Misplaced Pages

Global workspace theory: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editNext edit →Content deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 05:11, 27 July 2005 editKSchutte (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users5,660 editsNo edit summary← Previous edit Revision as of 16:44, 21 August 2005 edit undoJanice Rowe (talk | contribs)187 editsNo edit summaryNext edit →
Line 1: Line 1:
'''Global Workspace Theory''' (GWT) was proposed by ] (1988, 1997). It is based on the concept of a human ] – the "inner domain in which we can rehearse telephone numbers to ourselves or, more interestingly, in which we carry on the narratives of our lives. It is usually thought to include inner speech and visual imagery." The contents of this small, short-term ] closely correspond to what we are ] of, and get broadcast to the vast multitude of ] cognitive brain ] which require entrance into the workspace to be globally accessible. These ] processes, operating in parallel with limited ] between them, can form coalitions whose participants cooperate to achieve certain goals. Individual as well as allied processes compete for access to the global workspace, striving to disseminate their messages to all other processes in an effort to obtain more cohorts and thereby increase the likelihood of achieving their goals. While Baars (1997) concedes that the global workspace "is closely related to conscious experience, though not identical to it", GWT can successfully explain several characteristics of consciousness, such as its role in handling novel situations, its limited capacity, and its sequential nature. Moreover, it lends itself well to computational modeling. '''Global Workspace Theory''' (GWT) is a simple cognitive architecture that has been developed to account qualitatively for a large set of matched pairs of conscious and unconscious processes, and it was proposed by ] (1988, 1997). It is based on the concept of a human ] – the "inner domain in which we can rehearse telephone numbers to ourselves or, more interestingly, in which we carry on the narratives of our lives. It is usually thought to include inner speech and visual imagery." The contents of this small, short-term ] closely correspond to what we are ] of, and get broadcast to the vast multitude of ] cognitive brain ] which require entrance into the workspace to be globally accessible. These ] processes, operating in parallel with limited ] between them, can form coalitions whose participants cooperate to achieve certain goals. Individual as well as allied processes compete for access to the global workspace, striving to disseminate their messages to all other processes in an effort to obtain more cohorts and thereby increase the likelihood of achieving their goals. While Baars (1997) concedes that the global workspace "is closely related to conscious experience, though not identical to it", GWT can successfully explain several characteristics of consciousness, such as its role in handling novel situations, its limited capacity, and its sequential nature. Moreover, it lends itself well to computational modeling.


This theory explores "behind the scenes" contextual systems, which shape conscious contents without ever becoming conscious, such as the ] ] stream of the visual system. This architectural approach leads to specific neural hypotheses. ] ] in different modalities may be mutually inhibitory, within approximately 100-ms time steps.


== References == == References ==

Revision as of 16:44, 21 August 2005

Global Workspace Theory (GWT) is a simple cognitive architecture that has been developed to account qualitatively for a large set of matched pairs of conscious and unconscious processes, and it was proposed by Bernard Baars (1988, 1997). It is based on the concept of a human working memory – the "inner domain in which we can rehearse telephone numbers to ourselves or, more interestingly, in which we carry on the narratives of our lives. It is usually thought to include inner speech and visual imagery." The contents of this small, short-term memory closely correspond to what we are conscious of, and get broadcast to the vast multitude of unconscious cognitive brain processes which require entrance into the workspace to be globally accessible. These subconscious processes, operating in parallel with limited communication between them, can form coalitions whose participants cooperate to achieve certain goals. Individual as well as allied processes compete for access to the global workspace, striving to disseminate their messages to all other processes in an effort to obtain more cohorts and thereby increase the likelihood of achieving their goals. While Baars (1997) concedes that the global workspace "is closely related to conscious experience, though not identical to it", GWT can successfully explain several characteristics of consciousness, such as its role in handling novel situations, its limited capacity, and its sequential nature. Moreover, it lends itself well to computational modeling.

This theory explores "behind the scenes" contextual systems, which shape conscious contents without ever becoming conscious, such as the dorsal cortical stream of the visual system. This architectural approach leads to specific neural hypotheses. Sensory consciousness in different modalities may be mutually inhibitory, within approximately 100-ms time steps.

References

  • Baars, Bernard (1988), A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press)
  • Baars, Bernard (1997), In the Theater of Consciousness (New York, NY: Oxford University Press)


See also

Category: