This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Werdna (talk | contribs) at 18:54, 25 August 2022 (NPoV tag). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.
Revision as of 18:54, 25 August 2022 by Werdna (talk | contribs) (NPoV tag)(diff) ← Previous revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)The neutrality of this article is disputed. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please do not remove this message until conditions to do so are met. (August 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this message) |
The Wonder Weeks is the English translation of the Dutch book Oei, ik groei! (literal translation: Ai, I'm growing!) by physical anthropologist Hetty van de Rijt and ethologist and developmental psychologist Frans Plooij. Originally published in 1992, it has been republished several times, with an updated 6th edition published in 2019. Drawing on many years of observation and analysis of infant development, it gives parents practical guidance to help their baby's cognitive development through its predictable stages or 'leaps'. Its theoretical basis is Perceptual Control Theory, which predicts and explains the observed phenomena. Questioning of the research with mother-infant dyads has received some attention in the press and social media, though sometimes reducing it to a matter of sleep schedules. The research has been replicated several times, and Brazelton found it to be mutually confirmatory with his own work. The book continues to be popular, and the publisher has produced a mobile app based on the book.
Claims
According to the book, babies go through 10 predictable 'leaps' in their cognitive development during the first 20 months of life. These correspond to successive developments in the growing brain, as manifested in the child's ability to perceive and then to control a new kind of perceptions at a higher level of the perceptual hierarchy.
This book derives from almost five decades of research, beginning at first with ethological observation of free-living chimpanzee mothers and infants undertaken in 1971 in cooperation with Jane Goodall, and then extending to more elaborate research with human mothers and infants. The existence of regular 'regression periods' and their correlation with stages of cognitive development are well documented in the literature of ethology and child psychology, and the findings underlying the book have been replicated in different countries and cultures by several independent research groups. Further corroboration comes from independent lines of investigation into periodicity of early childhood illness, peaks in the ages of infant mortality attributed to SIDS, and disorders of the central nervous system in infants. Brazelton and Plooij found that their independent lines of research were mutually corroborating. The leaps are predicted to occur at approximately 5, 8, 12, 19, 26, 37, 46, 55, 64 and 75 weeks old. A great deal of the observed variability is because development starts with conception, not with birth, and gestation is of variable duration. (Weeks are counted from the due date if the child is premature.) Development of specific skills that become possible with each higher order of perception is also variable. Beyond the scope of the book, additional regression periods have been documented, even into the teen years.
Theoretical basis
These findings conform to and help to confirm the comprehensive theoretical and research framework of Perceptual Control Theory (PCT), which was the orienting framework for the investigation from the outset.
According to Perceptual Control Theory, the brain controls a perception by means of setting the preferences (termed reference values) for the lower-level perceptions that go into constructing it. As the baby's brain grows it develops the capacity to take previously familiar perceptions as inputs from which it constructs new perceptions of a higher order in the hierarchy. The brain must then develop preferences for these new perceptions and learn to control them by setting reference values for the lower-level input perceptions. During the time that the brain is reorganizing, the kinds of experiences which the infant had grown confident controlling at that lower level of perception are destabilized, but before long learning from experience gives rise to new cognitive and behavioral capabilities. Consequently, a 'leap' consist of two phases: A phase where the baby 'regresses' as though to a younger age, seeking secure closeness with the mother and being generally 'cranky', due to cognitive disruption of previously gained competencies, followed by a period where the baby is generally happy and outgoing, exploring new experiences with the newly gained cognitive skills.
Effects of stressful conditions
Other conditions may cause insecurity and frustration in a child's life, "circumstances such as chaos, travel, moving house" and the like. The research underlying this book carefully controlled for such extraneous disturbances. Healthy, stable families were selected because the aim was to "understand normal development before venturing into understanding pathology". When there are "special parental conditions, such as rigid schedule care or depression and phobias, ... ips instead of peaks in crying ... have been found at the regression periods. Peaks in crying could be found only in the first few months ... because the crying is practically nonexistent thereafter ... under these special circumstances ... smiling appears to take over from crying as a means to try and get mother's attention." Such difficulties and their effects on the developing child are serious matters for further research, but are not within the scope of this book.
Controversy
As a PhD student, Carolina de Weerth, proposed to replicate the ethological findings with four mother-infant dyads. She measured emotional instability through direct observation of four behavior categories: ‘crying’, ‘fretting/fussing’, ‘smile’, and ‘body contact’. She found correlations of behavior categories with the predicted regression periods for only one of the four infants. After presenting these results in her PhD dissertation, she published them jointly with her dissertation supervisor, Paul van Geert. They concluded that age-related regression periods do not exist and that infant development is chaotic, with wide individual variation. This conclusion, which is consistent with their commitment to the application of mathematical concepts of chaos and complex systems to psychology, was immediately disputed by Plooij. Subsequently they published her data on cortisol levels measured in saliva considered as a measure of stress in these same four infants.
De Weerth's methodology has been criticized as a failure to replicate the research. Plooij argued that care was not taken to select subjects free from external sources of stress. Two of the mothers imposed strict schedules of sleeping, contact, and feeding, with the infant placed for hours in a playpen. This failed to replicate the research protocol calling for a "child-following" mother, and obviated any naturalistic record of maternal contacts in response to the infant's expressions of wanting contact. One of these two infants was separated from her mother for ten days because the mother "was not up to it". This infant also received at least 8 months of physiotherapy. The mother of another of the four had been in therapy for depression prior to the birth, and experienced post-partum depression and diverse phobias. Depression has well-documented adverse effects on the interactions that are central to replicating this work, as well as on the healthy development of the child. De Weerth's observational data consequently showed the predicted regression periods only for the fourth mother-infant dyad, who were relatively free from extrinsic sources of stress.
The controversial study by de Weerth has not been replicated. The findings of Plooij and his colleagues have been replicated by three independent research groups. No rejoinder has been published to the several 2003 refutations. Woolmore and Richer went so far as to say that "studies that fail to find regression periods must be carefully scrutinized to ensure they have not simply failed to see through the 'noise' in their data, such that they have mistaken their own failure to find the phenomenon for the phenomenon's nonexistence."
References
- Rijt, Hetty van de; Plooij, Frans (2018). Oei, ik groei! De 10 sprongen in de mentale ontwikkeling van je baby (in Dutch). Amsterdam: Fontaine Uitgevers B.V. ISBN 9789059568488.
- ^ Plas-Plooij, Xaviera; Plooij, Frans X.; Rijt-Plooij, Hetty van de (September 2019). The wonder weeks: A stress-free guide to your baby's behavior (6 ed.). New York: The Countryman Press (Norton). ISBN 978-1-68268-427-6.
- Wapner, Jessica (2020-04-16). "Are Sleep Regressions Real?". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 2021-02-16.
- "The Wonder Weeks App". The Wonder Weeks. Retrieved 2017-08-27.
- Plooij, F. X. (1984). Lipsitt, Lewis P. (ed.). The behavioral development of free-living chimpanzee babies and infants. Illustrated by David Bygott. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation. ISBN 9780893911157.
- Plooij, F. X.; Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. van de; Fischer, M.; Pusey, A. (2014). "Longitudinal recordings of the vocalizations of immature Gombe chimpanzees for developmental studies". Scientific Data. 1 (140025): 140025. doi:10.1038/sdata.2014.25. PMC 4322583. PMID 25977782.
- Plooij, F. X.; Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. van de; Fischer, M.; Wilson, M. L.; Pusey, A. (2014). "An archive of longitudinal recordings of the vocalizations of adult Gombe chimpanzees". Scientific Data. 2 (150027): 140025. doi:10.1038/sdata.2014.25. PMC 4322583. PMID 25977782.
- Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. van de; Plooij, F. X. (1987). "Growing independence, conflict and learning in mother-infant relations in free-ranging chimpanzees". Behaviour. 101 (1): 1–86. doi:10.1163/156853987X00378.
- Horwich, R. H. (1974). "Regressive periods in primate behavioral development with reference to other mammals". Primates. 15 (2–3): 141–149. doi:10.1007/BF01742277. S2CID 6922407.
- Trevarthen, C.; Aitken, K. (2003). "Regulation of brain development and age-related changes in infants' motives: The developmental function of regressive periods". In Heimann, M. (ed.). Regression periods in human infancy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation. pp. 107–184. ISBN 9780893911157.
- Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. van de; Plooij, F. X. (1992), "Infantile regressions: Disorganization and the onset of transition periods", Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 10 (3): 129–149, doi:10.1080/02646839208403946
- Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. van de; Plooij, F. X. (1993), "Distinct periods of mother-infant conflict in normal development: Sources of progress and germs of pathology", Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34 (2): 229–245, doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb00981.x, PMID 8444994
- ^ Sadurní, M.; Rostan, C. (2002). "Regression periods in infancy: A case study from Catalonia". Spanish Journal of Psychology. 5 (1): 36–44. doi:10.1017/s1138741600005813. PMID 12025364.
- ^ Sadurní, M.; Rostan, C. (2003). "Reflections on regression periods in the development of Catalan infants". In Heimann, Mikael (ed.). Regression periods in human infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 7–22. ISBN 0-8058-4098-2.
- ^ Woolmore, A.; Richer, J. (2003), "Detecting infant regression periods: weak signals in a noisy environment", in Heimann, M. (ed.), Regression periods in human infancy, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 41–55, ISBN 978-0805840988
- ^ Lindahl, L.; Heimann, M.; Ullstadius, E. (2003), "Occurrence of regressive periods in the normal development of Swedish infants", in Heimann, M. (ed.), Regression periods in human infancy, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 41–55, ISBN 978-0805840988
- Plooij, F. X.; Stelt, J. M. van der; Helmers, R. (2003), "Illness peaks during infancy and regression periods", in Heimann, M. (ed.), Regression periods in human infancy, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 81–95, ISBN 0-8058-4098-2
- Plooij, F. X.; Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. van de; Helmers, R. (2003), "Multimodal distribution of SIDS and regression periods", in Heimann, M. (ed.), Regression periods in human infancy, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 97–106, ISBN 0-8058-4098-2
- Mende, W.; Wermke, K.; Schindler, S.; Wilzopolski, K.; Höck, S. (1990). "Variability of the cry melody and the melody spectrum as indicators for certain CNS disorders". Early Child Development and Care. 65: 95–107. doi:10.1080/0300443900650112. Retrieved 7 March 2021. For further discussion, see Soltis, Joseph (2004). "The signal functions of early infant crying" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 27: 443–490. Retrieved 7 March 2021. They "strongly recommend an inclusion of structural developmental data before further testing any hypothesis" (p. 475).
- Sparrow, J. (2013). "Newborn Behavior, Parent–Infant Interaction, and Developmental Change Processes: Research Roots of Developmental, Relational, and Systems-Theory-Based Practice". Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing. 26 (3): 180–185. doi:10.1111/jcap.12047. PMID 23909940.
- Plooij, F. X. (2010), "The 4 WHY's of age-linked regression periods in infancy", in Lester, B. M.; Sparrow, J. D. (eds.), Nurturing Children and Families: Building on the Legacy of T. Berry Brazelton, Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 107–119
- Plooij, F. X.; Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. van de (1990). "Developmental treansitions as successive reorganizations of a control hierarchy". American Behavioral Scientist. 34 (1): 67–80. doi:10.1177/0002764290034001007. S2CID 144183592.
- Plooij, F. X. (2013), "The trilogy of mind", in Heimann, Mikael (ed.), Regression periods in human infancy, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 185–205, ISBN 978-0-8058-4098-8
- Plooij, F. X. (2003), "The phylogeny, ontogeny, causation and function of regression periods explained by reorganizations of the hierarchy of perceptual control systems.", in Mansell, Warren (ed.), The interdisciplinary handbook of Perceptual Control Theory: Living control systems IV, London: Academic Press (published 2020), pp. 199–225, ISBN 978-0805840988
- ^ Plooij, F. X.; Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. van de (2003), "The effects of sources of "noise" on direct observation measures of regression periods: Case studies of four infants' adaptations to special parental conditions.", in Heimann, M. (ed.), Regression periods in human infancy, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 57–80, ISBN 978-0805840988
- Weerth, C. d. (1998). Emotion-related behaviors in infants: A longitudinal study of patterns and variability (PhD thesis). Groningen, NL: University of Groningen.
- Weerth, C. d.; Geert, P. v. (1998-03-01). "Emotional instability as an indicator of strictly timed infantile developmental transitions". British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 16 (1): 15–44. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00748.x. ISSN 2044-835X.
- Geert, P. v.; Weerth, C. d. (1998). "Empirische indicatoren voor regressies en sprongen bij baby's". Acta Neuropsychiatrica. 10 (3): 67–73. doi:10.1017/S0924270800036607. PMID 26971740.
- Geert, P. v. (2009). "Nonlinear complex dynamical systems in developmental psychology". In Guastello, S. J.; Koopmans, M.; Pincus, D. (eds.). Chaos and complexity in psychology: The theory of nonlinear dynamical systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 242–281. ISBN 9780521887267.
- ^ Plooij, F. X. (1998). "Repliek op 'Empirische indicatoren voor regressies en sprongen bij baby's'". Acta Neuropsychiatrica. 10 (3): 74–77. doi:10.1017/S0924270800036619. PMID 26971741.
- Geert, P. v.; Weerth, C. d. (2002). "A longitudinal study of basal cortisol in infants: Intra-individual variability, circadian rhythm and developmental trends". Infant Behavior and Development. 25 (4): 375–398. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00141-8.