Misplaced Pages

Behavior analysis of child development

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
(Redirected from Child development in behavior analytic theory)

The behavioral analysis of child development originates from John B. Watson's behaviorism.

History

In 1948, Sidney Bijou took a position as associate professor of psychology at the University of Washington and served as director of the university's Institute of Child Development. Under his leadership, the Institute added a child development clinic and nursery school classrooms where they conducted research that would later accumulate into the area that would be called "Behavior Analysis of Child Development". Skinner's behavioral approach and Kantor's interbehavioral approach were adopted in Bijou and Baer's model. They created a three-stage model of development (e.g., basic, foundational, and societal). Bijou and Baer looked at these socially determined stages, as opposed to organizing behavior into change points or cusps (behavioral cusp). In the behavioral model, development is considered a behavioral change. It is dependent on the kind of stimulus and the person's behavioral and learning function. Behavior analysis in child development takes a mechanistic, contextual, and pragmatic approach.

From its inception, the behavioral model has focused on prediction and control of the developmental process. The model focuses on the analysis of a behavior and then synthesizes the action to support the original behavior. The model was changed after Richard J. Herrnstein studied the matching law of choice behavior developed by studying of reinforcement in the natural environment. More recently, the model has focused more on behavior over time and the way that behavioral responses become repetitive. it has become concerned with how behavior is selected over time and forms into stable patterns of responding. A detailed history of this model was written by Pelaez. In 1995, Henry D. Schlinger, Jr. provided the first behavior analytic text since Bijou and Baer comprehensively showed how behavior analysis—a natural science approach to human behavior—could be used to understand existing research in child development. In addition, the quantitative behavioral developmental model by Commons and Miller is the first behavioral theory and research to address notion similar to stage.

Research methods

The methods used to analyze behavior in child development are based on several types of measurements. Single-subject research with a longitudinal study follow-up is a commonly-used approach. Current research is focused on integrating single-subject designs through meta-analysis to determine the effect sizes of behavioral factors in development. Lag sequential analysis has become popular for tracking the stream of behavior during observations. Group designs are increasingly being used. Model construction research involves latent growth modeling to determine developmental trajectories and structural equation modeling. Rasch analysis is now widely used to show sequentiality within a developmental trajectory.

A recent methodological change in the behavioral analytic theory is the use of observational methods combined with lag sequential analysis can determine reinforcement in the natural setting.

Quantitative behavioral development

The model of hierarchical complexity is a quantitative analytic theory of development. This model offers an explanation for why certain tasks are acquired earlier than others through developmental sequences and gives an explanation of the biological, cultural, organizational, and individual principles of performance. It quantifies the order of hierarchical complexity of a task based on explicit and mathematical measurements of behavior.

Research

Contingencies, uncertainty, and attachment

The behavioral model of attachment recognizes the role of uncertainty in an infant and the child's limited communication abilities. Contingent relationships are instrumental in the behavior analytic theory, because much emphasis is put on those actions that produce parents' responses.

The importance of contingency appears to be highlighted in other developmental theories, but the behavioral model recognizes that contingency must be determined by two factors: the efficiency of the action and that efficiency compared to other tasks that the infant might perform at that point. Both infants and adults function in their environments by understanding these contingent relationships. Research has shown that contingent relationships lead to emotionally satisfying relationships.

Since 1961, behavioral research has shown that there is relationship between the parents' responses to separation from the infant and outcomes of a "stranger situation.". In a study done in 2000, six infants participated in a classic reversal design (see single-subject research) study that assessed infant approach rate to a stranger. If attention was based on stranger avoidance, the infant avoided the stranger. If attention was placed on infant approach, the infant approached the stranger.

Recent meta-analytic studies of this model of attachment based on contingency found a moderate effect of contingency on attachment, which increased to a large effect size when the quality of reinforcement was considered. Other research on contingency highlights its effect on the development of both pro-social and anti-social behavior. These effects can also be furthered by training parents to become more sensitive to children's behaviors, Meta-analytic research supports the notion that attachment is operant-based learning.

An infant's sensitivity to contingencies can be affected by biological factors and environment changes. Studies show that being placed in erratic environments with few contingencies may cause a child to have conduct problems and may lead to depression. (see Behavioral Development and Depression below). Research continues to look at the effects of learning-based attachment on moral development. Some studies have shown that erratic use of contingencies by parents early in life can produce devastating long-term effects for the child.

Motor development

Since Watson developed the theory of behaviorism, behavior analysts have held that motor development represents a conditioning process. This holds that crawling, climbing, and walking displayed by infants represents conditioning of biologically innate reflexes. In this case, the reflex of stepping is the respondent behavior and these reflexes are environmentally conditioned through experience and practice. This position was criticized by maturation theorists. They believed that the stepping reflex for infants actually disappeared over time and was not "continuous". By working with a slightly different theoretical model, while still using operant conditioning, Esther Thelen was able to show that children's stepping reflex disappears as a function of increased physical weight. However, when infants were placed in water, that same stepping reflex returned. This offered a model for the continuity of the stepping reflex and the progressive stimulation model for behavior analysts.

Infants deprived of physical stimulation or the opportunity to respond were found to have delayed motor development. Under conditions of extra stimulation, the motor behavior of these children rapidly improved. Some research has shown that the use of a treadmill can be beneficial to children with motor delays including Down syndrome and cerebral palsy. Research on opportunity to respond and the building of motor development continues today.

The behavioral development model of motor activity has produced a number of techniques, including operant-based biofeedback to facilitate development with success. Some of the stimulation methods such as operant-based biofeedback have been applied as treatment to children with cerebral palsy and even spinal injury successfully. Brucker's group demonstrated that specific operant conditioning-based biofeedback procedures can be effective in establishing more efficient use of remaining and surviving central nervous system cells after injury or after birth complications (like cerebral palsy). While such methods are not a cure and gains tend to be in the moderate range, they do show ability to enhance functioning.

Imitation and verbal behavior

Behaviorists have studied verbal behavior since the 1920s. E.A. Esper (1920) studied associative models of language, which has evolved into the current language interventions of matrix training and recombinative generalization. Skinner (1957) created a comprehensive taxonomy of language for speakers. Baer, along with Zettle and Haynes (1989), provided a developmental analysis of rule-governed behavior for the listener. and for the listener Zettle and Hayes (1989) with Don Baer providing a developmental analysis of rule-governed behavior. According to Skinner, language learning depends on environmental variables, which can be mastered by a child through imitation, practice, and selective reinforcement including automatic reinforcement.

B.F. Skinner was one of the first psychologists to take the role of imitation in verbal behavior as a serious mechanism for acquisition. He identified echoic behavior as one of his basic verbal operants, postulating that verbal behavior was learned by an infant from a verbal community. Skinner's account takes verbal behavior beyond an intra-individual process to an inter-individual process. He defined verbal behavior as "behavior reinforced through the mediation of others". Noam Chomsky refuted Skinner's assumptions.

In the behavioral model, the child is prepared to contact the contingencies to "join" the listener and speaker. At the very core, verbal episodes involve the rotation of the roles as speaker and listener. These kinds of exchanges are called conversational units and have been the focus of research at Columbia's communication disorders department.

Conversational units is a measure of socialization because they consist of verbal interactions in which the exchange is reinforced by both the speaker and the listener. H.C. Chu (1998) demonstrated contextual conditions for inducing and expanding conversational units between children with autism and non-disabled siblings in two separate experiments. The acquisition of conversational units and the expansion of verbal behavior decrease incidences of physical "aggression" in the Chu study and several other reviews suggest similar effects. The joining of the listener and speaker progresses from listener speaker rotations with others as a likely precedent for the three major components of speaker-as-own listener—say so correspondence, self-talk conversational units, and naming.

Development of self

Robert Kohelenberg and Mavis Tsai (1991) created a behavior analytic model accounting for the development of one's "self". Their model proposes that verbal processes can be used to form a stable sense of who we are through behavioral processes such as stimulus control. Kohlenberg and Tsai developed functional analytic psychotherapy to treat psychopathological disorders arising from the frequent invalidations of a child's statements such that "I" does not emerge. Other behavior analytic models for personality disorders exist. They trace out the complex biological–environmental interaction for the development of avoidant and borderline personality disorders. They focus on Reinforcement sensitivity theory, which states that some individuals are more or less sensitive to reinforcement than others. Nelson-Grey views problematic response classes as being maintained by reinforcing consequences or through rule governance.

Socialization

Over the last few decades, studies have supported the idea that contingent use of reinforcement and punishment over extended periods of time lead to the development of both pro-social and anti-social behaviors. However research has shown that reinforcement is more effective than punishment when teaching behavior to a child. It has also been shown that modeling is more effective than "preaching" in developing pro-social behavior in children. Rewards have also been closely studied in relation to the development of social behaviors in children. The building of self-control, empathy, and cooperation has all implicated rewards as a successful tactic, while sharing has been strongly linked with reinforcement.

The development of social skills in children is largely affected in that classroom setting by both teachers and peers. Reinforcement and punishment play major roles here as well. Peers frequently reinforce each other's behavior. One of the major areas that teachers and peers influence is sex-typed behavior, while peers also largely influence modes of initiating interaction, and aggression. Peers are more likely to punish cross-gender play while at the same time reinforcing play specific to gender. . Some studies found that teachers were more likely to reinforce dependent behavior in females.

Behavioral principles have also been researched in emerging peer groups, focusing on status. Research shows that it takes different social skills to enter groups than it does to maintain or build one's status in groups. Research also suggests that neglected children are the least interactive and aversive, yet remain relatively unknown in groups. Children with social problems do see an improvement in social skills after behavior therapy and behavior modification (see applied behavior analysis). Modeling has been successfully used to increase participation by shy and withdrawn children. Shaping of socially desirable behavior through positive reinforcement seems to have some of the most positive effects in children experiencing social problems.

Anti-social behavior

In the development of anti-social behavior, etiological models for anti-social behavior show considerable correlation with negative reinforcement and response matching (see matching law). Escape conditioning, through the use of coercive behavior, has a powerful effect on the development and use of future anti-social tactics. The use of anti-social tactics during conflicts can be negatively reinforced and eventually seen as functional for the child in moment to moment interactions. Anti-social behaviors will also develop in children when imitation is reinforced by social approval. If approval is not given by teachers or parents, it can often be given by peers. An example of this is swearing. Imitating a parent, brother, peer, or a character on TV, a child may engage in the anti-social behavior of swearing. Upon saying it they may be reinforced by those around them which will lead to an increase in the anti-social behavior. The role of stimulus control has also been extensively explored in the development of anti-social behavior. Recent behavioral focus in the study of anti-social behavior has been a focus on rule-governed behavior. While correspondence for saying and doing has long been an interest for behavior analysts in normal development and typical socialization, recent conceptualizations have been built around families that actively train children in anti-social rules, as well as children who fail to develop rule control.

Developmental depression with origins in childhood

Behavioral theory of depression was outlined by Charles Ferster. A later revision was provided by Peter Lewisohn and Hyman Hops. Hops continued the work on the role of negative reinforcement in maintaining depression with Anthony Biglan. Additional factors such as the role of loss of contingent relations through extinction and punishment were taken from early work of Martin Seligman. The most recent summary and conceptual revisions of the behavioral model was provided by Johnathan Kanter. The standard model is that depression has multiple paths to develop. It can be generated by five basic processes, including: lack or loss of positive reinforcement, direct positive or negative reinforcement for depressive behavior, lack of rule-governed behavior or too much rule-governed behavior, and/or too much environmental punishment. For children, some of these variables could set the pattern for lifelong problems. For example, a child whose depressive behavior functions for negative reinforcement by stopping fighting between parents could develop a lifelong pattern of depressive behavior in the case of conflicts. Two paths that are particularly important are (1) lack or loss of reinforcement because of missing necessary skills at a developmental cusp point or (2) the failure to develop adequate rule-governed behavior. For the latter, the child could develop a pattern of always choosing the short-term small immediate reward (i.e., escaping studying for a test) at the expense of the long-term larger reward (passing courses in middle school). The treatment approach that emerged from this research is called behavioral activation.

In addition, use of positive reinforcement has been shown to improve symptoms of depression in children. Reinforcement has also been shown to improve the self-concept in children with depression comorbid with learning difficulties. Rawson and Tabb (1993) used reinforcement with 99 students (90 males and 9 females) aged from 8 to 12 with behavior disorders in a residential treatment program and showed significant reduction in depression symptoms compared to the control group.

Cognitive behavior

As children get older, direct control of contingencies is modified by the presence of rule-governed behavior. Rules serve as an establishing operation and set a motivational stage as well as a discriminative stage for behavior. While the size of the effects on intellectual development are less clear, it appears that stimulation does have a facilitative effect on intellectual ability. However, it is important to be sure not to confuse the enhancing effect with the initial causal effect. Some data exists to show that children with developmental delays take more learning trials to acquire in material.

Learned units and developmental retardation

Behavior analysts have spent considerable time measuring learning in both the classroom and at home. In these settings, the role of a lack of stimulation has often been evidenced in the development of mild and moderate intellectual disability. Recent work has focused on a model of "developmental retardation,". an area that emphasizes cumulative environmental effects and their role in developmental delays. To measure these developmental delays, subjects are given the opportunity to respond, defined as the instructional antecedent, and success is signified by the appropriate response and/or fluency in responses. Consequently, the learned unit is identified by the opportunity to respond in addition to given reinforcement.

One study employed this model by comparing students' time of instruction was in affluent schools to time of instruction in lower income schools. Results showed that lower income schools displayed approximately 15 minutes less instruction than more affluent schools due to disruptions in classroom management and behavior management. Altogether, these disruptions culminated into two years worth of lost instructional time by grade 10. The goal of behavior analytic research is to provide methods for reducing the overall number of children who fall into the retardation range of development by behavioral engineering.

Hart and Risely (1995, 1999) have completed extensive research on this topic as well. These researchers measured the rates of parent communication with children of the ages of 2–4 years and correlated this information with the IQ scores of the children at age 9. Their analyses revealed that higher parental communication with younger children was positively correlated with higher IQ in older children, even after controlling for race, class, and socio-economic status. Additionally, they concluded a significant change in IQ scores required intervention with at-risk children for approximately 40 hours per week.

Class formation

The formation of class-like behavior has also been a significant aspect in the behavioral analysis of development. . This research has provided multiple explanations to the development and formation of class-like behavior, including primary stimulus generalization, an analysis of abstraction, relational frame theory, stimulus class analysis (sometimes referred to as recombinative generalization), stimulus equivalence, and response class analysis. Multiple processes for class-like formation provide behavior analysts with relatively pragmatic explanations for common issues of novelty and generalization.

Responses are organized based upon the particular form needed to fit the current environmental challenges as well as the functional consequences. An example of large response classes lies in contingency adduction, which is an area that needs much further research, especially with a focus on how large classes of concepts shift. For example, as Piaget observed, individuals have a tendency at the pre-operational stage to have limits in their ability to preserve information(Piaget & Szeminska, 1952). While children's training in the development of conservation skills has been generally successful, complications have been noted. Behavior analysts argue that this is largely due to the number of tool skills that need to be developed and integrated. Contingency adduction offers a process by which such skills can be synthesized and which shows why it deserves further attention, particularly by early childhood interventionists.

Autism

Ferster (1961) was the first researcher to posit a behavior analytic theory for autism. Ferster's model saw autism as a by-product of social interactions between parent and child. Ferster presented an analysis of how a variety of contingencies of reinforcement between parent and child during early childhood might establish and strengthen a repertoire of behaviors typically seen in children diagnosed with autism. A similar model was proposed by Drash and Tutor (1993), who developed the contingency-shaped or behavioral incompatibility theory of autism. They identified at least six reinforcement paradigms that may contribute to significant deficiencies in verbal behavior typically characteristic of children diagnosed as autistic. They proposed that each of these paradigms may also create a repertoire of avoidance responses that could contribute to the establishment of a repertoire of behavior that would be incompatible with the acquisition of age-appropriate verbal behavior. More recent models attribute autism to neurological and sensory models that are overly worked and subsequently produce the autistic repertoire. Lovaas and Smith (1989) proposed that children with autism have a mismatch between their nervous systems and the environment, while Bijou and Ghezzi (1999) proposed a behavioral interference theory. However, both the environmental mismatch model and the inference model were recently reviewed, and new evidence shows support for the notion that the development of autistic behaviors are due to escape and avoidance of certain types of sensory stimuli. However, most behavioral models of autism remain largely speculative due to limited research efforts.

Role in education

One of the largest impacts of behavior analysis of child development is its role in the field of education. In 1968, Siegfried Englemann used operant conditioning techniques in a combination with rule learning to produce the direct instruction curriculum. In addition, Fred S. Keller used similar techniques to develop programmed instruction. B.F. Skinner developed a programmed instruction curriculum for teaching handwriting. One of Skinner's students, Ogden Lindsley, developed a standardized semilogarithmic chart, the "Standard Behavior Chart," now "Standard Celeration Chart," used to record frequencies of behavior, and to allow direct visual comparisons of both frequencies and changes in those frequencies (termed "celeration"). The use of this charting tool for analysis of instructional effects or other environmental variables through the direct measurement of learner performance has become known as precision teaching.

Behavior analysts with a focus on behavioral development form the basis of a movement called positive behavior support (PBS). PBS has focused on building safe schools.

In education, there are many different kinds of learning that are implemented to improve skills needed for interactions later in life. Examples of this differential learning include social and language skills. According to the NWREL (Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory), too much interaction with technology will hinder a child's social interactions with others due to its potential to become an addiction and subsequently lead to anti-social behavior. In terms of language development, children will start to learn and know about 5–20 different words by 18 months old.

Critiques of behavioral approach and new developments

Behavior analytic theories have been criticized for their focus on the explanation of the acquisition of relatively simple behavior (i.e., the behavior of nonhuman species, of infants, and of individuals who are intellectually disabled or autistic) rather than of complex behavior (see Commons & Miller). Michael Commons continued behavior analysis's rejection of mentalism and the substitution of a task analysis of the particular skills to be learned. In his new model, Commons has created a behavior analytic model of more complex behavior in line with more contemporary quantitative behavior analytic models called the model of hierarchical complexity. Commons constructed the model of hierarchical complexity of tasks and their corresponding stages of performance using just three main axioms.

In the study of development, recent work has been generated regarding the combination of behavior analytic views with dynamical systems theory. The added benefit of this approach is its portrayal of how small patterns of changes in behavior in terms of principles and mechanisms over time can produce substantial changes in development.

Current research in behavior analysis attempts to extend the patterns learned in childhood and to determine their impact on adult development.

Professional organizations

The Association for Behavior Analysis International has a special interest group for the behavior analysis of child development.

Doctoral level behavior analysts who are psychologists belong to American Psychological Association's division 25: behavior analysis.

The World Association for Behavior Analysis has a certification in behavior therapy. The exam draws questions on behavioral theories of child development as well as behavioral theories of child psychopathology.

Development Stages

The early years of a child's life are most critical to their development.

In understanding child behavior there are many different types ways to analyze it. There is a multiple stage behavior model that it can be broken down into. During the earliest years it's important for parents to keep track of each little millstone to ensure the babies health. Starting with each month there are milestones that should be achieved in social/emotional, language/ communication, cognitive, and movement/physical areas. Although all babies are different and might not hit each milestone at exactly the same time, if you child's development strongly differs from the expected milestones, see a doctor to make ensure the health of your child. A child's environment is crucial to their development, any trauma that a child experiences could potentially have long term affects on their adult life. Children learn and develop best in strong nurturing environments, in which they are cared for and safe. Development doesn't end after infant and toddler stages, as they start to enter school education places an important role in social and intellectual development. School allows students to build character and broadens the horizons of developing children. It's important for parents to be aware of each step of their child's development to ensure health and safety.

See also

References

  1. Watson, J.B. (1926). "What the nursery has to say about instincts. In C. Murchison (Eds.) Psychologies reinforcement and resistance to extinction in young children". Child Development. 28 (1): 47–54. doi:10.2307/1125999. JSTOR 1125999. PMID 13404656.
  2. Bijou, S.W. (1958). "Operant extinction after fixed-interval schedules with young children". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1 (1): 25–29. doi:10.1901/jeab.1958.1-25. PMC 1403884. PMID 16811199.
  3. Bijou, S.W.; Baer, D.M. (1961). Child Development: Vol. 1: a Systematic and Empirical Theory. Prentice-Hall. ISBN 978-0-13-130377-5.
  4. Bosch, S.; Hixson, M.D. (2004). "The Final Piece to a Complete Science of Behavior: Behavior Development and Behavioral Cusps". The Behavior Analyst Today. 5 (3): 244–253. doi:10.1037/h0100033. S2CID 54935610.
  5. Baer, D.M. (1982). "Behavior analysis and developmental psychology: Discussant comments". Human Development. 25: 357–361.
  6. Morris, E.K. (1988). "Contextualism: The worldview of behavior analysis". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 46 (3): 289–323. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.459.9320. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(88)90063-X.
  7. Schlinger, H.D. (2004). "The almost blank slate: Making a case for human nurture". Skeptic. 11: 34–43.
  8. Baer, D.M. (1973). The control of developmental process: Why wait? In J.R. Nesselrode & H.W. Reese (Eds.) Life Span Developmental Psychology: Methodological Issues. Oxford, England: Academic Press
  9. ^ Schlinger, H.D., Jr. (1995). A Behavior Analytic View of Child Development. NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers
  10. Catania, A.C. (1998). Learning (fourth edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
  11. Morris, E.K.; Hursch, D.E. (1982). "Behavior analysis and developmental psychology: Metatheoretical considerations". Human Development. 25 (5): 344–349. doi:10.1159/000272818. PMID 7173847.
  12. Reese, H.W. (2005). "A Conceptual Analysis of Selectionism: Parts I and II". Behavioral Development Bulletin. 12 (1): 8–16. doi:10.1037/h0100555.
  13. Vyse, S. (2004). "Stability over time: Is behavior analysis a trait psychology?". The Behavior Analyst. 27 (1): 43–54. doi:10.1007/BF03392091. PMC 2755368. PMID 22478416.
  14. Pelaez (1998). "Behavior analysis of development: History, theory, and research". Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis. 24: 85–95.
  15. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 13 (1)
  16. Snyder, J.J.; Patterson, G.R. (1986). "The effects of consequences on patterns of social interaction: A quasi-experimental approach to reinforcement in the natural environment". Child Development. 57 (5): 1257–1268. doi:10.2307/1130448. JSTOR 1130448. PMID 3769609.
  17. Commons, M.L.; et al. (1995). "Formal, systematic, and metasystematic operations with a balance-beam task series: A reply to Kallio's claim of no distinct systematic stage". Adult Development. 2 (3): 193–199. doi:10.1007/bf02265717. S2CID 143181596.
  18. ^ Cavell, T.A. & Strand, P.S. (2002). Parent-Based interventions for aggressive, antisocial children: Adapting to a bilateral lens. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.) Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relations. (pp. 395–419). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  19. Gewirtz, J.L. & Peláez-Nogueras, M. (1991). The attachment metaphor and the conditioning of infant separation protests. In J.L. Gewirtz & W.M. Kurtines (Eds.), Intersections with attachment. (pp. 123–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
  20. Patterson, G.R. (2002). The early development of coercive family processes. In J.B. Reid, G.R. Patterson & J. Snyder, J.J. (Eds.). Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A Developmental analysis and model for intervention. APA Press
  21. Watson, J.S. (1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In E.B. Thoman (Ed.), Origins of the infant's social responsiveness. In E.B. Thoman (Ed.) Origins of the infant's social responsiveness. (pp. 33–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
  22. Gewirtz, J.L. & Boyd, E.F. (1977). Experiments on mother-infant interaction underlying mutual attachment acquisition: The infant conditions the mother. In T. Alloway, P. Plinger & L. Krames (Eds.). Attachment behavior. (pp. 109–143). New York: Plenum Press
  23. Dunst, C.J.; Raab, M.; Trivette, C.M.; Parkey, C.; Gatens, M.; Wilson, L.L. French; Hamby, D.W. (2007). "Child and Adult Social-Emotional Benefits of Response-Contingent Child Learning Opportunities". JEIBI. 4 (2): 379–391.
  24. Gewirtz, J.L. (1961). A learning analysis of the effects of normal stimulation, privation, and deprivation on the acquisition of social motivation and attachment. In B.M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour. (pp. 213–229). New York: Wiley.
  25. Gewirtz, J. & Pelaez-Nogueras, M. (2000). Infant emotions under the positive-reinforcer control of caregiver attention and touch. In J.C. Leslie & D. Blackman (Eds.), Issues in Experimental and Applied Analysis of Human Behavior. (pp. 271–291). Reno, NV: Context Press
  26. Kassow, D.Z.; Dunst, C.J. (2004). "Relationship between parental contingent-responsiveness and attachment outcomes" (PDF). Bridges. 2 (4): 1–17. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-08-02. Retrieved 2008-03-09.
  27. Wahler, R.G.; Herring, M.; Edwards, M. (2001). "Coregulation of balance between children's prosocial approaches and acts of compliance: A pathway to mother–child cooperation?". Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 30 (4): 473–478. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3004_04. PMID 11708235. S2CID 46680594.
  28. Wahler, R.G.; Bellamy, A. (1997). "Generating reciprocity with conduct problem children and their mothers: The effectiveness of compliance teaching and responsive parenting". Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 14 (4): 549–564. doi:10.1177/0265407597144009. S2CID 145392568.
  29. Wahler, R.G.; Smith, G.G. (1999). "Effective parenting as the integration of lessons and dialogue". Journal of Child and Family Studies. 8 (2): 135–149. doi:10.1023/A:1022031716547. S2CID 141305505.
  30. Dunst, C.J.; Kassow, D.Z. (2004). "Characteristics of interventions promoting parental sensitivity to child behavior". Bridges. 2: 1–17.
  31. Dunst, C.J.; Kassow, D.Z. (2008). "Caregiver Sensitivity, Contingent Social Responsiveness, and Secure Infant Attachment". Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention. 5 (1): 40–56. doi:10.1037/h0100409.
  32. Cairns, R.B. (1979). The analysis of social interaction: Methods, issues, and illustrations. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
  33. Quay, H.C. (1987). Patterns of delinquent behavior. In H.C. Quay (Ed.). Handbook of Juvenile Delinquency. (pp. 118–138). New York: Wiley
  34. Commons, M.L. (1991). A comparison and synthesis of Kohlberg's cognitive developmental theories and Gewirtz's learning-developmental attachment theories. In J.L. Gewirtz & W.M. Kurtines (Eds.) Intersections with attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
  35. Commons, M.L.; Miller, P.M. (2007). "How Early Negative Caregiving: Experiences Relate to Stage of Attachment". Behavioral Development Bulletin. 13: 14–17. doi:10.1037/h0100495. S2CID 147024705.
  36. Savelsbergh, G.J.P. (2005). "Discovery Of Motor Development: A Tribute To Esther Thelen". The Behavior Analyst Today. 6 (4): 243–249. doi:10.1037/h0100074.
  37. Thelen, E.; Fisher, D.M. (1982). "Newborn stepping: An explanation for a 'disappearing reflex'". Developmental Psychology. 18 (5): 760–775. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.760.
  38. Dennis, W. (1960). "Causes of retardation among institutional children: Iran". Journal of Genetic Psychology. 96: 47–59. doi:10.1080/00221325.1960.10534274. PMID 13815945.
  39. Dennis, W.; Najarian, P. (1957). "Infant development under environmental handicap". Psychological Monographs: General and Applied. 71 (7): 1–13. doi:10.1037/h0093705.
  40. Sayegh, Y.; Dennis, W. (1965). "The effects of supplementary experience upon behavioral development of infants in institutions". Child Development. 36 (1): 81–90. doi:10.2307/1126782. JSTOR 1126782. PMID 14296799.
  41. Masiello, T.; Tucker (2006). "Influences of treadmill training on the motor development of infants with and without disabilities" (PDF). Bridges. 4 (5): 1–17. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-09-15. Retrieved 2008-03-09.
  42. Cautilli, J.D.; Dziewolska, H. (2005). "A Brief Report: Can Contingent Imitation Reinforce Truck Lifting In A Three-Month-Old Infant?". The Behavior Analyst Today. 6 (4): 229–234. doi:10.1037/h0100076.
  43. Dziewolska, H.; Cautilli, J.D. (2006). "The Effects of a Motor Training Package on Minimally Assisted Standing Behavior in a Three-Month-Old Infant". The Behavior Analyst Today. 7 (1): 111–124. doi:10.1037/h0100149. S2CID 143833745.
  44. Cautilli, J.D.; Dziewolska, H. (2006). "Brief Report: The Use of Opportunity to Respond and Practice to increase efficiency of the stepping reflex in a five-month-old infant". Behavior Analyst Today. 7 (4): 538–547. doi:10.1037/h0100088.
  45. Ince, L.; Brucker, B. & Alba A. (1977): Behavioral techniques applied to the care of patients with spinal cord injuries. In J. Kamiya, T.X. Barber, N.E. Miller, D. Shapiro & J. Stoyva (Eds.) Biofeedback and Self-control. Chicago, IL: Alpine, 515–523.
  46. Brucker B. (1983): Spinal cord injuries. In Burish and Bradley (Ed.) Coping with Chronic Disease: Research and Applications. New York: Academic Press, 285–311.
  47. Brucker B. (1984): Biofeedback in rehabilitation. In Golden (Ed.) Current Topics in Rehabilitation Psychology. San Diego, California: Grune and Stratton, 173–199
  48. Brucker, B.; & Ince, L. (1979): Biofeedback as an experimental treatment for postural hypotension in a patient with a spinal cord lesion. In J. Stoyva, J. Kamiya, T.X. Barber, N.E. Miller & D. Shapiro (Eds.). Biofeedback and Self Control. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine, 557–561.
  49. Brucker, B. (1980): Biofeedback and rehabilitation. In L.P. Ince (Ed.) Behavioral Psychology in Rehabilitation Medicine: Clinical Applications. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 188–217.
  50. Miller, N. & Brucker, B. (1981): A learned visceral response apparently independent of skeletal ones in patients paralyzed by spinal lesions. In D. Shapiro, J. Stoyva, J. Kamiya, T.X. Barber, N.E. Miller & G.E. Schwartz (Eds.). Biofeedback and behavioral medicine. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine, 355–372
  51. Brucker, B.S.; Bulaeva, N.V. (1996). "Biofeedback effect on electromyography responses in patients with spinal cord injury". Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 77 (2): 133–137. doi:10.1016/S0003-9993(96)90157-4. PMID 8607736.
  52. Watson, J.B. (1924). Talking and thinking. In J.B Watson's Behaviorism. Norton, New York.
  53. Kantor, J.R. (1929). "Can psychology contribute to the study of linguistics?". The Monist. 38 (4): 630–648. doi:10.5840/monist19283846.
  54. Weiss, A.P. (1929). The language responses. In A.P. Weiss's (Ed.) A theoretical basis of human behavior. Adams, Columbus, Oh
  55. Esper, E.A. (1925). A technique for the experimental investigation of associative inference in artificial linguistic systems. Language Monographs No. 1
  56. Mineo, D.A.; Goldstein, H. (1990). "Generalized learning of receptive and expressive action–object responses by language delayed preschoolers". Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 55 (4): 665–678. doi:10.1044/jshd.5504.665. PMID 2232747.
  57. Wetherby, B. (1978). Miniature language and the functional analysis of verbal behavior. In R. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Bases of language intervention. (pp. 397–448). Baltimore: University Park Press.
  58. ^ Skinner, B.F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. New York: Appleton
  59. Riegler, H.C. & Baer, D.M. (1989). A developmental analysis of rule-following. In H.W. Reese (Ed.) Advances in child development and behavior. (Vol. 21). San Diego: Academic Press.
  60. Skinner, B.F. (1957), page 4
  61. Chomsky, N.; Skinner, B.F. (1959). "Review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior". Language. 35 (1): 26–58. doi:10.2307/411334. JSTOR 411334.
  62. Greer, R.D.; Keohane, D. (2006). "The Evolution of Verbal Behavior in Children". The Journal of Speech and Language Pathology – Applied Behavior Analysis. 1 (2): 111–140. doi:10.1037/h0100194. S2CID 144268690.
  63. Reese, H.W. (2004). "Private Speech and Other Forms of Self-Communication". The Behavior Analyst Today. 5 (2): 182–189. doi:10.1037/h0100030.
  64. Skinner (1957). Verbal Behavior. Croft
  65. Ross, D.E.; Nuzzolo, R.; Stolfi, L.; Natarelli, S. (2006). "Effects of Speaker Immersion on the Spontaneous Speaker Behavior of Preschool Children with Communication Delays". JEIBI. 3 (1): 135–145.
  66. Greer, R.D.; Ross, D.E. (2004). "Verbal behavior analysis: A program of research in the induction and expansion of complex verbal behavior". Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention. 1 (2): 141–165. doi:10.1037/h0100286.
  67. ^ Chu, H.C. (1998). A comparison of verbal-behavior and social-skills approaches for development of social interaction skills and concurrent reduction of aberrant behaviors of children with developmental disabilities in the context of matching theory. (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, 1998). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations . Dissertations Abstracts Item: AAT 9838900.
  68. ^ Patterson, G.R. (2002). "Etiology and Treatment of Child and Adolescent Antisocial Behavior". The Behavior Analyst Today. 3 (2): 133–145. doi:10.1037/h0099971.
  69. Cautilli, J.; Tillman, T.C. (2004). "Evidence Based Practice in the Home and School to Help Educate the Socially Maladjusted Child". JEIBI. 1 (1): 28–35.
  70. Bevill-Davis, A.; Clees, T.J.; Gast, D.L. (2004). "Correspondence Training: A Review of the Literature". JEIBI. 1 (1): 14–28.
  71. Greer, R.D. & Speckman, J.M. (in press). The Integration of Speaker and Listener Responses: A Theory of the Development of Verbal Behavior. The Psychological Record.
  72. Kohlenberg, R. & Tsai, M. (1991)
  73. Nelson-Gray, R.O.; Mitchell, J.T.; Kimbrel, N.A.; Hurst, R.M. (2007). "The Development and Maintenance of Personality Disorders: A Behavioral Perspective". The Behavior Analyst Today. 8 (4): 443–445. doi:10.1037/h0100633.
  74. Eisenberg, N.; Wolchik, S.A.; Goldberg, L.; Engel, I. (1992). "Parental values, reinforcement, and young children's prosocial behavior: a longitudinal study". J Genet Psychol. 153 (1): 19–36. doi:10.1080/00221325.1992.10753699. PMID 1645156.
  75. ^ Snyder, J.J.; Patterson, G.R. (1995). "Individual differences in social aggression: a test of a reinforcement model of socialization in the natural environment". Behav Ther. 26 (2): 371–91. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80111-X.
  76. Patterson, G.R. (2002). "Etiology and treatment of child and adolescent antisocial behavior" (PDF). Behav Analyst Today (PDF). 3 (2): 133–45. doi:10.1037/h0099971.
  77. Bryan, J.H.; Walbek, N.H. (1970). "Impact of words and deeds concerning altruism upon children". Child Development. 41 (3): 747–759. doi:10.2307/1127221. JSTOR 1127221.
  78. Bryan, J.H.; Walbek, N. (1970). "Preaching and practicing generosity: Children's action and reaction". Child Development. 41 (2): 329–353. doi:10.2307/1127035. JSTOR 1127035.
  79. Barry, L.M.; Haraway, D.L. (2005). "Self-Management and ADHD: A Literature Review". The Behavior Analyst Today. 6 (1): 48–64. doi:10.1037/h0100051.
  80. Maccoby, E.M. (1968). The development of moral values and behavior in childhood. In J.A. Clausen's (Ed). Socialization and Society. Little Brown Books: Boston
  81. Aronfreed, J. (1968). Conduct and conscience: The socializing of internalized control of overt behavior. New York: Academic Press
  82. Aronfreed, J. (1970). The socialization of altruistic and sympathetic behavior: Some theoretical and experimental analysis. In J. Macauley & L. Berkowitz (Eds.) Altruism and helping behavior. New York: Academic Press.
  83. Charlesworth, R.; Hartup, W.W. (1967). "Positive social reinforcement in the nursery school per group". Child Development. 38 (4): 993–1002. doi:10.2307/1127097. JSTOR 1127097. PMID 5583075.
  84. Furman, W.; Masters, J.C. (1980). "Affective consequences of social reinforcement, punishment, and neutral behavior". Developmental Psychology. 16 (2): 100–104. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.16.2.100.
  85. ^ Lamb, M.E.; Easterbrooks, M.A.; Holden, G.W. (1980). "Reinforcement and punishment among preschoolers: Characteristics, effects, and correlates". Child Development. 51 (4): 1230–1236. doi:10.2307/1129565. JSTOR 1129565.
  86. ^ Lamb, M.E.; Roopnarine, J.L. (1979). "Peer influences on sex-role development in preschoolers". Child Development. 50 (4): 1219–1222. doi:10.2307/1129353. JSTOR 1129353.
  87. Leiter, M.P. (1977). "A study of reciprocity in preschool play groups". Child Development. 48 (4): 1288–1295. doi:10.2307/1128486. JSTOR 1128486.
  88. Patterson, G.R.; Littman, R.A. & Bricker, W. (1967). Assertive behavior in children: A step toward a theory of aggression. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 32, (5, Serial No. 113).
  89. Fagot, Beverly I.; Patterson, Gerald R. (1969). "An in vivo analysis of reinforcing contingencies for sex-role behaviors in the preschool child". Developmental Psychology. 1 (5): 563–568. doi:10.1037/h0027965.
  90. Serbin, L.O'Leary; Kent, R.; Tonick, I.; Tonick, I.J. (1973). "A comparison of teacher responses to pre-academic and problem behavior of boys and girls". Child Development. 44 (4): 796–804. doi:10.2307/1127726. JSTOR 1127726. PMID 4751301.
  91. Coie, J.D.; Kupersmidt, J.B. (1983). "A Behavioral Analysis of Emerging Social Status in Boys' Groups". Child Development. 54 (6): 1400–1416. doi:10.2307/1129803. JSTOR 1129803.
  92. Jakibuchuk, Z.; Smeriglio, V.L. (1976). "The influence of symbolic modeling on the social behavior of preschool children with low levels of social responsiveness". Child Development. 47 (3): 838–841. doi:10.2307/1128203. JSTOR 1128203.
  93. O'Connor, R.D. (1972). "Relative efficacy of modeling, shaping, and the combined procedure for the modification of social withdrawal". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 79 (3): 327–334. doi:10.1037/h0033226. PMID 5033375. S2CID 15753273.
  94. Snyder, J.; Stoolmiller, M.; Patterson, G.R.; Schrepferman, L.; Oeser, J.; Johnson, K.; Soetaert, D. (2003). "The Application of Response Allocation Matching to Understanding Risk Mechanisms in Development: The Case of Young Children's Deviant Talk and Play, and Risk for Early-Onset Antisocial Behavior". The Behavior Analyst Today. 4 (4): 435–453.
  95. Patterson, G.R.; Reid, J.B. & Eddy, J.M. (2002). A brief history of the Oregon model. In J.B. Reid, G.R. Patterson & J. Snyder (Eds.) Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for interventions. APA Press
  96. Osnes, P.G.; Adelinis, J. (2005). "Correspondence Training, Rule Governance, Generalization, and Stimulus Control: Connections or Disconnections?". Behavioral Development Bulletin. 12 (1): 48–55. doi:10.1037/h0100560.
  97. Bevill-Davis Alicia; Clees Tom J.; Gast David L. (2004). "Correspondence Training: A Review of the Literature". JEIBI. 1 (1): 14–27.
  98. Peleaz, M.; Moreno, R. (1998). "A taxonomy of rules and their correspondence in rule governed behavior". Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis. 24 (2): 197–214.
  99. Snyder, J.J.; McEachern, A.; Schrepferman, L.; Zettle, R.; Johnson, K.; Swink, N.; McAlpine, C. (2006). "Rule-Governance, Correspondence Training, and Discrimination Learning: A Developmental Analysis of Covert Conduct Problems". The Journal of Speech and Language Pathology – Applied Behavior Analysis. 1 (1): 43–65. doi:10.1037/h0100187.
  100. Barry, L.M.; Haraway, D.L. (2005). "Behavioral Self-Control Strategies for Young Children". JEIBI. 2 (2): 79–89.
  101. Ferster, C.B. (1973). "A functional analysis of depression". American Psychologist. 28 (10): 857–870. doi:10.1037/h0035605. PMID 4753644. S2CID 13717254.
  102. Hops, H. (1992). "Parental depression and child behaviour problems: Implications for behavioural family intervention". Behavior Change. 9 (3): 126–138. doi:10.1017/S0813483900006276. S2CID 147876161.
  103. Hops, H.; Biglan, A.; Sherman, L. Arhr; Freidman, L.; Osteen, V.; Osteen, V. (1987). "Home observation of family interactions of depressed woman". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 55 (3): 341–346. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.55.3.341. PMID 3597946.
  104. Biglan, A.; Hops, H. & Sherman, L. (1988). Coercive family processes and maternal depression. In R.D. Peters & R.J. McMahon (Eds.) Social learning and systems approaches to marriage and family. (pp. 72–103). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
  105. Biglan, A.; Rothlind, J.; Hops, H.; Sherman, L. (1989). "Impact of distressed and aggressive behavior". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 98 (3): 218–228. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.98.3.218. PMID 2768656.
  106. Kanter, J.W.; Cautilli, J.D.; Busch, A.M.; Baruch, D.E. (2005). "Toward a Comprehensive Functional Analysis of Depressive Behavior: Five Environmental Factors and a Possible Sixth and Seventh". The Behavior Analyst Today. 6 (1): 65–74. doi:10.1037/h0100055.
  107. In addition to loss of reinforcement, the loss of contingency between behavior and reinforcement can also lead to depression
  108. Rawson, H.E.; Tabb, L.C. (1993). "Effects of therapeutic intervention on childhood depression". Child and Adolesent Social Work Journal. 10: 39–53. doi:10.1007/BF00758022. S2CID 140295834.
  109. Rawson, H.E.; Cassady, J.C. (1995). "Effects of therapeutic intervention on self-concepts of children with learning disabilities". Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal. 12 (1): 19–31. doi:10.1007/BF01876137. S2CID 144557857.
  110. Kendler, H. & Kendler, T. (1975). From discrimination learning to cognitive development: A neobehavioristic odyessey. In W.K. Estes (Ed.) Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawerence Erlbaum Associates.
  111. Dunst, C.J.; Raab, M.; Wilson, L.L.; Parkey, C. (2007). "Relative Efficiency Of Stimulation". The Behavior Analyst Today. 8 (2): 226–236. doi:10.1037/h0100615.
  112. Rondal, J.A.; Docquier, L. (2006). "Maternal Speech To Children With Down Syndrome: An Update". The Journal of Speech and Language Pathology – Applied Behavior Analysis. 1 (3): 218–226. doi:10.1037/h0100197.
  113. Hutto, M. (2003). "Latency to learn in contingency studies of young children with disabilities or developmental delays" (PDF). Bridges. 1 (5): 1–16. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-09-15. Retrieved 2008-03-09.
  114. Bijou, S.W. (1966). A functional analysis of retarded development. In N.R. Ellis (Ed.) International Review of Research in Mental Retardation. NY: Academic Press.
  115. Greenwood, C.R.; Hart, B.; Walker, D. & Risley, T. (1994). The opportunity to respond and academic performance revisited: A behavioral theory of developmental retardation and its prevention. In R. Gardener, D.M. Sainato, J.O. Cooper, T.E. Heron, W.L. Heward, J. Eshleman & T.A. Grossi (Eds.) Behavior analysis in education: Focus on measurably superior instruction. (pp. 161–171) Pacific Grove, CA: Brookes-Cole
  116. Greenwood, Ward; Luze (2003). "The Early Communication Indicator (ECI) for Infants and Toddlers: What It Is, Where It's Been, and Where It Needs to Go". The Behavior Analyst Today. 3 (4): 383–392. doi:10.1037/h0099995.
  117. Greer, D.; Keohane, D.; Healy (2002). "Quality and Comprehensive Applications of Behavior Analysis to Schooling". The Behavior Analyst Today. 3 (2): 120–132. doi:10.1037/h0099977.
  118. Greenwood, C.R.; Carta, J.J.; Hart, B.; Kamps, D.; Terry, D.; Delquardi, J.C.; Walker, D.; Risley, T.R.; et al. (1992). "Out of the laboratory and into the community: Twenty-six years of applied behavior analysis at Juniper Gardens Children Center". American Psychologist. 47 (11): 1464–1474. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.47.11.1464. PMID 1482007.
  119. Bijou, S.W. (1983). The prevention of mild and moderate retarded development. In F.J. Menolascino, R. Neman & J.A. Stark (Eds.) Curative aspects of mental retardation: Biomedical and behavioral advances. (pp. 223–241). Baltimore: Brookes.
  120. Hart, B. & Risley, T.R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes
  121. Hart, B. & Risley, T.R. (1999). The social world of children learning to talk. Baltimore: Brookes
  122. Zentall, T.R.; Galizio, M.; Critchfield, T.S. (2002). "Categorization, concept learning, and behavior analysis: An introduction". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 78 (3): 237–248. doi:10.1901/jeab.2002.78-237. PMC 1284898. PMID 12507002.
  123. Goldiamond, I. (1966). Perception, language, and conceptualization rules. In B. Kleinmutz (Ed.) Carnegie Institute of Technology Annual Symposium on Cognition. John Wiley & Sons, New York.
  124. Becker, W.C. (1971). Teaching concepts and operations, or how to make kids smart. In W.C. Becker (Ed.) An empirical basis for changing education. Science research associates. Palo Alto, CA.
  125. Herrnstein, R. (1990). "Levels of stimulus control: A. functional approach". Cognition. 37 (1–2): 133–166. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90021-B. PMID 2269005. S2CID 17119527.
  126. Quine, W.V.O. (1953). From a logical point of view. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  127. Quine, W.V.O. (1960). Word and Object. Cambridge. MA: MIT Press.
  128. Hawkins, A.S. (1964). "Verbal identification of stimulus components in ambiguously named compounds". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1 (3): 227–240. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(64)90038-4.
  129. Steiner, T.E.; Sobel, R. (1968). "Intercomponent association formation during paired-associate training with compound stimuli". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 77 (2): 275–280. doi:10.1037/h0025778. PMID 5655120.
  130. Blackledge (2003). "An Introduction to Relational Frame Theory: Basics and Applications" (PDF). The Behavior Analyst Today. 3 (4): 421–430. doi:10.1037/h0099997. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-06-20. Retrieved 2012-04-23.
  131. Barnes-Holmes, Yvonne; McHugh, Louise; Barnes-Holmes, Dermot (2004). "Perspective-Taking and Theory of Mind: A Relational Frame Account" (PDF). The Behavior Analyst Today. 5 (1): 15–25. doi:10.1037/h0100133.
  132. Hayes, S.C.; Hayes, L.J. (1992). "Verbal relations and the evolution of behavior analysis". American Psychologist. 47 (11): 1383–1395. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.47.11.1383.
  133. Roche, B; Barnes-Holmes, Y; Barnes-Holmes, D; Stewart, I; O'Hora, D. (2002). "Relational Frame Theory: A new paradigm for the analysis of social behavior". The Behavior Analyst. 25 (1): 75–91. doi:10.1007/BF03392046. PMC 2731595. PMID 22478379.
  134. Esper, E.A. (1925). A technique for the experimental investigation of associative inference in artificial linguistic systems. Language Monographs No. 1.
  135. Wetherby, B. (1978). Miniature language and the functional analysis of verbal behavior. In R. Schiefelbusch (Ed.) Bases of language intervention. (pp. 397–448). Baltimore: University Park Press.
  136. Sidman, M. (2000). "Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 74 (1): 127–146. doi:10.1901/jeab.2000.74-127. PMC 1284788. PMID 10966100.
  137. Krasner, L. (1958). "Studies of conditioning of verbal behavior". Psychological Bulletin. 55 (3): 148–170. doi:10.1037/h0040492. PMID 13542700.
  138. Salzinger, K. (1959). "Experimental manipulation of verbal behavior: A review". Journal of General Psychology. 61 (1): 65–94. doi:10.1080/00221309.1959.9710241. PMID 14441175.
  139. Salzinger, K. (1967). The problem of response class in verbal behavior. In K. Salzinger & S. Salzinger (Eds.) Research in verbal behavior and some neurophysical implications. Academic Press, New York.
  140. Sailor, W. (1971). "Reinforcement and generalization of productive plural allomorphs in two retarded children". Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 4 (4): 305–310. doi:10.1901/jaba.1971.4-305. PMC 1310708. PMID 16795307.
  141. Baer, D.M. (1982). "The imposition of structure on behavior and the demolition of behavioral structures. In H.E. Howe (Ed.)". Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 29: 217–254.
  142. Baer, D.M.; Guess, D. (1973). "Teaching productive noun suffixes to severely retarded children". American Journal of Mental Deficiency. 77 (5): 498–505. PMID 4122226.
  143. Andronis, P.T.; Lang, T.V.J.; Goldiamond, I. (1997). "Contingency adduction of "symbolic aggression" by pigeon". The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. 16: 10–23.
  144. Bucher, B.; Schneider, R.E. (1973). "Acquisition and generalization of conservation by pre-schoolers, using operant training". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 16 (2): 187–204. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(73)90160-4.
  145. Waghorn, L.; Sullivan, E.V. (1970). "The exploration of transition rules in conservation of quantity (substance) using film mediated modeling". Acta Psychologica. 32 (1): 65–80. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(70)90091-0. PMID 5497127.
  146. Field, D. (1987). "A review of preschool conservation training: An analysis of analyses". Developmental Review. 7 (3): 210–251. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(87)90013-X.
  147. Ferster, C.B. (1961). "Positive reinforcement and the behavioral deficits of autistic children". Child Development. 32 (3): 437–456. doi:10.2307/1126210. JSTOR 1126210. PMID 13699189.
  148. Drash, P.W.; Tudor, R.M. (1993). "A functional analysis of verbal delay in preschool children: Implications for prevention and total recovery". The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. 11: 19–29. doi:10.1007/BF03392884. PMC 2748560. PMID 22477077.
  149. Lovaas, O.I.; Smith, T. (1989). "A comprehensive behavior theory of autistic children: Paradigm for research and treatment". Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 20 (1): 17–29. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(89)90004-9. PMID 2671048.
  150. Bijou, S.W. & Ghezzi, P.M. (1999). The behavior interference theory of autistic behavior in young children. In P.M. Ghezzi, W.L. Williams & J.E. Carr (Eds.), Autism: Behavior analytic perspectives. (pp. 33–43). Reno, NV: Context Press.
  151. Hixson, M.D.; Wilson, J.L.; Doty, S.J. & Vladescu, J.C. (2008). A Review of the Behavioral Theories of Autism and Evidence for an Environmental Etiology. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Applied Behavior Analysis. Special Compiled Issue 2.4 - 3.1, 46–59.
  152. Engelmann, S. & Carnine, D. (1991). Theory of instruction: Principles and applications. Eugene, OR: ADI Press
  153. Englemann, S.E. (1968). "Relating operant techniques to programming and teaching". Journal of School Psychology. 6 (2): 89–96. doi:10.1016/0022-4405(68)90002-2.
  154. Calkin, A.B. (2005). "Precision teaching: the standard celeration charts". Behav Analyst Today (PDF). 6 (4): 207–16. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.565.8320. doi:10.1037/h0100073.
  155. Scott, T.M. Gagnon; Nelson, C.M. (2008). "School-Wide Systems of Positive Behavior Support: A Framework for Reducing School Crime and Violence". Journal of Behavior Analysis of Offender and Victim: Treatment and Prevention. 1 (4): 69–80. doi:10.1037/h0100448.
  156. "Child Development" Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory. 2001. 5 February 2008. http://www.nwrel.org/request/june01/child.html
  157. Meltz, Barbara F. (October 22, 2007). "Zora and the explorer". The Boston Globe.
  158. "Language Development In Children". Child Development Institute.
  159. Commons, M.L.; Miller, P.M. (2001). "A Quantitative Behavioral Model of Developmental Stage Based upon the Model of Hierarchical Complexity". The Behavior Analyst Today. 2 (3): 222–252. doi:10.1037/h0099940. S2CID 55996550.
  160. Commons, M.L.; Miller, P.M. (1998). "A quantitative behavior-analytic theory of development". Mexican Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 24 (2): 153–180.
  161. Commons, M.L.; Trudeau, E.J.; Stein, S.A.; Richards, F.A.; Krause, S.R. (1998). "Hierarchical Complexity of Tasks Shows the Existence of Developmental Stages". Developmental Review. 8 (3): 237–278. doi:10.1006/drev.1998.0467.
  162. Novak, G. & Pelaez, M. (2004). Child and adolescent development: A behavioral systems approach. Sage
  163. Novak, G.; Pelaez, M. (2002). "A behavior-analytic developmental model is better". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 25 (4): 466–468. doi:10.1017/s0140525x02340081. S2CID 143785638.
  164. "World Center for Behavior Analysis". Archived from the original on 2011-01-10. Retrieved 2011-01-21.
  165. ^ CDC (2023-02-23). "Child Development Basics | CDC". Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Retrieved 2023-03-07.
  166. ^ CDC (2022-12-29). "What is a Developmental Milestone?". Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Retrieved 2023-03-07.
  167. ^ "8 Things to Remember about Child Development". Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University. Retrieved 2023-03-09.
  168. ^ "Pregnancy, Parenting, Lifestyle, Beauty: Tips & Advice | mom.com". mom.com. Retrieved 2023-03-09.
  169. Roisman, G.I. "Child Development". Child Development. 94 (1).

External links

Categories: