Revision as of 13:26, 30 June 2012 editArtifexMayhem (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users3,625 edits →Genetics: clearer← Previous edit | Revision as of 14:46, 30 June 2012 edit undoArtifexMayhem (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users3,625 edits →Environment: Replaced with text actually supported by the source.Next edit → | ||
Line 6: | Line 6: | ||
==Approaches== | ==Approaches== | ||
===Environment=== | ===Environment=== | ||
Environment has a significant effect on genetic expression. Disadvantaged environments enhance antisocial gene expression, suppress prosocial gene action and prevent the realization of genetic potential.<ref name=Asghate2011/> | |||
Biosocial criminology has sometimes been criticized for ignoring environmental influences{{who|date=June 2012}}. Biosocial researchers argues that this is incorrect but that on the other hand many sociologically influenced criminological approaches completely ignore the potential role of genetics which means that the results may be ]ed by genetic factors. For example, if a study finds an association between parental abusive and antisocial behavior when the children become adults this is often assumed to be evidence for a causation between parental abuse and antisocial behavior. However, another possibility is that shared genes may cause both behaviors. Furthermore, the environment is often influenced by genes which is an example of ]. One example being that genes may influence which environment a person prefers and selects to be in. Thus, a strong association between exposure to delinquent peers and a person's own delinquent behavior has been taken as evidence for causal process where a person learns to become a criminal. However, research on the role of genetic factors have found that such exposure is largely caused by genetic factors.<ref name=Asghate2011/> | |||
{{quotation|Genes and environments operating in tandem (interacting) were required to produce significant antisocial behavior, while neither was powerful enough to produce it independent of the other. That is, children genetically at risk for antisocial behavior reared in positive family environments did not display antisocial behavior, and children not at genetic risk did not become antisocial in adverse family environments.<ref name=Asghate2011/>}} | |||
Sociological approaches to criminality, while recognizing the importance of environmental factors through an individual's life, have mostly studied factors influential during adolescence or adulthood. Biological approaches have in addition emphasized the importance of early development when the brain is rapidly growing and developing. ]s can cause brain damage and are associated with later criminality. Also the environment during first few years after birth are critical for the growing brain. This early environment has been linked with child temperaments that are associated with later problematic behaviors.<ref name=Asghate2011/> | |||
===Genetics=== | ===Genetics=== |
Revision as of 14:46, 30 June 2012
Criminology and penology | |||
---|---|---|---|
Theory
|
|||
Major theorists | |||
Types of crime | |||
Methods | |||
Penology
|
|||
Schools | |||
Subfields | |||
Browse | |||
The neutrality of this article is disputed. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please do not remove this message until conditions to do so are met. (June 2012) (Learn how and when to remove this message) |
This article contains weasel words: vague phrasing that often accompanies biased or unverifiable information. Such statements should be clarified or removed. (June 2012) |
Biosocial criminology is an interdisciplinary field that aims to explain crime and antisocial behavior by exploring both biological factors and environmental factors. While contemporary criminology has been dominated by sociological theories, biosocial criminology also recognizes the potential contributions of fields such as genetics, neuropsychology, and evolutionary psychology.
Approaches
Environment
Environment has a significant effect on genetic expression. Disadvantaged environments enhance antisocial gene expression, suppress prosocial gene action and prevent the realization of genetic potential.
Genes and environments operating in tandem (interacting) were required to produce significant antisocial behavior, while neither was powerful enough to produce it independent of the other. That is, children genetically at risk for antisocial behavior reared in positive family environments did not display antisocial behavior, and children not at genetic risk did not become antisocial in adverse family environments.
Genetics
One approach to studying the role of genetics for crime is to calculate the heritability coefficient. It describes the proportion of the variance that is due to actualized genetic effects for some trait in a given population in a specific environment at a specific time. The heritability coefficient for antisocial behavior is estimated to be between 0.40 and 0.58.
Traditional sociologically oriented theories explain relatively little of the variance. Even when multiple factors from one or several such theories are combined and examined together they typically explain far less than 30% of the variance.
A more recent approach with a large potential is to directly study the DNA and examine if specific genetic markers are associated with criminal behaviors. A number of such associations have already been found, especially between neurotransmitter genes and criminal behaviors. Each individual association when studied in isolation has a relatively small effect. However, single genes can have a much larger effect in specific environments which is an example of gene–environment interaction.
Neurophysiology
Another approach is to examine the relationship between neurophysiology and criminality. One example is that measured levels of neurotransmitters such as serotonin and dopamin have been associated with criminal behavior. Another is that neuroimaging studies give strong evidence for that both brain structure and function are involved in criminal behaviors. The limbic system creates emotions such as anger and jealousy that ultimately may cause criminal behavior. The prefrontal cortex is involved in delaying gratification and impulse control and moderates the impulses from the limbic system. If this balance is shifted in favor of the limbic system this may contribute to criminal behavior. Terrie Moffitt's developmental theory of crime argues that "life-course-persistent offenders" make up only 6% of the population but commits more than 50% of all crimes and that this is due to a combination neurophysiological deficits and an adverse environment that creates a criminal path that is very difficult to break once started.
Evolutionary psychology
Men can potentially have many children with little effort; women only a few with great effort. This is argued to contribute to males having more variable reproductive success than females. One argued consequence of this is that males are more aggressive, and more violently aggressive, than females since they face higher reproductive competition from their own sex than females. In particular, low status males may easily become completely childless. Under such circumstances it may been have evolutionary useful to take very high risks and use violent aggression in order to try increase status and reproductive success rather than become genetically extinct. This may explain why males have higher crime rates than females and why low status and being unmarried is associated with criminality. It may also explain why the degree of income inequality of a society is a better predictor than the absolute income level of the society for male-male homocides. This because increased income inequality increases status differences in society while changing the average income level may not do so. Furthermore, competition over females are argued to evolutionary have been particularly intensive in late adolescence and young adulthood which may explain why crime rates are particularly high during this period.
The "evolutionary neuroandrogenic theory" focuses on the hormone testosterone as a factor influencing aggression and criminality and being evolutionary beneficial during certain forms of competition. In most species, males are more aggressive than females. Castration of males usually has a pacifying effect on aggressive behavior in males. In humans, males engage in crime and especially violent crime more than females. The involvement in crime usually rises in the early teens to mid teens which happen at the same time as testosterone levels rise. Research on the relationship between testosterone and aggression is difficult since the only reliable measurement of brain testosterone is by a lumbar puncture which is not done for research purposes. Studies therefore have often instead used more unreliable measurements from blood or saliva. Most studies support a link between adult criminality and testosterone although the relationship is modest if examined separately for each sex. Nearly all studies of juvenile delinquency and testosterone are not significant. Most studies have also found testosterone to be associated with behaviors or personality traits linked with criminality such as antisocial behavior and alcoholism. Many studies have also been done on the relationship between more general aggressive behavior/feelings and testosterone. About half the studies have found a relationship and about half no relationship.
Many conflicts causing homocides involve status conflicts, protecting reputation, and seemingly trivial insults. Steven Pinker in his book The Blank Slate argues that in non-state societies without a police it was very important to have a credible deterrence against aggression. Therefore it was important to have a reputation for retaliation, causing humans to develop instincts for revenge as well as for protecting reputation ("honor"). Pinker argues that the development of the state and the police have dramatically reduced the level of violence compared to the ancestral environment. Whenever the state breaks down, which can be very locally such as in poor areas of a city, humans again organize in groups for protection and aggression and concepts such as violent revenge and protecting honor again become extremely important.
Some cultures place greater emphasis on protecting honor than other cultures. One explanation is that protecting honor was in the ancestral past relatively more important for herders than for farmers. The livestock of herders were easily and quickly stolen. As a result it was important to constantly show toughness as a deterrence which may cause a higher level of violence. The predictions of the theory was confirmed in a cross-cultural examination of traditional farming and herding Spanish-American societies. However, the prediction that sedentary fishing societies would place a low emphasis on honor was not confirmed.
The degree of cultural collectivism is strongly associated with the burden of infectious disease. It has been argued that this is due to collectivism and associated characteristics such as outgroup avoidance limiting the spread of infectious diseases. Other characteristics such as strong in-group–out-group bias and willingness to defend the ingroup's honor may promote violence. A study found strong associations between several forms of violent criminal behavior and both infectious disease rates across U.S states and degree of cultural collectivism across U.S. states. The associations remained strong after controlling for income inequality.
Specific forms
Evolutionary psychology researchers have proposed several evolutionary explanations for psychopathy. One is that psychopathy represents a frequency-dependent, socially parasitic strategy. This may work as long as there are few other psychopaths in the community since more psychopaths means increasing the risk of encountering another psychopath as well as non-psychopaths likely adapting more countermeasures against cheaters.
Sociobiological theories of rape are theories that explore to what degree, if any, evolutionary adaptations influence the psychology of rapists. Such theories are highly controversial, as traditional theories typically do not consider rape to be a behavioral adaptation. Some object to such theories on ethical, religious, political, as well as scientific grounds. Others argue that a correct knowledge of the causes of rape is necessary in order to develop effective preventive measures.
The Cinderella effect is a term used to describe the observed, in many but not all studies, significantly higher rate of stepchildren being abused by stepparents as compared to genetic parents. The effect has been explained by application of evolutionary psychology theories. There have also been various criticisms of these theories.
Infanticide is one of the few forms of violence more often done by women than men. Cross-cultural research have found that this is more likely to occur when the child has deformities or illnesses as well as when there are lacking resources due to factors such as poverty, other children requiring resources, and no male support. Such a child may have a low chance of reproductive success in which case it would decrease the mother's inclusive fitness to spend resources on the child, in particular since women generally have a greater parental investment than men.
Criminal justice
Punishment of exploitative behaviors harmful to the group was likely a recurring problem in the ancestral environment. As such humans are argued to have developed a range of psychological mechanisms for handling this. Punishment can be a deterrent to undesired behaviors but excessive punishment can also be harmful to the group. Thus, human are argued to favor a proportional response based on how severe the offence is. Cross-cultural research have a found a high agreement regarding how relatively harmful different crimes are perceived to be. On the other hand, evolutionary novel factors that may be rational to consider from a deterrent perspective, such as how difficult it is for the modern police to detect the crime, do not seem to affect people's perceptions of appropriate punishments.
Once a crime's severity has been judged there is a choice regarding how to respond. In some cases in the ancestral environment there may have been benefits from future interactions with the offender which some forms of punishment may have prevented as compared to responses such as reparations or rehabilitation. Research suggests that individuals may modify what they think are appropriate forms of response to offenders based on factors that once in the past small-group environment may have indicated that they could personally benefit from continued interactions with the offender such as kinship, in-group or out-group membership, possession of resources, sexual attractiveness, expressed remorse, intentionality, and prior history of cooperation and exploitation.
See also
- Anthropological criminology
- Biocultural anthropology
- Biosocial theory
- Evolutionary psychology
- Sociobiology
References
- ^ Kevin M. Beaver and Anthony Walsh. 2011. Biosocial Criminology. Chapter 1 in The Ashgate Research Companion to Biosocial Theories of Crime. 2011. Ashgate.
- ^ Aurelio José Figueredo, Paul Robert Gladden, Zachary Hohman. The evolutionary psychology of criminal behaviour. In Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with
|doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001
instead. - Lee Ellis; Kevin M. Beaver; John Wright (1 April 2009). Handbook of Crime Correlates. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-373612-3.
- Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with
|doi=10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009
instead. - Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with
|doi=10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x
instead. - Daly & Wilson (2007) Is the "Cinderella Effect" controversial? In Crawford & Krebs (Eds) Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology, pp. 383-400. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1037/a0026610, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with
|doi=10.1037/a0026610
instead. - ^ Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with
|doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001
instead.
Further reading
- Anthony Walsh, Kevin M. Beaver, Biosocial criminology: new directions in theory and research, Taylor & Francis, 2008, ISBN 0-415-98944-2
- Anthony Walsh, Lee Ellis, Biosocial criminology: challenging environmentalism's supremacy, Nova Science Publishers, 2003, ISBN 1-59033-774-3
- Kevin Beaver. Biosocial Criminology: A Primer Ken Hunt Publishing Company. 2009.
- The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture
- Jerome H. Barkow (Editor), Leda Cosmides (Editor), John Tooby (Editor)
- Homicide (Foundations of Human Behavior)
- Margo Wilson (Author), Martin Daly (Author)
- How the Mind Works , Steven Pinker (Author)
- Demonic Males by Richard Wrangham and Dale Peterson
- Before the Dawn: Recovering the Lost History of Our Ancestors
- Human Morality and Sociality: Evolutionary and Comparative Perspectives by Henrik Hogh-Olesen, Christophe Boesch, Leda Cosmides and Azar Gat (Jan 19, 2010)
- Sex, Evolution and Behavior by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson
- Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (4th Edition) by David M. Buss (Feb 28, 2011)