi:10.1037/apl0000770. PMID 32772526. S2CID 221098319.
- Hunt, Earl B. (2010). Human Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. p. 387. ISBN 978-1139495110.
- "PISA 2018 Data". PISA 2018 Results (Volume II). 2019. doi:10.1787/fb6da32e-en. ISBN 978-92-64-89352-8.
- Hughes-Hassell, Sandra; Rodge, Pradnya (2007). "The Leisure Reading Habits of Urban Adolescents". Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy. 51 (1): 22–33. doi:10.1598/JAAL.51.1.3. ISSN 1081-3004. JSTOR 40015543.
- Fernandez-Baizan, C.; Arias, J.L.; Mendez, M. (February 2019). "Spatial memory in young adults: Gender differences in egocentric and allocentric performance". Behavioural Brain Research. 359: 694–700. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2018.09.017. hdl:10651/49938. PMID 30273614. S2CID 52879258.
- ^ Voyer, Daniel; Voyer, Susan D.; Saint-Aubin, Jean (April 2017). "Sex differences in visual-spatial working memory: A meta-analysis". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 24 (2): 307–334. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-1085-7. PMID 27357955.
All the tasks produced a male advantage, except for memory for location, where a female advantage emerged.
- Chrisler, Joan C; Donald R. McCreary (2010). Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology. Springer. p. 265. ISBN 9781441914644.
- Becker, Jill B.; Berkley, Karen J.; Geary, Nori; Hampson, Elizabeth; Herman, James P.; Young, Elizabeth (2007). Sex Differences in the Brain: From Genes to Behavior. Oxford University Press. p. 316. ISBN 978-0198042556.
- ^ Bosson, Jennifer K.; Buckner, Camille E.; Vandello, Joseph A. (2021). The Psychology of Sex and Gender. SAGE Publications. p. 271. ISBN 978-1544394039.
- Weiten, Wayne (2016). Psychology: Themes and Variations. Cengage Learning. p. 27. ISBN 978-1-30-585612-7.
- Lippa RA, Collaer ML, Peters M (2010). "Sex differences in mental rotation and line angle judgments are positively associated with gender equality and economic development across 53 nations". Arch Sex Behav. 39 (4): 990–7. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9460-8. PMID 19130205.
- Devlin, Ann Sloan, Mind and maze: spatial cognition and environmental behavior, Praeger, 2001, ISBN 0-275-96784-0, ISBN 978-0-275-96784-0
- ^ Fine C (2005). Delusions of Gender: The Real Science Behind Sex Differences. Icon Books. p. 96. ISBN 1848313969.
- Newcombe, Nora S. (2007). "Taking Science Seriously: Straight Thinking About Spatial Sex Differences". Why aren't more women in science?: Top researchers debate the evidence. pp. 69–77. doi:10.1037/11546-006. ISBN 978-1-59147-485-2.
- ^ Freiman, Viktor; Tassell, Janet Lynne (2018). Creativity and Technology in Mathematics Education. Springer. p. 310. ISBN 978-3-31-972381-5.
- Quandt, Thorsten; Kröger, Sonja (2013). Multiplayer: The Social Aspects of Digital Gaming. Routledge. p. 36. ISBN 978-1-13-409219-2.
- Donsbach, Wolfgang (2015). The Concise Encyclopedia of Communication. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 104–105. ISBN 978-1-11-878923-0.
- ^ Collaer, Marcia L.; Hines, Melissa (February 2020). "No Evidence for Enhancement of Spatial Ability with Elevated Prenatal Androgen Exposure in Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia: A Meta-Analysis". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 49 (2): 395–411. doi:10.1007/s10508-020-01645-7. PMC 8784244. PMID 32052211. S2CID 211101661.
- Karalexi, Maria A.; Georgakis, Marios K.; Dimitriou, Nikolaos G.; Vichos, Theodoros; Katsimpris, Andreas; Petridou, Eleni Th.; Papadopoulos, Fotios C. (September 2020). "Gender-affirming hormone treatment and cognitive function in transgender young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 119: 104721. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104721. PMID 32512250. S2CID 219123865.
- ^ Voyeur, Daniel (2014). "Gender Differences in Scholastic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 140 (4): 1174–1204. doi:10.1037/a0036620. PMID 24773502.
- ^ Stoet, Gijsbert; Geary, David C. (1 January 2015). "Sex differences in academic achievement are not related to political, economic, or social equality". Intelligence. 48: 137–151. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.006. S2CID 143234406.
- Liberto, Adriana Di; Casula, Laura. "Teacher Assessments versus Standardized Tests: Is Acting "Girly" an Advantage?". IZA. Retrieved 2024-09-22.
- Terrier, Camille (2020). "Boys lag behind: How teachers' gender biases affect student achievement". Economics of Education Review. 77. Elsevier BV: 101981. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2020.101981. hdl:10419/149202. ISSN 0272-7757.
- Lievore, Ilaria; Triventi, Moris (2022-10-17). "Do teacher and classroom characteristics affect the way in which girls and boys are graded?". British Journal of Sociology of Education. 44 (1). Informa UK Limited: 97–122. doi:10.1080/01425692.2022.2122942. ISSN 0142-5692.
- Berg, Petter; Palmgren, Ola; Tyrefors, Björn (2020-06-24). "Gender grading bias in junior high school mathematics" (PDF). Applied Economics Letters. 27 (11): 915–919. doi:10.1080/13504851.2019.1646862. ISSN 1350-4851.
- Coughlan, Sean (2015-03-05). "Teachers 'give higher marks to girls'". BBC News. Retrieved 2024-10-09.
- Lips, Hilary M. (1997). Sex & Gender: An Introduction (3rd ed.). Mountain View, Calif.: Mayfield. p. 40. ISBN 978-1559346306.
- Denmark, Florence L.; Paludi, Michele A. (2008). Psychology of Women: A Handbook of Issues and Theories (2nd ed.). Westport, Conn.: Praeger. pp. 7–11. ISBN 978-0275991623.
- Margarete Grandner, Austrian women in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries: cross-disciplinary perspectives, Berghahn Books, 1996, ISBN 1-57181-045-5, ISBN 978-1-57181-045-8
- Burt, C. L.; Moore, R. C. (1912). "The mental differences between the sexes". Journal of Experimental Pedagogy. 1 (273–284): 355–388.
- Terman, Lewis M. (1916). The measurement of intelligence: an explanation of and a complete guide for the use of the Stanford revision and extension of the Binet-Simon intelligence scale. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. pp. 68–72. OCLC 186102.
- Dennis W (1972). Historical readings in developmental psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts. p. 214. ISBN 9780390262998.
Sex differences in humans | ||
---|---|---|
Biology | ||
Medicine and Health | ||
Neuroscience and Psychology | ||
Sociology |