Misplaced Pages

Octopus: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editNext edit →Content deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 22:24, 19 October 2010 view source67.163.141.165 (talk) Biology← Previous edit Revision as of 22:29, 19 October 2010 view source 67.163.141.165 (talk) BiologyNext edit →
Line 25: Line 25:
== Biology == == Biology ==
] (''Octopus vulgaris'')]] ] (''Octopus vulgaris'')]]
Octopuses are characterized by their eight ], usually bearing ]s. The arms of octopuses are often distinguished from the pair of feeding ]s found in ] and ].<ref>Norman, M. 2000. ''Cephalopods: A World Guide''. ConchBooks, Hackenheim. p. 15. "There is some confusion around the terms ''arms'' versus ''tentacles''. The numerous limbs of ]es are called tentacles. The ring of eight limbs around the mouth in cuttlefish, squids and octopuses are called ''arms''. Cuttlefish and squid also have a pair of specialized limbs attached between the bases of the third and fourth arm pairs . These are known as ''feeding tentacles'' and are used to shoot out and grab prey."</ref> Both types of limbs are ]s. Unlike most other cephalopods, the majority of octopuses – those in the suborder most commonly known, ] – have almost entirely soft bodies with no internal ]. They have neither a protective outer ] like the ], nor any vestige of an internal shell or ]s, like cuttlefish or squid. A ], similar in shape to a ]'s beak, is the only hard part of their body. This enables them to squeeze through very narrow slits between underwater rocks, which is very helpful when they are fleeing from ] or other predatory fish. The octopuses in the less familiar ] suborder have two fins and an internal shell, generally reducing their ability to squeeze into small spaces. Octopi are characterized by their eight ], usually bearing ]s. The arms of octopi are often distinguished from the pair of feeding ]s found in ] and ].<ref>Norman, M. 2000. ''Cephalopods: A World Guide''. ConchBooks, Hackenheim. p. 15. "There is some confusion around the terms ''arms'' versus ''tentacles''. The numerous limbs of ]es are called tentacles. The ring of eight limbs around the mouth in cuttlefish, squids and octopi are called ''arms''. Cuttlefish and squid also have a pair of specialized limbs attached between the bases of the third and fourth arm pairs . These are known as ''feeding tentacles'' and are used to shoot out and grab prey."</ref> Both types of limbs are ]s. Unlike most other cephalopods, the majority of octopi – those in the suborder most commonly known, ] – have almost entirely soft bodies with no internal ]. They have neither a protective outer ] like the ], nor any vestige of an internal shell or ]s, like cuttlefish or squid. A ], similar in shape to a ]'s beak, is the only hard part of their body. This enables them to squeeze through very narrow slits between underwater rocks, which is very helpful when they are fleeing from ] or other predatory fish. The octopi in the less familiar ] suborder have two fins and an internal shell, generally reducing their ability to squeeze into small spaces.


]s during ]]] ]s during ]]]


Octopuses have a relatively short ], and some species live for as little as six months. Larger species, such as the ], may live for up to five years under suitable circumstances. However, reproduction is a cause of death: males can only live for a few months after mating, and females die shortly after their eggs hatch. They neglect to eat during the (roughly) one month period spent taking care of their unhatched eggs, but they do not die of starvation. Endocrine secretions from the two optic glands are the cause of genetically programmed death (and if these glands are surgically removed, the octopus may live many months beyond reproduction, until she finally starves).{{Citation needed|date=August 2010}} Octopi have a relatively short ], and some species live for as little as six months. Larger species, such as the ], may live for up to five years under suitable circumstances. However, reproduction is a cause of death: males can only live for a few months after mating, and females die shortly after their eggs hatch. They neglect to eat during the (roughly) one month period spent taking care of their unhatched eggs, but they do not die of starvation. Endocrine secretions from the two optic glands are the cause of genetically programmed death (and if these glands are surgically removed, the octopus may live many months beyond reproduction, until she finally starves).{{Citation needed|date=August 2010}}


]'', a finned octopus of the suborder ]]] ]'', a finned octopus of the suborder ]]]


Octopuses have three ]s. Two pump blood through each of the two ]s, while the third pumps blood through the body. Octopus ] contains the ]-rich protein ] for transporting ]. Although less efficient under ] than the ]-rich ] of vertebrates, in cold conditions with low oxygen pressure, hemocyanin oxygen transportation is more efficient than hemoglobin oxygen transportation. The hemocyanin is dissolved in the ] instead of being carried within ]s and gives the blood a bluish color. Octopuses draw water into their mantle cavity where it passes through its gills. As ], octopuses have gills that are finely divided and vascularized outgrowths of either the outer or the inner body surface. Octopi have three ]s. Two pump blood through each of the two ]s, while the third pumps blood through the body. Octopus ] contains the ]-rich protein ] for transporting ]. Although less efficient under ] than the ]-rich ] of vertebrates, in cold conditions with low oxygen pressure, hemocyanin oxygen transportation is more efficient than hemoglobin oxygen transportation. The hemocyanin is dissolved in the ] instead of being carried within ]s and gives the blood a bluish color. Octopi draw water into their mantle cavity where it passes through its gills. As ], octopi have gills that are finely divided and vascularized outgrowths of either the outer or the inner body surface.


==== Intelligence ==== ==== Intelligence ====
{{Main|Cephalopod intelligence}} {{Main|Cephalopod intelligence}}
Octopuses are highly ], likely more so than any other order of ]s. The exact extent of their intelligence and learning capability is much debated among biologists,<ref name = "ham"/><ref name="doug">, Is the octopus really the invertebrate intellect of the sea, by Doug Stewart. In: National Wildlife. Feb/Mar 1997, vol.35 no.2. Octopi are highly ], likely more so than any other order of ]s. The exact extent of their intelligence and learning capability is much debated among biologists,<ref name = "ham"/><ref name="doug">, Is the octopus really the invertebrate intellect of the sea, by Doug Stewart. In: National Wildlife. Feb/Mar 1997, vol.35 no.2.
</ref><ref name="denizen"></ref><ref>, How Smart is the Octopus?</ref> but maze and ] experiments have shown that they do have both ] and ]. Their short lifespans limit the amount they can ultimately learn. There has been much speculation to the effect that almost all octopus behaviors are independently learned rather than instinct-based, although this remains largely unproven. They learn almost no behaviors from their parents, with whom young octopuses have very little contact. </ref><ref name="denizen"></ref><ref>, How Smart is the Octopus?</ref> but maze and ] experiments have shown that they do have both ] and ]. Their short lifespans limit the amount they can ultimately learn. There has been much speculation to the effect that almost all octopus behaviors are independently learned rather than instinct-based, although this remains largely unproven. They learn almost no behaviors from their parents, with whom young octopi have very little contact.


] ]


An octopus has a highly complex ], only part of which is localized in its ]. Two-thirds of an octopus's ]s are found in the nerve cords of its arms, which have a remarkable amount of autonomy. Octopus arms show a wide variety of complex ] actions arising on at least three different levels of the nervous system. Unlike vertebrates, the complex motor skills of octopuses in their higher brain are not organized using an internal ] of its body.<ref>Zullo L, Sumbre G, Agnisola C, Flash T, Hochner B. (2009). Nonsomatotopic organization of the higher motor centers in octopus. An octopus has a highly complex ], only part of which is localized in its ]. Two-thirds of an octopus's ]s are found in the nerve cords of its arms, which have a remarkable amount of autonomy. Octopus arms show a wide variety of complex ] actions arising on at least three different levels of the nervous system. Unlike vertebrates, the complex motor skills of octopi in their higher brain are not organized using an internal ] of its body.<ref>Zullo L, Sumbre G, Agnisola C, Flash T, Hochner B. (2009). Nonsomatotopic organization of the higher motor centers in octopus.
Curr Biol. 19(19):1632-6. PMID 19765993</ref> Some octopuses, such as the ], will move their arms in ways that emulate the movements of other ]. Curr Biol. 19(19):1632-6. PMID 19765993</ref> Some octopi, such as the ], will move their arms in ways that emulate the movements of other ].


In laboratory experiments, octopuses can be readily trained to distinguish between different shapes and patterns. They have been reported to practice ],<ref></ref> although the validity of these findings is widely contested on a number of grounds.<ref name="ham">. By Garry Hamilton.</ref><ref name="doug"/> Octopuses have also been observed in what some have described as ]: repeatedly releasing bottles or toys into a circular current in their aquariums and then catching them.<ref>. By Dr. Jennifer Mather, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, ] and Roland C. Anderson, The ].</ref> Octopuses often break out of their aquariums and sometimes into others in search of food. They have even boarded ]s and opened holds to eat crabs.<ref name="denizen" /> In laboratory experiments, octopi can be readily trained to distinguish between different shapes and patterns. They have been reported to practice ],<ref></ref> although the validity of these findings is widely contested on a number of grounds.<ref name="ham">. By Garry Hamilton.</ref><ref name="doug"/> Octopi have also been observed in what some have described as ]: repeatedly releasing bottles or toys into a circular current in their aquariums and then catching them.<ref>. By Dr. Jennifer Mather, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, ] and Roland C. Anderson, The ].</ref> Octopi often break out of their aquariums and sometimes into others in search of food. They have even boarded ]s and opened holds to eat crabs.<ref name="denizen" />


In some countries, octopuses are on the list of ] on which surgery may not be performed without ]. In the UK, cephalopods such as octopuses are regarded as ''honorary vertebrates'' under the ] and other ] legislation, extending to them protections not normally afforded to invertebrates.<ref></ref> In some countries, octopi are on the list of ] on which surgery may not be performed without ]. In the UK, cephalopods such as octopi are regarded as ''honorary vertebrates'' under the ] and other ] legislation, extending to them protections not normally afforded to invertebrates.<ref></ref>


The octopus is the only invertebrate which has been conclusively shown to ]. At least four specimens of the Veined Octopus ('']'') have been witnessed retrieving discarded ] shells, manipulating them, and then reassembling them to use as shelter. This discovery was documented in the journal '']'' and has also been caught on video.<ref>{{cite news| url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/sci/tech/8408233.stm | work=BBC News | title=Octopus snatches coconut and runs | date=2009-12-14 | accessdate=2010-05-20}}</ref><ref>http://www.edutube.org/video/coconut-shelter-evidence-tool-use-octopuses</ref> The octopus is the only invertebrate which has been conclusively shown to ]. At least four specimens of the Veined Octopus ('']'') have been witnessed retrieving discarded ] shells, manipulating them, and then reassembling them to use as shelter. This discovery was documented in the journal '']'' and has also been caught on video.<ref>{{cite news| url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/sci/tech/8408233.stm | work=BBC News | title=Octopus snatches coconut and runs | date=2009-12-14 | accessdate=2010-05-20}}</ref><ref>http://www.edutube.org/video/coconut-shelter-evidence-tool-use-octopi</ref>


=== Defense === === Defense ===
] (''Hapalochlaena lunulata'')]] ] (''Hapalochlaena lunulata'')]]
An octopus's main (primary) defense is to hide, either not to be seen at all, or not to be detected as an octopus.<ref name=behaviour>Hanlon, R.T. & J.B. Messenger 1996. ''Cephalopod Behaviour''. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.</ref> Octopuses have several secondary defenses (defenses they use once they have been seen by a predator). The most common secondary defense is fast escape. Other defenses include the use of ]s, ], and ]. An octopus's main (primary) defense is to hide, either not to be seen at all, or not to be detected as an octopus.<ref name=behaviour>Hanlon, R.T. & J.B. Messenger 1996. ''Cephalopod Behaviour''. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.</ref> Octopi have several secondary defenses (defenses they use once they have been seen by a predator). The most common secondary defense is fast escape. Other defenses include the use of ]s, ], and ].


Most octopuses can eject a thick blackish ] in a large cloud to aid in escaping from predators. The main coloring agent of the ink is melanin, which is the same chemical that gives humans their hair and ]. This ink cloud is thought to reduce the efficiency of olfactory organs, which would aid an octopus's evasion from predators that employ ] for hunting, such as ]s. Ink clouds of some species might serve as pseudomorphs, or decoys that the predator attacks instead.<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Caldwell | first1 = R. L. | year = 2005 | title = An Observation of Inking Behavior Protecting Adult ''Octopus bocki'' from Predation by Green Turtle (''Chelonia mydas'') Hatchlings | url = | journal = Pacific Science | volume = 59 | issue = 1| pages = 69–72 }}</ref> Most octopi can eject a thick blackish ] in a large cloud to aid in escaping from predators. The main coloring agent of the ink is melanin, which is the same chemical that gives humans their hair and ]. This ink cloud is thought to reduce the efficiency of olfactory organs, which would aid an octopus's evasion from predators that employ ] for hunting, such as ]s. Ink clouds of some species might serve as pseudomorphs, or decoys that the predator attacks instead.<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Caldwell | first1 = R. L. | year = 2005 | title = An Observation of Inking Behavior Protecting Adult ''Octopus bocki'' from Predation by Green Turtle (''Chelonia mydas'') Hatchlings | url = | journal = Pacific Science | volume = 59 | issue = 1| pages = 69–72 }}</ref>


]'' travels with shells it has collected for protection]] ]'' travels with shells it has collected for protection]]


An octopus's camouflage is aided by certain specialized skin cells which can change the apparent color, opacity, and reflectiveness of the epidermis. ]s contain yellow, orange, red, brown, or black pigments; most species have three of these colors, while some have two or four. Other color-changing cells are reflective ]s, and ]s (white).<ref>{{cite web | url = http://www.dnr.sc.gov/marine/sertc/species_month.htm | title = Tales from the Cryptic: The Common Atlantic Octopus | accessdate = 2006-07-27 | author = Meyers, Nadia|publisher = Southeastern Regional Taxonomic Center}}</ref> This color-changing ability can also be used to communicate with or warn other octopi. The very venomous ] becomes bright yellow with blue rings when it is provoked. Octopuses can use muscles in the skin to change the texture of their mantle to achieve a greater camouflage. In some species the mantle can take on the spiky appearance of seaweed, or the scraggly, bumpy texture of a rock, among other disguises. However in some species skin anatomy is limited to relatively patternless shades of one color, and limited skin texture. It is thought that octopuses that are day-active and/or live in complex habitats such as coral reefs have evolved more complex skin than their nocturnal and/or sand-dwelling relatives.<ref name=behaviour /> An octopus's camouflage is aided by certain specialized skin cells which can change the apparent color, opacity, and reflectiveness of the epidermis. ]s contain yellow, orange, red, brown, or black pigments; most species have three of these colors, while some have two or four. Other color-changing cells are reflective ]s, and ]s (white).<ref>{{cite web | url = http://www.dnr.sc.gov/marine/sertc/species_month.htm | title = Tales from the Cryptic: The Common Atlantic Octopus | accessdate = 2006-07-27 | author = Meyers, Nadia|publisher = Southeastern Regional Taxonomic Center}}</ref> This color-changing ability can also be used to communicate with or warn other octopi. The very venomous ] becomes bright yellow with blue rings when it is provoked. Octopi can use muscles in the skin to change the texture of their mantle to achieve a greater camouflage. In some species the mantle can take on the spiky appearance of seaweed, or the scraggly, bumpy texture of a rock, among other disguises. However in some species skin anatomy is limited to relatively patternless shades of one color, and limited skin texture. It is thought that octopi that are day-active and/or live in complex habitats such as coral reefs have evolved more complex skin than their nocturnal and/or sand-dwelling relatives.<ref name=behaviour />


When under attack, some octopuses can perform arm ], in a similar manner to the way ]s and other ]s detach their tails. The crawling arm serves as a distraction to would-be predators. When under attack, some octopi can perform arm ], in a similar manner to the way ]s and other ]s detach their tails. The crawling arm serves as a distraction to would-be predators.


A few species, such as the ], have a fourth defense mechanism. They can combine their highly flexible bodies with their color changing ability to accurately mimic other, more dangerous animals such as ], ], and ]s.<ref>Norman, M.D., J. Finn & T. Tregenza (2001). {{PDFlink||312&nbsp;KB}} ''Proceedings of the Royal Society'' '''268''': 1755–1758.</ref><ref>Norman, M.D. & F.G.Hochberg (2005). The "Mimic Octopus" (''Thaumoctopus mimicus'' n. gen. et sp.), a new octopus from the tropical Indo-West Pacific (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae). ''Molluscan Research'' '''25''': 57–70. </ref> A few species, such as the ], have a fourth defense mechanism. They can combine their highly flexible bodies with their color changing ability to accurately mimic other, more dangerous animals such as ], ], and ]s.<ref>Norman, M.D., J. Finn & T. Tregenza (2001). {{PDFlink||312&nbsp;KB}} ''Proceedings of the Royal Society'' '''268''': 1755–1758.</ref><ref>Norman, M.D. & F.G.Hochberg (2005). The "Mimic Octopus" (''Thaumoctopus mimicus'' n. gen. et sp.), a new octopus from the tropical Indo-West Pacific (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae). ''Molluscan Research'' '''25''': 57–70. </ref>
<!--=== Diet === <!--=== Diet ===
Octopuses usually eat small crabs and scallops, plus some snails, fish, turtles, crustaceans (like shrimp), and other octopuses. They catch prey with their arms, then kill it by biting it with their tough beak, paralysing the prey with a nerve poison, and softening the flesh. They then suck out the flesh. Octopuses hunt mostly at night. Only the Australian Blue-ringed octopus has a poison strong enough to kill a person.<ref>http://www.enchantedlearning.com/subjects/invertebrates/octopus/Octopuscoloring.shtml</ref>--> Octopi usually eat small crabs and scallops, plus some snails, fish, turtles, crustaceans (like shrimp), and other octopi. They catch prey with their arms, then kill it by biting it with their tough beak, paralysing the prey with a nerve poison, and softening the flesh. They then suck out the flesh. Octopi hunt mostly at night. Only the Australian Blue-ringed octopus has a poison strong enough to kill a person.<ref>http://www.enchantedlearning.com/subjects/invertebrates/octopus/Octopuscoloring.shtml</ref>-->


=== Reproduction === === Reproduction ===
When octopuses reproduce, males use a specialized arm called a ] to insert ]s (packets of sperm) into the female's mantle cavity. The hectocotylus in ] octopuses is usually the third right arm. Males die within a few months of mating. In some species, the female octopus can keep the sperm alive inside her for weeks until her eggs are mature. After they have been fertilized, the female lays about 200,000 eggs (this figure dramatically varies between families, genera, species and also individuals). The female hangs these eggs in strings from the ceiling of her lair, or individually attaches them to the ] depending on the species. The female cares for the eggs, guarding them against predators, and gently blowing currents of water over them so that they get enough oxygen. The female does not hunt during the roughly one-month period spent taking care of the unhatched eggs and may ingest some of her own arms for sustenance. At around the time the eggs hatch, the mother leaves the lair and is too weak to defend herself from predators like cod, often succumbing to their attacks. The young larval octopuses spend a period of time drifting in clouds of ], where they feed on ]s, larval ]s and larval ] until they are ready to descend to the ocean bottom, where the cycle repeats. This is a dangerous time for the larval octopuses; in the plankton cloud they are vulnerable to plankton eaters. In some deeper dwelling species, the young do not go through this period.{{Citation needed|date=October 2009}} When octopi reproduce, males use a specialized arm called a ] to insert ]s (packets of sperm) into the female's mantle cavity. The hectocotylus in ] octopi is usually the third right arm. Males die within a few months of mating. In some species, the female octopus can keep the sperm alive inside her for weeks until her eggs are mature. After they have been fertilized, the female lays about 200,000 eggs (this figure dramatically varies between families, genera, species and also individuals). The female hangs these eggs in strings from the ceiling of her lair, or individually attaches them to the ] depending on the species. The female cares for the eggs, guarding them against predators, and gently blowing currents of water over them so that they get enough oxygen. The female does not hunt during the roughly one-month period spent taking care of the unhatched eggs and may ingest some of her own arms for sustenance. At around the time the eggs hatch, the mother leaves the lair and is too weak to defend herself from predators like cod, often succumbing to their attacks. The young larval octopi spend a period of time drifting in clouds of ], where they feed on ]s, larval ]s and larval ] until they are ready to descend to the ocean bottom, where the cycle repeats. This is a dangerous time for the larval octopi; in the plankton cloud they are vulnerable to plankton eaters. In some deeper dwelling species, the young do not go through this period.{{Citation needed|date=October 2009}}


=== Sensation === === Sensation ===
]'']] ]'']]
Octopuses have keen eyesight. Octopuses, like other cephalopods, can distinguish the ] of light. ] appears to vary from species to species, being present in ''Octopus aegina'' but absent in ''Octopus vulgaris''.<ref>Kawamura, G., et al. (2001). {{PDFlink||453&nbsp;KB}} . ''Nippon Suisan Gakkashi'' 67(1): 35–39.</ref> Attached to the brain are two special organs, called ]s, that allow the octopus to sense the orientation of its body relative to horizontal. An ] response keeps the octopus's eyes oriented so that the pupil slit is always horizontal. Octopi have keen eyesight. Octopi, like other cephalopods, can distinguish the ] of light. ] appears to vary from species to species, being present in ''Octopus aegina'' but absent in ''Octopus vulgaris''.<ref>Kawamura, G., et al. (2001). {{PDFlink||453&nbsp;KB}} . ''Nippon Suisan Gakkashi'' 67(1): 35–39.</ref> Attached to the brain are two special organs, called ]s, that allow the octopus to sense the orientation of its body relative to horizontal. An ] response keeps the octopus's eyes oriented so that the pupil slit is always horizontal.


Octopuses also have an excellent ]. An octopus's suction cups are equipped with ] so that the octopus can ] what it is touching. The arms contain ] sensors so that the octopus knows whether its arms are stretched out. However, the octopus has a very poor ] sense. The tension receptors are not sufficient for the octopus brain to determine the position of the octopus's body or arms. (It is not clear that the octopus brain would be capable of processing the large amount of information that this would require; the flexibility of an octopus's arms is much greater than that of the limbs of vertebrates, which devote large areas of ] to the processing of proprioceptive inputs.) As a result, the octopus does not possess ]; that is, it does not form a ] of the overall shape of the object it is handling. It can detect local texture variations, but cannot integrate the information into a larger picture.<ref name="wells">Wells. Martin John. ''Octopus: physiology and behaviour of an advanced invertebrate''. London : ] ; New York : distributed in the U.S.A. by Halsted Press, 1978.</ref> Octopi also have an excellent ]. An octopus's suction cups are equipped with ] so that the octopus can ] what it is touching. The arms contain ] sensors so that the octopus knows whether its arms are stretched out. However, the octopus has a very poor ] sense. The tension receptors are not sufficient for the octopus brain to determine the position of the octopus's body or arms. (It is not clear that the octopus brain would be capable of processing the large amount of information that this would require; the flexibility of an octopus's arms is much greater than that of the limbs of vertebrates, which devote large areas of ] to the processing of proprioceptive inputs.) As a result, the octopus does not possess ]; that is, it does not form a ] of the overall shape of the object it is handling. It can detect local texture variations, but cannot integrate the information into a larger picture.<ref name="wells">Wells. Martin John. ''Octopus: physiology and behaviour of an advanced invertebrate''. London : ] ; New York : distributed in the U.S.A. by Halsted Press, 1978.</ref>


The neurological autonomy of the arms means that the octopus has great difficulty learning about the detailed effects of its motions. The brain may issue a high-level command to the arms, but the nerve cords in the arms execute the details. There is no neurological path for the brain to receive feedback about just how its command was executed by the arms; the only way it knows just what motions were made is by observing the arms visually.<ref name="wells"/> The neurological autonomy of the arms means that the octopus has great difficulty learning about the detailed effects of its motions. The brain may issue a high-level command to the arms, but the nerve cords in the arms execute the details. There is no neurological path for the brain to receive feedback about just how its command was executed by the arms; the only way it knows just what motions were made is by observing the arms visually.<ref name="wells"/>


Octopuses appear to have limited hearing.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/earth/hi/earth_news/newsid_8095000/8095977.stm|title=The cephalopods can hear you |publisher=]|date=15 June 2009|author=Matt Walker|accessdate = 2010-04-02}}</ref> Octopi appear to have limited hearing.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/earth/hi/earth_news/newsid_8095000/8095977.stm|title=The cephalopods can hear you |publisher=]|date=15 June 2009|author=Matt Walker|accessdate = 2010-04-02}}</ref>


] ]


=== Locomotion === === Locomotion ===
] ]
Octopuses move about by crawling or swimming. Their main means of slow travel is crawling, with some swimming. Jet propulsion is their fastest means of locomotion, followed by swimming and walking.<ref name="biologists2006"></ref> Octopi move about by crawling or swimming. Their main means of slow travel is crawling, with some swimming. Jet propulsion is their fastest means of locomotion, followed by swimming and walking.<ref name="biologists2006"></ref>


They crawl by walking on their arms, usually on many at once, on both solid and soft surfaces, while supported in water. In 2005 it was reported that some octopuses ('']'' and '']'' under current taxonomy) can walk on two arms, while at the same time resembling plant matter.<ref></ref> This form of locomotion allows these octopuses to move quickly away from a potential predator while possibly not triggering that predator's search image for octopus (food).<ref name="biologists2006"/> They crawl by walking on their arms, usually on many at once, on both solid and soft surfaces, while supported in water. In 2005 it was reported that some octopi ('']'' and '']'' under current taxonomy) can walk on two arms, while at the same time resembling plant matter.<ref></ref> This form of locomotion allows these octopi to move quickly away from a potential predator while possibly not triggering that predator's search image for octopus (food).<ref name="biologists2006"/>


Octopuses swim by expelling a jet of water from a contractile ], and aiming it via a muscular ]. Octopi swim by expelling a jet of water from a contractile ], and aiming it via a muscular ].


=== Size === === Size ===

Revision as of 22:29, 19 October 2010

This article is about the order of cephalopod. For other uses, see Octopus (disambiguation).

Octopus
The Common Octopus, Octopus vulgaris.
Scientific classification Edit this classification
Domain: Eukaryota
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Mollusca
Class: Cephalopoda
Superorder: Octopodiformes
Order: Octopoda
Leach, 1818
Suborders
Synonyms
  • Octopoida
    Leach, 1817

The octopus is a cephalopod mollusc in the order Octopoda. Octopuses have two eyes and four pairs of arms, and like other cephalopods they are bilaterally symmetric. An octopus has a hard beak, with its mouth at the center point of the arms. Most octopuses have no internal or external skeleton, allowing them to squeeze through tight places. Octopuses are highly intelligent, and are among the most intelligent of all invertebrates.

The octopus inhabits many diverse regions of the ocean, especially coral reefs. For defense against predators, they hide, flee quickly, expel ink, or use color-changing camouflage. An octopus trails its eight arms behind it as it swims. All octopuses are venomous, but only the small blue-ringed octopuses are known to be deadly to humans.

In the larger sense, there are around 300 recognized octopus species, which is over one-third of the total number of known cephalopod species. The term octopus may also be used to refer only to those creatures in the genus Octopus.

Biology

A Common Octopus (Octopus vulgaris)

Octopi are characterized by their eight arms, usually bearing suction cups. The arms of octopi are often distinguished from the pair of feeding tentacles found in squid and cuttlefish. Both types of limbs are muscular hydrostats. Unlike most other cephalopods, the majority of octopi – those in the suborder most commonly known, Incirrina – have almost entirely soft bodies with no internal skeleton. They have neither a protective outer shell like the nautilus, nor any vestige of an internal shell or bones, like cuttlefish or squid. A beak, similar in shape to a parrot's beak, is the only hard part of their body. This enables them to squeeze through very narrow slits between underwater rocks, which is very helpful when they are fleeing from morays or other predatory fish. The octopi in the less familiar Cirrina suborder have two fins and an internal shell, generally reducing their ability to squeeze into small spaces.

An octopus moving between tide pools during low tide

Octopi have a relatively short life expectancy, and some species live for as little as six months. Larger species, such as the North Pacific Giant Octopus, may live for up to five years under suitable circumstances. However, reproduction is a cause of death: males can only live for a few months after mating, and females die shortly after their eggs hatch. They neglect to eat during the (roughly) one month period spent taking care of their unhatched eggs, but they do not die of starvation. Endocrine secretions from the two optic glands are the cause of genetically programmed death (and if these glands are surgically removed, the octopus may live many months beyond reproduction, until she finally starves).

Grimpoteuthis discoveryi, a finned octopus of the suborder Cirrina

Octopi have three hearts. Two pump blood through each of the two gills, while the third pumps blood through the body. Octopus blood contains the copper-rich protein hemocyanin for transporting oxygen. Although less efficient under normal conditions than the iron-rich hemoglobin of vertebrates, in cold conditions with low oxygen pressure, hemocyanin oxygen transportation is more efficient than hemoglobin oxygen transportation. The hemocyanin is dissolved in the plasma instead of being carried within red blood cells and gives the blood a bluish color. Octopi draw water into their mantle cavity where it passes through its gills. As mollusks, octopi have gills that are finely divided and vascularized outgrowths of either the outer or the inner body surface.

Intelligence

Main article: Cephalopod intelligence

Octopi are highly intelligent, likely more so than any other order of invertebrates. The exact extent of their intelligence and learning capability is much debated among biologists, but maze and problem-solving experiments have shown that they do have both short- and long-term memory. Their short lifespans limit the amount they can ultimately learn. There has been much speculation to the effect that almost all octopus behaviors are independently learned rather than instinct-based, although this remains largely unproven. They learn almost no behaviors from their parents, with whom young octopi have very little contact.

An octopus opening a container with a screw cap

An octopus has a highly complex nervous system, only part of which is localized in its brain. Two-thirds of an octopus's neurons are found in the nerve cords of its arms, which have a remarkable amount of autonomy. Octopus arms show a wide variety of complex reflex actions arising on at least three different levels of the nervous system. Unlike vertebrates, the complex motor skills of octopi in their higher brain are not organized using an internal somatotopic map of its body. Some octopi, such as the mimic octopus, will move their arms in ways that emulate the movements of other sea creatures.

In laboratory experiments, octopi can be readily trained to distinguish between different shapes and patterns. They have been reported to practice observational learning, although the validity of these findings is widely contested on a number of grounds. Octopi have also been observed in what some have described as play: repeatedly releasing bottles or toys into a circular current in their aquariums and then catching them. Octopi often break out of their aquariums and sometimes into others in search of food. They have even boarded fishing boats and opened holds to eat crabs.

In some countries, octopi are on the list of experimental animals on which surgery may not be performed without anesthesia. In the UK, cephalopods such as octopi are regarded as honorary vertebrates under the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and other cruelty to animals legislation, extending to them protections not normally afforded to invertebrates.

The octopus is the only invertebrate which has been conclusively shown to use tools. At least four specimens of the Veined Octopus (Amphioctopus marginatus) have been witnessed retrieving discarded coconut shells, manipulating them, and then reassembling them to use as shelter. This discovery was documented in the journal Current Biology and has also been caught on video.

Defense

Greater Blue-ringed Octopus (Hapalochlaena lunulata)

An octopus's main (primary) defense is to hide, either not to be seen at all, or not to be detected as an octopus. Octopi have several secondary defenses (defenses they use once they have been seen by a predator). The most common secondary defense is fast escape. Other defenses include the use of ink sacs, camouflage, and autotomising limbs.

Most octopi can eject a thick blackish ink in a large cloud to aid in escaping from predators. The main coloring agent of the ink is melanin, which is the same chemical that gives humans their hair and skin color. This ink cloud is thought to reduce the efficiency of olfactory organs, which would aid an octopus's evasion from predators that employ smell for hunting, such as sharks. Ink clouds of some species might serve as pseudomorphs, or decoys that the predator attacks instead.

Amphioctopus marginatus travels with shells it has collected for protection

An octopus's camouflage is aided by certain specialized skin cells which can change the apparent color, opacity, and reflectiveness of the epidermis. Chromatophores contain yellow, orange, red, brown, or black pigments; most species have three of these colors, while some have two or four. Other color-changing cells are reflective iridophores, and leucophores (white). This color-changing ability can also be used to communicate with or warn other octopi. The very venomous blue-ringed octopus becomes bright yellow with blue rings when it is provoked. Octopi can use muscles in the skin to change the texture of their mantle to achieve a greater camouflage. In some species the mantle can take on the spiky appearance of seaweed, or the scraggly, bumpy texture of a rock, among other disguises. However in some species skin anatomy is limited to relatively patternless shades of one color, and limited skin texture. It is thought that octopi that are day-active and/or live in complex habitats such as coral reefs have evolved more complex skin than their nocturnal and/or sand-dwelling relatives.

When under attack, some octopi can perform arm autotomy, in a similar manner to the way skinks and other lizards detach their tails. The crawling arm serves as a distraction to would-be predators.

A few species, such as the Mimic Octopus, have a fourth defense mechanism. They can combine their highly flexible bodies with their color changing ability to accurately mimic other, more dangerous animals such as lionfish, sea snakes, and eels.

Reproduction

When octopi reproduce, males use a specialized arm called a hectocotylus to insert spermatophores (packets of sperm) into the female's mantle cavity. The hectocotylus in benthic octopi is usually the third right arm. Males die within a few months of mating. In some species, the female octopus can keep the sperm alive inside her for weeks until her eggs are mature. After they have been fertilized, the female lays about 200,000 eggs (this figure dramatically varies between families, genera, species and also individuals). The female hangs these eggs in strings from the ceiling of her lair, or individually attaches them to the substrate depending on the species. The female cares for the eggs, guarding them against predators, and gently blowing currents of water over them so that they get enough oxygen. The female does not hunt during the roughly one-month period spent taking care of the unhatched eggs and may ingest some of her own arms for sustenance. At around the time the eggs hatch, the mother leaves the lair and is too weak to defend herself from predators like cod, often succumbing to their attacks. The young larval octopi spend a period of time drifting in clouds of plankton, where they feed on copepods, larval crabs and larval starfish until they are ready to descend to the ocean bottom, where the cycle repeats. This is a dangerous time for the larval octopi; in the plankton cloud they are vulnerable to plankton eaters. In some deeper dwelling species, the young do not go through this period.

Sensation

Eye of Octopus vulgaris

Octopi have keen eyesight. Octopi, like other cephalopods, can distinguish the polarization of light. Color vision appears to vary from species to species, being present in Octopus aegina but absent in Octopus vulgaris. Attached to the brain are two special organs, called statocysts, that allow the octopus to sense the orientation of its body relative to horizontal. An autonomic response keeps the octopus's eyes oriented so that the pupil slit is always horizontal.

Octopi also have an excellent sense of touch. An octopus's suction cups are equipped with chemoreceptors so that the octopus can taste what it is touching. The arms contain tension sensors so that the octopus knows whether its arms are stretched out. However, the octopus has a very poor proprioceptive sense. The tension receptors are not sufficient for the octopus brain to determine the position of the octopus's body or arms. (It is not clear that the octopus brain would be capable of processing the large amount of information that this would require; the flexibility of an octopus's arms is much greater than that of the limbs of vertebrates, which devote large areas of cerebral cortex to the processing of proprioceptive inputs.) As a result, the octopus does not possess stereognosis; that is, it does not form a mental image of the overall shape of the object it is handling. It can detect local texture variations, but cannot integrate the information into a larger picture.

The neurological autonomy of the arms means that the octopus has great difficulty learning about the detailed effects of its motions. The brain may issue a high-level command to the arms, but the nerve cords in the arms execute the details. There is no neurological path for the brain to receive feedback about just how its command was executed by the arms; the only way it knows just what motions were made is by observing the arms visually.

Octopi appear to have limited hearing.

Octopi swim headfirst, with arms trailing behind

Locomotion

Video of an octopus in its natural habitat

Octopi move about by crawling or swimming. Their main means of slow travel is crawling, with some swimming. Jet propulsion is their fastest means of locomotion, followed by swimming and walking.

They crawl by walking on their arms, usually on many at once, on both solid and soft surfaces, while supported in water. In 2005 it was reported that some octopi (Adopus aculeatus and Amphioctopus marginatus under current taxonomy) can walk on two arms, while at the same time resembling plant matter. This form of locomotion allows these octopi to move quickly away from a potential predator while possibly not triggering that predator's search image for octopus (food).

Octopi swim by expelling a jet of water from a contractile mantle, and aiming it via a muscular siphon.

Size

See also: Cephalopod size
An adult North Pacific Giant Octopus, Enteroctopus dofleini

The North Pacific Giant Octopus, Enteroctopus dofleini, is often cited as the largest octopus species. Adults usually weigh around 15 kg (33 lb), with an arm span of up to 4.3 m (14 ft). The largest specimen of this species to be scientifically documented was an animal with a live mass of 71 kg (156.5 lb). The alternative contender is the Seven-arm Octopus, Haliphron atlanticus, based on a 61 kg (134 lb) carcass estimated to have a live mass of 75 kg (165 lb). However, there are a number of questionable size records that would suggest E. dofleini is the largest of all octopus species by a considerable margin; one such record is of a specimen weighing 272 kg (600 lb) and having an arm span of 9 m (30 ft).

Terminology

The term octopus, Template:Pron-en, is from Greek Template:Polytonic (oktapous), "eight-footed", with plural forms: octopuses /ˈɒktəpʊsɪz/, octopi /ˈɒktəpaɪ/, or octopodes /ɒkˈtɒpədiːz/. Currently, octopuses is the most common form in both the US and the UK; octopodes is rare, and octopi is often objectionable.

The plural form octopi is often defined as a hypercorrection. The Oxford English Dictionary (2008 Draft Revision) lists octopuses, octopi and octopodes (in that order); it labels octopodes "rare", although the correct Greek plural form, and notes that octopi derives from the "apprehension" that octōpūs is a second declension Latin noun, though it is not. It is a Latinization of Greek third-declension masculine oktṓpous (Template:Polytonic, 'eight-foot'), plural oktṓpodes (Template:Polytonic). If the word were native to Latin, it would be octōpēs, plural octōpedes, after the pattern of pēs ('foot'), plural pedēs, analogous to "centipede". The actual Latin word for octopus and other similar species is polypus, from Greek polýpous (Template:Polytonic, 'many-foot'); usually the inaccurate plural polypī is used instead of polypodēs.

In modern Greek, the word is khtapódi (Template:Polytonic), plural khtapódia (Template:Polytonic), from Medieval oktapódion (Template:Polytonic), equivalent to Classical oktápous (Template:Polytonic), variant of oktṓpous.

Chambers 21st Century Dictionary and the Compact Oxford Dictionary list only octopuses, although the latter notes that octopodes is "still occasionally used"; the British National Corpus has 29 instances of octopuses, 11 of octopi and 4 of octopodes. Merriam-Webster 11th Collegiate Dictionary lists octopuses and octopi, in that order; Webster's New World College Dictionary lists octopuses, octopi and octopodes (in that order).

Fowler's Modern English Usage states that "the only acceptable plural in English is octopuses," and that octopi is misconceived and octopodes pedantic.

The term octopod (plural octopods or octopodes) is taken from the taxonomic order Octopoda but has no classical equivalent. The collective form octopus is usually reserved for animals consumed for food.

Relationship to humans

Left: Vase from a Mycenaean Greek cemetery at Prosymna, Argos, grave 2, c. 1500 BCE
Centre: Moche Octopus (200 AD), Larco Museum Collection, Lima, Peru
Right: Ancient Greek black-figure amphora, 530–520 BC. On the left, a hoplite with an octopus image on his shield. Staatliche Antikensammlungen, Munich, Germany

Ancient peoples of the Mediterranean were aware of the octopus, as evidenced by certain artworks and designs of prehistory. For example, a stone carving found in the archaeological recovery from Bronze Age Minoan Crete at Knossos has a depiction of a fisherman carrying an octopus.

Octopuses were often depicted in the art of the Moche people of ancient Peru, who worshipped the sea and its animals.

In mythology

The Hawaiian creation myth relates that the present cosmos is only the last of a series, having arisen in stages from the wreck of the previous universe. In this account, the octopus is the lone survivor of the previous, alien universe.

In literature

The octopus has a significant role in Victor Hugo's book Travailleurs de la mer (Toilers of the Sea).

Photo of dozens of octopus in metal bins
Octopus at Tsukiji fish market

As food

Humans eat octopus in many cultures. The arms and sometimes other body parts are prepared in various ways, often varying by species.

Octopus is a common ingredient in Japanese cuisine, including sushi, takoyaki, and Akashiyaki. Some small species are sometimes eaten alive as a novelty food. Similarly, a live octopus may be sliced up and the legs eaten while still squirming, which continues for some minutes.

Photo of captured octopus and polespear
Octopuses are "tickled" out of their holes‎ in the Hawaiian Islands with 3-pronged polespears

Octopus is eaten regularly in Hawaii, since many popular dishes are Asian in origin. Locally known by their Hawaiian or Japanese names ("he'e" and "tako" respectively), octopus is also a popular fish bait.

Octopus is a common food in Mediterranean cuisine and Portuguese cuisine. In Galicia, polbo á feira (market fair style octopus) is a local delicacy. Restaurants which specialize or serve this dish are known as pulperías. On the Tunisian island of Djerba, local people catch octopuses by taking advantage of the animals' habit of hiding in safe places during the night. In the evening they put grey ceramic pots on the sea bed. The morning of the following day they check them for octopuses that sheltered there. A common scene in the Greek islands is octopuses hanging in the sunlight from a rope, just like laundry from a clothesline. They are often caught by spear fishing close to the shore. The fisherman brings his prey to land and tenderizes the flesh by pounding the carcass against a stone surface. Thus treated they are hung out to dry, and later will be served grilled either hot, or chilled in a salad. They are considered a superb meze, especially alongside ouzo.

According to the USDA Nutrient Database (2007), cooked octopus contains approximately 139 calories per three ounce portion, and is a source of vitamin B3, B12, potassium, phosphorus, and selenium.

Care must be taken to boil the octopus properly, to rid it of slime, smell, and residual ink.

As pets

Though octopuses can be difficult to keep in captivity, some people keep them as pets. Octopuses often escape even from supposedly secure tanks, due to their problem-solving skills, mobility and lack of rigid structure.

The variation in size and life span among octopus species makes it difficult to know how long a new specimen can naturally be expected to live. That is, a small octopus may be just born or may be an adult, depending on its species. By selecting a well-known species, such as the California Two-spot Octopus, one can choose a small octopus (around the size of a tennis ball) and be confident that it is young with a full life ahead of it.

Octopuses are also quite strong for their size. Octopuses kept as pets have been known to open the covers of their aquariums and survive for a time in the air in order to get to a nearby feeder tank and gorge themselves on the fish there. Large octopuses have also been known to catch and kill some species of sharks.

Classification

Cirrothauma murrayi
Amphitretus pelagicus

See also

Template:Misplaced Pages-Books

References

  1. ITIS Report: Octopoda Leach, 1818
  2. Helsinki.fi, Mikko's Phylogeny Archive: Coleoidea – Recent cephalopods
  3. Unimelb.edu.au, Tentacles of venom: new study reveals all octopuses are venomous, University of Melbourne, Media Release, Wednesday 15 April 2009
  4. Norman, M. 2000. Cephalopods: A World Guide. ConchBooks, Hackenheim. p. 15. "There is some confusion around the terms arms versus tentacles. The numerous limbs of nautiluses are called tentacles. The ring of eight limbs around the mouth in cuttlefish, squids and octopi are called arms. Cuttlefish and squid also have a pair of specialized limbs attached between the bases of the third and fourth arm pairs . These are known as feeding tentacles and are used to shoot out and grab prey."
  5. ^ What is this octopus thinking?. By Garry Hamilton.
  6. ^ NFW.org?, Is the octopus really the invertebrate intellect of the sea, by Doug Stewart. In: National Wildlife. Feb/Mar 1997, vol.35 no.2.
  7. ^ Giant Octopus—Mighty but Secretive Denizen of the Deep
  8. Slate.com, How Smart is the Octopus?
  9. Zullo L, Sumbre G, Agnisola C, Flash T, Hochner B. (2009). Nonsomatotopic organization of the higher motor centers in octopus. Curr Biol. 19(19):1632-6. PMID 19765993
  10. Octopus intelligence: jar opening
  11. What behavior can we expect of octopi?. By Dr. Jennifer Mather, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge and Roland C. Anderson, The Seattle Aquarium.
  12. United Kingdom Animals (Scientific Procedures) act of 1986
  13. "Octopus snatches coconut and runs". BBC News. 2009-12-14. Retrieved 2010-05-20.
  14. http://www.edutube.org/video/coconut-shelter-evidence-tool-use-octopi
  15. ^ Hanlon, R.T. & J.B. Messenger 1996. Cephalopod Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  16. Caldwell, R. L. (2005). "An Observation of Inking Behavior Protecting Adult Octopus bocki from Predation by Green Turtle (Chelonia mydas) Hatchlings". Pacific Science. 59 (1): 69–72.
  17. Meyers, Nadia. "Tales from the Cryptic: The Common Atlantic Octopus". Southeastern Regional Taxonomic Center. Retrieved 2006-07-27.
  18. Norman, M.D., J. Finn & T. Tregenza (2001). Template:PDFlink Proceedings of the Royal Society 268: 1755–1758.
  19. Norman, M.D. & F.G.Hochberg (2005). The "Mimic Octopus" (Thaumoctopus mimicus n. gen. et sp.), a new octopus from the tropical Indo-West Pacific (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae). Molluscan Research 25: 57–70. Abstract
  20. Kawamura, G., et al. (2001). Template:PDFlink . Nippon Suisan Gakkashi 67(1): 35–39.
  21. ^ Wells. Martin John. Octopus: physiology and behaviour of an advanced invertebrate. London : Chapman and Hall ; New York : distributed in the U.S.A. by Halsted Press, 1978.
  22. Matt Walker (15 June 2009). "The cephalopods can hear you". BBC. Retrieved 2010-04-02.
  23. ^ Locomotion by Abdopus aculeatus, C.L. Huffard 2006
  24. Science, vol. 307, p. 1927
  25. Smithsonian National Zoological Park: Giant Pacific Octopus
  26. Cosgrove, J.A. 1987. Aspects of the Natural History of Octopus dofleini, the Giant Pacific Octopus. M.Sc. Thesis. Department of Biology, University of Victoria (Canada), 101 pp.
  27. O'Shea, S. 2004. The giant octopus Haliphron atlanticus (Mollusca : Octopoda) in New Zealand waters. New Zealand Journal of Zoology 31(1): 7–13.
  28. O'Shea, S. 2002. Haliphron atlanticus — a giant gelatinous octopus. Biodiversity Update 5: 1.
  29. Norman, M. 2000. Cephalopods: A World Guide. ConchBooks, Hackenheim. p. 214.
  30. High, W.L. (1976). "The giant Pacific octopus". U.S. National Marine Fisheries Service, Marine Fisheries Review. 38 (9): 17–22.
  31. Oktapous, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, at Perseus
  32. Scientific Latin from Greek Template:Polytonic (also Template:Polytonic) "eight-footed" > Template:Polytonic or Template:Polytonic and Template:Polytonic "foot". Cf. Modern Greek χταπόδι < οκταπόδι < οκταπόδιον < Template:Polytonic.
  33. Peters, Pam (2004). The Cambridge Guide to English Usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-62181-X, p. 388.
  34. OED.com (subscription required). Retrieved February 1, 2010.
  35. "centipede". Oxford English Dictionary (Online ed.). Oxford University Press. (Subscription or participating institution membership required.)
  36. Chambersharrap.co.uk, Retrieved October 19, 2007.
  37. Askoxford.com, Retrieved October 19, 2007.
  38. C. Michael Hogan. 2007 Knossos fieldnotes, The Modern Antiquarian
  39. Berrin, Katherine & Larco Museum. The Spirit of Ancient Peru:Treasures from the Museo Arqueológico Rafael Larco Herrera. New York: Thames and Hudson, 199 7.
  40. Dixon, Roland Burrage (1916). The Mythology of All Races. Vol. 9. Marshall Jones. p. 15. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |subtitle= ignored (help)
  41.  Chisholm, Hugh, ed. (1911). Encyclopædia Britannica (11th ed.). Cambridge University Press. {{cite encyclopedia}}: Missing or empty |title= (help)
  42. Octopus Calories And Nutrition
  43. Archived Google video of an octopus catching a shark, from The Octopus Show by Mike deGruy

External links

Principal commercial fishery species groups
Wild
Large pelagic fish
Forage fish
Demersal fish
Freshwater fish
Other wild fish
Crustaceans
Molluscs
Echinoderms
Fisheries
Atlantic cod

Lobster

Pacific oysters
Farmed
Commercial mollusks
Marine gastropods
Land and freshwater gastropods
Free-swimming marine bivalves
Infaunal bivalves
Sessile bivalves
Freshwater bivalves
Cephalopods
Techniques
List of fishing topics by subject
Edible mollusks
Bivalves
Clams
Cockles
Mussels
Oysters
Scallops
Gastropods
Abalone
Conches
Limpets
Periwinkles
Whelks
Other snails
Sea
Land
Freshwater
Inkfish
Cuttlefish
Octopus
Squid
Chitons
Related topics
Category
Categories: