Misplaced Pages

Eye–hand coordination: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editNext edit →Content deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 16:07, 16 March 2011 editTony1 (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Extended confirmed users, Pending changes reviewers, Template editors275,901 edits Do not make WP:POINTY edits: instead, raise the matter on the talk page, Anderson← Previous edit Revision as of 16:20, 16 March 2011 edit undoPmanderson (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Extended confirmed users, Pending changes reviewers62,752 edits Restore what appears to be the caseNext edit →
Line 1: Line 1:
'''Eye–hand coordination''' (also known as '''hand–eye coordination''') is the coordinated control of ] with hand movement, and the ] to guide reaching and grasping along with the use of ] to guide the eyes. It has been studied in activities as diverse as tea making, the movement of solid objects such as wooden blocks, sporting performance, ], online first-person computer gaming, and copy-typing. It is a way of performing everyday tasks and in its absence most people would be unable to carry out even the simplest of actions such as picking up a book from a table or playing a video game. '''Hand-eye coordination''' (less commonly know as '''eye-hand coordination''') is the coordinated control of ] with hand movement, and the ] to guide reaching and grasping along with the use of ] to guide the eyes. It has been studied in activities as diverse as tea making, the movement of solid objects such as wooden blocks, sporting performance, ], online first-person computer gaming, and copy-typing. It is a way of performing everyday tasks and in its absence most people would be unable to carry out even the simplest of actions such as picking up a book from a table or playing a video game.


==Behavior and kinematics== ==Behavior and kinematics==

Revision as of 16:20, 16 March 2011

Hand-eye coordination (less commonly know as eye-hand coordination) is the coordinated control of eye movement with hand movement, and the processing of visual input to guide reaching and grasping along with the use of proprioception of the hands to guide the eyes. It has been studied in activities as diverse as tea making, the movement of solid objects such as wooden blocks, sporting performance, music reading, online first-person computer gaming, and copy-typing. It is a way of performing everyday tasks and in its absence most people would be unable to carry out even the simplest of actions such as picking up a book from a table or playing a video game.

Behavior and kinematics

Neuroscientists have extensively researched human gaze behavior, with studies noting that the use of the gaze is very task specific, but that humans typically exhibit proactive control over movements in order to guide movements. Usually, the eyes fixate upon a target before the hands are used to engage in a movement, indicating that the eyes are used to provide spatial information for the hands . Furthermore, the duration that the eyes appear to be locked onto a goal for a hand movement varies, with the eyes sometimes remaining fixated until a task is completed. Other times, the eyes seem to scout ahead toward other objects of interest before the hand even grasps and manipulates the object. Conversely, humans have been shown to be able to aim eye movements toward the hand without vision, using spatial information from hand proprioception.

Eye-guided hand movements

The more dominant behavior in humans, studies have shown that when eyes and hands are used for core exercises, the eyes generally direct the movement of the hands to targets. Furthermore, the eyes provide initial information of the object, including its size, shape, and possibly grasping sites which are used to determine the force needed to be exerted by the fingertips for engaging in a given task. For shorter tasks, the eyes often shift onto another task in order to provide additional input for planning further movements. However, for more precise movements or longer duration movements, continued visual input is used to adjust for errors in movement and to create more precise movements.

For sequential tasks, it has been observed that eye gaze movements occur during important kinematic events like changing the direction of a movement, or when passing perceived landmarks . This is related to the task search oriented nature of the eyes and their relation to movement planning of the hands, and the errors between motor signal output and consequences perceived by the eyes and other senses which can be used for corrective movements. Furthermore, the eyes have been shown to have a tendency to ‘refixate’ upon a target in order to refresh the memory of its shape, or to update for changes in its shape or geometry. This has also been shown to be true in drawing tasks with relating visual input and converting it into hand movements to produce a copy of what was perceived. Finally, in high accuracy tasks, studies have shown that when acting upon greater amounts of visual stimuli, the time it takes to plan and execute movements, increases linearly as per Fitts’s Law

Hand-guided saccades

Humans have been demonstrated to be able to aim eye movements toward the hand without vision, using the sense of proprioception, with only minor errors related to the internal knowledge of limb position. It has been shown the proprioception of limbs, both active and passive movements of the limbs are result in eye saccades overshoots when the hands are being used to guide eye movements. These overshoots have been determined to result from the control of eye saccades rather than previous movements of the hands in experiments. As a result, limb based proprioception has been determined to be capable of being transformed into ocular motor coordinates to guide eye saccades, which allows for the guidance of the saccades by the hands and feet.

Neural mechanisms

The neural control of eye–hand coordination is complex because it involves every part of the central nervous system involved in vision, eye movements, touch, and hand control. This includes the eyes themselves, the cerebral cortex, subcortical structures (such as the cerebellum, basal ganglia, and brain stem), the spinal cord, and the peripheral nervous system. Some other areas involved in eye–hand coordination that have been most studied most intensely are the frontal and parietal cortex areas for the control of eye saccades and hand reach reach control. Both of these areas are believed to play a key role in eye-hand coordination and the planning of movements during tasks.

A more specific area, the parieto occipital junction, is believed to be involved in the transformation of peripheral visual input for reaching with the hands, as found via fMRI . This region in particular has been shown to have subdivisions for reach, grasp, and saccades. In additional to the parieto occipital junction, the posterior parietal cortex is believed to play an important role in relating proprioception and the transformation of motor sensory input to plan and control movement with regards to visual input .

Many of these areas, in addition to controlling saccades or reach, also show eye position signals that are required for transforming visual signals into motor commands. In addition, some of the areas involved in reach, like the medial intraparietal cortex, show a gaze-centered remapping of responses during eye movements in both monkeys and humans. However, when single neurons are recorded in these areas, the reach areas often show some saccade-related responses and the saccade areas often show some reach related responses. This may aid in eye–hand coordination or hint at the ability of cells to wire together as they’re used more frequently.

Clinical syndromes

Numerous disorders, diseases, and impairments have been found to result in disruption to the coordination of the eyes and hands, owing to damage to the brain itself, degeneration of the brain due to disease or aging, or an apparent inability to coordinate senses completely.

Aging

Impairments to eye-hand coordination have been shown to occur in older adults, especially during high velocity and precise movements. This has been attributed to the general degeneration of the brain’s cortex, resulting in a loss of the ability to compute visual inputs and relate them to hand movements. However, while older adults tend to take more time for these sorts of tasks, they are still able to remain just as accurate as younger adults, but only if the additional time is taken.

Balint’s Syndrome

Balint's Syndrome is characterized by a complete lack of eye-hand coordination and has been demonstrated to occur in isolation to Optic Ataxia. Generally characterized as a paralysis of eye saccades and inability to scan, this condition is most commonly caused by strokes, tumors, or Alzheimer’s Disease.

Optic Apraxia

Optic Apraxia is a condition that results from a total inability of a person to coordinate eye and hand movements. Though similar to Optic Ataxia, its effects are more severe and may not necessarily come from damage to the brain, but genetic defects or degeneration of tissue.

Optic Ataxia

Optic Ataxia is a clinical problem associated with damage to the occipital-parietal cortex in humans, resulting in a lack of coordination between the eyes and hand. Optic Ataxia has been often considered to be a high level impairment of eye hand coordination resulting from a cascade of failures in the sensory to motor transformations in the posterior parietal cortex. Optic ataxia has been often confused with Balint’s Syndrome, but recent research has shown that optic ataxia can occur independently of Balint’s Syndrome. Optic ataxia patients usually have troubles reaching toward visual objects on the side of the world opposite to the side of brain damage. Often these problems are relative to current gaze direction, and appear to be remapped along with changes in gaze direction. Some patients with damage to the parietal cortex show 'magnetic reaching': a problem in which reaches seem drawn toward the direction of gaze, even when it is deviated from the desired object of grasp.

Parkinson’s Disease

Adults with Parkinson’s disease have been observed to show the same impairments as normal aging, only to a more extreme degree, in addition to a loss of control of motor functions as per normal symptoms of the disease . This results from the general degeneration of neural tissue due to aging, compounded by the disruption of neurotransmitters caused by Parkinson’s resulting in slower movements than normal adults of a similar age. The ability to plan and learn from experience has been shown to allow adults with Parkinson’s to improvement times, but only under conditions where they are using medications to combat the effects of Parkinson’s.

See also

References

  1. Vidoni ED., McCarley JS., Edwards JD., Boyd LA. (2009). Manual and oculomotor performance develop contemporaneously but independently during continuous tracking Experimental Brain Research, 195, 611-620
  2. Johansson RS, Westling G, Bäckström A, Flanagan JR (2001) Eye–hand co-ordination in object manipulation, Journal of Neuroscience, 21(17), 6917–32.
  3. Liesker H., Brenner E., Smeets J. (2009). Combining eye and hand in search is suboptimal. Experimental Brain Research, 197, 395-401.
  4. Bowman M.C., Johannson R.S., Flanagan J.R. (2009). Eye-hand coordination in a sequential target contact task. Experimental Brain Research 195, 273-283
  5. Coen-Cagil R., Coraggio P., Napoletano P., Schwartz O., Ferraro M., Boccignone G. (2009). Visusomotor characterization of eye movements in a drawing task. Vision Research 49, 810-818
  6. Lazzari S., Mottet D., Vercher JL. (2009). Eye-Hand Coordination in Rhythmical Pointing, Journal of Motor Behavior, 41(4), 294-304.
  7. Ren, L., & Crawford, J. D. (2009). Coordinate transformations for hand-guided saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 195 (3), 455-465.
  8. Gomi H. (2008) Implicit online corrections in reaching movements, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 558-564.
  9. ^ Jackson, S. R., Newport, R., Husain, M., Fowlie, J. E., O'Donoghue, M., & Bajaj, N. (2009). There may be more to reaching than meets the eye: Re-thinking optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia, 47(6), 1397-1408.
  10. ^ Boisseau E., Scherzer P., Cohen H. (2002). Eye-Hand Coordination in Aging and in Parkinson’s Disease. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, Vol. 9, No. 4, 266-275.
  • Johansson RS, Westling G, Bäckström A, Flanagan JR (2001) Eye–hand co-ordination in object manipulation, Journal of Neuroscience, 21(17), 6917–32
  • Crawford JD, Medendorp WP, Marotta JJ (2004) Spatial transformations for eye–hand coordination, Journal of Neurophysiology,92(1),10-19 (Review)
Categories: